Accepted Manuscript Alexithymia and problematic alcohol use: A critical update
K.E. Cruise, R. Becerra PII: DOI: Reference:
S0306-4603(17)30370-2 doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2017.09.025 AB 5312
To appear in:
Addictive Behaviors
Received date: Revised date: Accepted date:
15 April 2017 27 September 2017 29 September 2017
Please cite this article as: K.E. Cruise, R. Becerra , Alexithymia and problematic alcohol use: A critical update. The address for the corresponding author was captured as affiliation for all authors. Please check if appropriate. Ab(2017), doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2017.09.025
This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 1
Alexithymia and Problematic Alcohol Use: A Critical Update Cruise, K.E. & Becerra, R.
Kate Cruise
School of Psychology and Social Sciences, Edith Cowan University; 270 Joondalup Drive, Joondalup, Western
School of Psychology and Social Sciences, Edith Cowan University; 270 Joondalup Drive, Joondalup, Western
RI
Rodrigo Becerra
PT
Australia, Australia 6027,
[email protected]
NU
SC
Australia, Australia 6027,
[email protected]
Corresponding Author:
MA
Kate Cruise, Edith Cowan University,
Email:
[email protected]
AC C
EP T
Phone: +61 8 6304 2786
ED
270 Joondalup Drive, Joondalup, Western Australia, 6027
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 2
Abstract There has been a substantial growth in empirical research aimed at examining the cooccurrence of alexithymia and problematic alcohol use and alcohol use disorder (AUD) since Thorberg, Young, Sullivan, & Lyvers (2009) original review article. The objective of the
PT
present paper is therefore to provide a critical update review of research on alexithymia and problematic alcohol use published since 2009. A systematic search was conducted through
RI
PsychINFO, Medline, and ProQuest databases to obtain relevant literature published between
SC
2009 and 2016. Studies that involved measures of alexithymia and problematic alcohol use among clinical and non-clinical samples were included. Prevalence rates of alexithymia
NU
among Alcohol Dependent (AD) samples were identified between 30 and 49% and therefore
MA
much lower than originally reported. The findings of this update review highlight an indirect relationship between alexithymia and alcohol problem severity that is mediated by a number of psychological drinking constructs (e.g., alcohol expectancy, drinking motives, craving and
ED
alcohol related intrusive thoughts) and psychological risk factors for the development of
EP T
alcohol related problems (e.g., mood and emotion dysregulation, attachment, trauma, and cognitive function). In addition, this review provides reasonable evidence to support
AC C
alexithymia as an independent risk factor for alcohol related problems among clinical samples only. In conclusion, alexithymia is a multifaceted construct that has a complex relationship with various risk factors and psychological drinking constructs. The growing body of research highlights the demand for understanding the interrelationships between alexithymia, psychosocial risk factors, and problematic alcohol use in order to tailor and target therapeutic interventions. Key words: Alexithymia; Alcohol Use Disorder; Alcohol Dependence; Alcohol Related Problems; Problematic Alcohol Use
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 3
1. Introduction Alexithymia is a multidimensional personality trait, defined by a set of four characteristics: 1) difficulty identifying feelings and distinguishing feelings from bodily sensations of emotional arousal, 2) difficulty describing and communicating feelings to
PT
others, 3) evidence of an impoverished fantasy life, and 4) an externally oriented style of thinking (Sifneos, 1973; Taylor, Bagby, & Luminet, 2000). Alexithymia has been associated
RI
with numerous psychiatric disorders, depressive disorders in particular (Leweke, Bausch,
SC
Leichsenring, Walter, & Stingl, 2009); and has featured strongly among research aimed at investigating problematic alcohol use and alcohol dependence (AD)*. Thorberg and
NU
colleagues (Thorberg, Young, Sullivan, & Lyvers, 2009) conducted a critical review of the
MA
literature on alexithymia and alcohol related disorders, reviewing 24 studies published between 1973 and 2008. A high prevalence of alexithymia was identified among alcohol dependent (AD) populations (45-67%; Thorberg et al., 2009), however the review only
ED
provided preliminary evidence for a relationship between alexithymia and alcohol
EP T
consumption, and limited evidence to support alexithymia as an independent risk factor for the development and maintenance of AD.
AC C
The empirical base of alexithymia and AD research has grown quite substantially since Thorberg et al.’s (2009) review and includes a much broader range of psychological drinking constructs (i.e., alcohol expectancy, drinking motives, craving and alcohol related intrusive thoughts). Research in this area has also examined alexithymia and AD in relation to a range of psychological risk factors reported to be associated with the development of alcohol related problems including trauma, attachment security, mood and emotion regulation, and cognitive function. Given the substantial empirical growth, this paper aims to provide a critical update of the literature on alexithymia and problematic alcohol use and alcohol use disorders (AUD).
