Abstract Introduc on and Purpose Methods Results ...

11 downloads 0 Views 8MB Size Report
Orff-‐Schulwerk is one of the most holisdc and creadve approaches in Music. Educadon. With a visionary pedagogical idea, Carl Orff connected language,.
Rhythms  of  the  body  -­‐  Rhythms  of  the  brain  (Language,  Music  and  Movement)   The  impact  of  the  Orff-­‐Schulwerk  approach  to  lived  emo1ons  on  the  ‘Musical  Brain’   João  C.  R.  Cunha  &  Sara  Carvalho        |        INET  -­‐  MD,  University  of  Aveiro  -­‐  Portugal  

Abstract  

Methods  

Conclusions  

Orff-­‐Schulwerk   is   one   of   the   most   holis1c   and   crea1ve   approaches   in   Music   Educa1on.   With   a   visionary   pedagogical   idea,   Carl   Orff   connected   language,   music,   movement   and   dance   in   what   he   nominated   “Elemental   Music”,   knowing   that   speech,   music   and   movement/dance   involve   the   systema1c   temporal  paGerns  of  rhythm.   In  this  poster,  we  propose  to  iden1fy  different  lived  emo1ons  boosted  by  Orff-­‐ Schulwerk  ac1vi1es  in  a  Music  Educa1on  context  and  to  connect  them  to  the   way   students   acquire   musical   knowledge   and,   consequently,   improve   their   ‘musical   brain’   development   (Koelsch,   2013;   Levi1n,   2008),   once   emo1ons   acquire  a  significant  relevance  in  cogni1ve  connec1ons  and  processes  (Clarke   et  al.,  2009;  Crick,  1995;  Damásio,  2002,  2010  and  references  therein;  Koelsch,   2013;   Lane   &   Nadel,   2002;   LeDoux,   1996;   Peretz   &   Zatorre,   2003,   Veloso   &   Carvalho,  2012).        

The   methodological   approach   taken   was   constructed   on   ques1onnaires   based   on   AFIMA   -­‐   Adapted   Flow   Indicators   in   Musical   Ac=vity   (Custodero,   2005   an   references   therin),  that  were  given  to  students  (n=50)  in  order  to  assess  their  musical  engagement   during  a  full  academic  year.  The  results  concern  the  “Affec1ve  Indicator”,  which  is  an   AFIMA   dimension   and   an   Op=mal   Experience/Flow   State   fundamental   element     according  Csikszentmihalyi’s    Flow  Theory.    

The   longitudinal   research   clearly   showed   that   the   musical   ac1vi1es   based   on   the   Orff-­‐Schulwerk  approach  provided  a  flow  sustaining  strategy  in  the  Music  Educa1on   class  (Cunha  &  Carvalho,  2012   and  references  therin).  The  data  of  this  par1cular   AFIMA  dimension  (“Affec1ve  Indicator”)  enabled  us  to  establish  that  through  Orff-­‐ Schulwerk’s   approach   children   lived   many   posi1ve   emo1ons   (in   higher   levels).   Therefore,  associated  with  brain  region  involved  in  reward,  mo1va1on  and  arousal   (Clarke   et   al.,   2009;   Koelsch,   2013;   Levi1n,   2008),   this   posi1ve   lived   emo1ons   demonstrated  to  be  significant  in  the  way  students  acquire  musical  knowledge  and,   subsequently,   develop   their   ‘musical   brain’.   In   fact,   posi1ve   emo1ons   can   guide,   influence,  or  constrain  cogni1on  (Crick,  1995;  Damásio,  2002,  2010  and  references   therein;   Lane   &   Nadel,   2002;   LeDoux,   1996;   Peretz   &   Zatorre,   2003,   Veloso   &   Carvalho,   2012),   once   they   has   important   effects   on   mental   func1ons   that   are   indisputably  cogni1ve,  such  as  memory,  aGen1on,  and  percep1on.  

 

IntroducKon  and  Purpose    

In   the   middle   of   20th   century,   Carl   Orff   and   Gunild   Keetman   conceived   the   now-­‐well-­‐known   Orff-­‐Shulwerk   approach,   combining   a   wide   range   of   rhythm   based   sources,   including   speech,   music,   movement   and   dance,   and     emphasizing  the  crea1ve  use  of  simple  percussion  instruments  (including  body   percussion   and   Orff   Instrumentarium),   while   building   upon   natural   (speaking   and  singing)  human  voice.     Recent   studies   in   Music/Music   Educa1on   (Chen   et   al.,   2009   and   references   therin;  Koelsch,  2013;  Levi1n,  2008;  Norton  et  al.,  2005;  Patel,  2010;  Steele  et   al.,  2013;  Thompson,  2009;  Zatorre,  2006;  Zatorre  et  al.,  2007)  suggested  that   the   connec1on   between   brain   and   music   occurs,   par1cularly,   at   cerebellum   level   and   is   connected   to   the   frontal   lobe,   the   centre   of   the   most   advanced   cogni1ons  in  humans  (Crick,  1995,  Levi1n,  2008).  This  connec1ons  run  in  both   direc1ons,   each   structure   influencing   the   other,   and   emo1ons   has   important   roles   in   those   cogni1ve   processes   (Clarke   et   al.,   2009;   Damásio,   2002,   2010   and  references  therin;  Lane  &  Nadel,  2002;  LeDoux,  1996).     We  consider  that  the  Orff-­‐Schulwerk  approach  works  by  being  a  guiding  force   for  building  the  referred  cogni1ve  connec1ons  between  brain  and  music  and   we   have   been   using   Flow   Theory   (Csikszentmihalyi,   1997   and   references   therin)  to  illustrate  this  development.      