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 4
*[Footnote ] Of those studies reviewed that involved a diagnostic measure, all utilised DSM-IV criteria for Alcohol Dependence. The term Alcohol Dependence (AD) as opposed to Alcohol Use Disorder (AUD) is therefore referred to throughout the manuscript.
2. Methods This systematic review followed the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) guidelines (Moher, Tetzlaff, Altman, & Group,
PT
2009). For identification of the relevant studies a combination of key words were used:
RI
‘alexithymia’, ‘alexithymic features’, ‘alexithymic’, ‘alcohol use disorder’, ‘alcohol
SC
dependence’, ‘alcohol abuse’, ‘alcohol use’, ‘alcoholism’, ‘binge drinking’, ‘alcohol intoxication’, ‘alcohol abstinence’, ‘alcohol problem’, ‘risky drinking’. A systematic search
NU
was conducted through PsychINFO, Medline, and ProQuest databases for relevant literature published in English, peer reviewed journals between 2009 and 2016 (7 November 2016).
MA
Only studies involving adult populations were selected. The reference lists of the articles included in the review were manually checked for relevant studies. Using these search
ED
strategies, 30 studies were found. Figure 1. summarises the identification, screening,
EP T
eligibility, and inclusion process. Table 1. provides a description of all the studies in terms of sample size, sample type and methodology, and identifies the significant relationships between alexithymia and other psychological drinking constructs and risk factors and
AC C
associated effect sizes.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
AC C
EP T
ED
MA
NU
SC
RI
PT
5
Figure. 1. Information flow on study selection
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 6
Table1 Descriptions and methodological aspects of studies reviewed Sample/Gen der comparison
Sample Type
Measu res
Methodol og y
Alcohol proble m measur e AUDIT
434, age (20.24±2.02). 236 males, 198 females. Combined
Athletes
TAS20 IPPA BFI
Cross sectional
Bruce et al. (2012)
862, age (26.1 ±9.12). 208 male, 654 female. Combined.
University
TAS20 DMQR TLFB
Cross sectional
Bujarski et al. (2010)
237, age (19.79±2.73). 73 males, 164 females. Combined.
University
Coriale et al. (2012)
110, age (44.3±9.7). 84 males, 26 females. Combined
Craparo et al.
117 AD, age (44.85±9.94).
Alexithymi a and psychologic al drinking constructs ES (d, f2 ) DIF associated with alcohol use (f2 = .14)
TLFB
ED
MA
NU
SC
RI
Andres et al. (2014)
Alexithymia and risk factors for alcohol related problems ES (d, f2 ) Maternal insecure attachment and low level of conscientiousne ss mediated the relationship between DIF and alcohol use1 . Not investigated
PT
Author
Cross sectional
AUDIT
AD inpatients
TAS20 COPENVI
Cross sectional
DSMIV LDH TLFB
AD inpatients
TAS20
Experimenta l
DSMIV-TR
AC C
EP T
TAS20 APRA ETISRSF
Relationship between alexithymia and trauma predicted alcohol risk perception at high trauma (f2 =.14) Relationship between alcohol abuse and avoidance coping mediated by alexithymia2
Alexithymia predicted
Drinking motives (social, enhancemen t, coping) mediated relationship between alexithymia and alcohol consumptio n1. Not investigated .
Greater alcohol consumptio n (d=.82), shorter period of abstinence (d= .45) for alexithymic than nonalexithymic. Alexithymic ≠ borderline alexithymic. Not investigated
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 7
(2014)
117 HC, age (43.98±9.57). 60 males, 57 females. Separate 156, age not reported. All male.
HC
DES-II TEC
dissociation in the AD group (f2 =.56)
AD inpatients
TAS20 TCI TEC
Cross sectional
AD inpatients
TAS20 MINI
Prospective
DSMIV
Greater trauma Not exposure investigated among alexithymic compared to nonalexithymic participants (.d=.33). Higher DIF (d=.47) and TAS-20 total (d=.42) among trauma group. Not investigated TAS-20 did not predict risk for relapse or alcohol related problems. No difference between high, moderate, low alexithymia for alcohol or psychiatry measures at baseline and 1 year follow-up. Three groups No identified based significant on attachment difference style and between alexithymia. three Depression (f2 = cluster .20), trait groups for anxiety (f2 =.25), severity of and personality alcohol use. (f2 =.69) differentiated between groups. Relationship Not between investigated alexithymia and alcohol problem severity
SC
RI
PT
Dalbudak et al. (2010)
60 males, 57 females.