 

 

Results  

  Regarding  the  lived  emo1ons  presented  in  the  figure  1,  which  occurred  during  the  Orff-­‐ Schulwerk   approach   ac1vi1es,   we   underline   that   the   majority   of   the   students   clearly   lived   "posi1ve   emo1ons"   in   all   classes.   In   addic1on,   higher   levels   (Quite   and   Very)   of   “Affec1ve   Indicators”   are   directly   correlated   with   posi1ve   lived   emo1ons,   e.g.   Happy,   Cheerful,   Involved   and   Sa=sfied   were   men1oned   with   maximum   level   (Very)   by   more   than   50%   of   the   students.   Moreover,   all   classes   which   included   movement   and   rhythmical  ac1vi1es  revealed  that  children’s  only  lived  “posi1ve  emo1ons”.                                                        

 

 

 

References  

  Chen,   J.   et   al.   (2009).   The   Role   of   Auditory   and   Premotor   Cortex   in   Sensorimotor   Transforma1on.   The   Neurosciences   and   Music   III   -­‐   Disorders  and  Plas=city:  Annual  New  York  Academy  of  Sciences  1169:  15-­‐34.   |  Clarke,  E.  et  al.  (2009).  Music  and  mind  in  everyday  life.   New   York:   Oxford   University   Press.   |   Crick,   F.   (1995).   The   Astonishing   Hypothesis:   The   Scien=fic   Search   for   the   Soul.   New   York:   Touchstone.   |   Csikszentmihalyi,  M.  (1997).  Finding  Flow:  The  Psychology  of  Engagement  with  Everyday  Life.  New  York:  Basic  Books.  |  Csikszentmihalyi,  M.   (2002).  Fluir:  A  Psicologia  da  Experiência  Óp=ma.  Santa  Maria  da  Feira:  Relógio  d’Água  Editores,  Lda.  (and  references  therin)  |Cunha,  J.  and   Carvalho,   S.   (2012).   Experienced   Emo1ons   through   the   Orff-­‐Schulwerk   Approach   in   Music   Educa1on   -­‐   A   Case   Study   Based   on   Flow   Theory-­‐.   In   Proceedings   of   12th   ICMPC   -­‐   8th   CESCSM,   (pp.   234-­‐241)   and   references   therein.   |   Custodero,   L.   (2005).   Observable   indicators   of   flow   experience:  A  developmental  perspec1ve  of  musical  engagement  in  young  children  from  infancy  to  school  age.  Music  Educa=on  Research,   7(2),   185-­‐209.   (and   references   therin)   |Damásio,   A.   (2002).   A   Second   Chance   for   Emo=on.   In   Richard   Lane   &   Lynn   Nadel,   (2002).   (eds).   Cogni=ve   Neuroscience   of   Emo=on.   (pp.   12-­‐23).   New   York:   Oxford   University   Press.   |   Damásio,   A.   (2010)   O   Livro   da   Consciência:   A   construção   do   cérebro   consciente.   Lisboa:   Publicações   Europa-­‐América.   (and   references   therin)   |   Koelsch,   S.   (2013).   Brain   and   Music.   Oxford:   Wiley-­‐Blackwell   Publishing.   |   Lane,   R.   &   Nadel,   L.   (2002).   (Eds).   Cogni=ve   Neuroscience   of   Emo=on.   New   York:   Oxford   University   Press.   |   LeDoux,   J.   (1996).   The   Emo=onal   Brain.   New   York:   Simon   /   Schuster.   |   LeviKn,   D.   (2008).   This   is   Your   Brain   on   Music.   London:   Atlan1c   Books.   |   Norton,   A.   et   al.   (2005).   Are   there   pre-­‐exis1ng   neural,   cogni1ve,   or   motoric   markers   for   musical   ability?   Brain   and   Cogni=on,   59,   124-­‐134.   |   Patel,   A.   (2010).   Music,   Language   and   the   Brain.   New   York:   Oxford   University   Press.   |   Peretz,   I.   &   Zatorre,   R.   (2003).  (Eds.).  The  Cogni=ve  Neuroscience  of  Music.  New  York:  Oxford  University  Press.  |  Steele,  C.  et  al.  (2013).  Early  musical  training  and   white-­‐maGer  plas1city  in  the  corpus  callosum:  evidence  for  a  sensi1ve  period.  The  Journal  of  Neuroscience.  33  (3),  1282–1290.  |  Thompson,   W.  (2009).  Music,  Thought,  and  Felling:  Understsnding  the  Psychology  of  Music.  New  York:  Oxford  University  Press.  |  Veloso,  A.  &  Carvalho,   S.  (2012)  “Music  Composi1on  as  a  Way  of  Learning:  Emo1ons  and  the  Situated  Self”,  chapter  V.  In  O.  Odena  (Ed.)  Musical  Crea1vity:  Insights   from   Music   Educa1on   Research.   Ashgate/Sempre   Studies   in   The   Psychology   of   Music   Series.   |   Zatorre,   R.   (2006).   Neural   correlates   of   rhythmic  expectancy.  Advances  in  Cogni=ve  Psychology,  2,  221-­‐231.  |  Zatorre,  R.  et  al.  (2007).  When  the  Brain  Plays  Music:  Auditory-­‐motor   Interac1ons  in  Music  Percep1on  and  Produc1on.  Nature  Reviews  |  Neuroscience,  8,  547-­‐558.      

Acknowledgments    

   

  Contact  details  

  João  C.  R.  Cunha    |  Sara  Carvalho