100, age (low alexithymic 44.2±9.2; moderate alexithymic 44.7±8.3; high alexithymic 40.8±9.7). All male.
De Rick et al. (2009)
101, age (43.89±7.76). 71 males, 30 females. Combined.
AD inpatients
BVAQ AAQ STAI BDI ADPIV
Cross sectional
EuropA SI
Evren et al. (2009)
176, age (43.1±8.3). 176 males. All male.
AD inpatients
TAS20 TCI BDI BAI
Cross sectional
DSMIV MAST
EuropA SI
AC C
EP T
ED
MA
NU
de Haan et al. (2012)
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 8
156, age (44.21±9.10). 156 males. All male.
AD inpatients
TAS20 CAPS TCI
Cross sectional
DSMIV (SCIDI)
Evren et al. (2010b)
156, age (44.21±9.10). 156 males. All male
AD inpatients
TAS20 TCI SF-36
Cross sectional
DSMIV (SCIDI) MAST
Evren et al. (2012)
118 AD, age (42.50±9.88) 60 HC, age (35.27±11.84) . All male.
AD inpatients HC
TAS20 TCI DSQ
Experimenta l
Honkala mpi et al. (2010)
290, no information on age or sex.
Communit y
Luminet et al. (2016)
158, age (58.76±10.39) . 103 males, 55 females. Separate.
MA
ED
DSMIV (SCIDI)
TAS20 BDI
Prospective
DSMIV SCID-I
TAS20 OCDS PANA S BDI
Cross sectional
Not assessed
AC C
EP T
NU
SC
RI
Evren, et al. (2010a)
AD inpatient
predicted depression (f2 =.51), state anxiety (f2 =.54) and trait anxiety (f2 =1.56). Higher TAS-20 total (d= .55), DIF (d=.58) and DDF (d=.34) for PTSD than non-PTSD group.DIF predictor of PTSD group 1 . TAS-20 total, DIF, and DDF correlated negatively with physical (d=.80, .90, .53) and mental dimensions (d=.85, .95, .60) of QoL Immature defense style for alexithymic than nonalexithymic (d= .85). DIF correlated with immature defense (d=.63). Depression but not alexithymia predicted subsequent depressive disorder, personality disorder, or alcohol disorder. Depression predicted alexithymia (f2 =.56) Low but not high EOT predicted obsessive craving among men (f2 =.14). Interaction between depression and DDF predicted compulsive craving among
PT
STAI
Not investigated
Higher alcoholrelated problem severity for alexithymic than nonalexithymic group (d=.60). Not investigated
Higher alcohol dependency at 3 year follow-up for high compared to low alexithymia (d=.66).
Not investigated
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 9
262, age (26.83±8.34). 84 males, 178 females. Separate.
Communit y+ University
TAS20 SPSR Q DMQ
Cross sectional
Lyvers et al. (2012b)
314, age (27.6±8.4). 145 males, 169 females. Combined.
Communit y+ University
TAS20 FrSBe
Cross sectional
Lyvers et al. (2014a)
113, age (22.11±.35). 35 males, 78 females. Combined.
University
Lyvers et al. (2014b)
100, age (21.00±2.70). 28 males, 72 females. Combined.
AUDIT
SC
RI
PT
Lyvers, et al. (2012a)
women (f2 =1.94) Alexithymia group x gender interaction for males only (d=.77).
TAS20 SPSR Q FrSBe TRI BAI
Cross sectional
AUDIT
TAS20 DEQ DMQ
Cross sectional
AUDIT
Frontal lobe dysfunction mediated the relationship between alexithymia and risky alcohol use (d=.06). Children of alcoholic sig greater DIF than nonalcoholic child (d=.11) Frontal lobe dysfunction mediated relationship between alexithymia and CEP1 .
AC C
EP T
ED
MA
NU
AUDIT CAST
University
Not investigated
Alexithymi a was not related to risky drinking. DIF and Coping mediated by Sensitivity to Punishment . SP and risky drinking mediated by Coping 2 Alexithymi a predicted risky alcohol use (f2 =.02)
Alexithymi a predicted risky alcohol use (f2 =.04). Intrusive alcohol related thoughts mediated relationship between alexithymia and alcohol related problems (f2 = 05). Alexithymi cs higher on Coping motives (d=.62) and affective change expectancie s (d= .75)
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 10
AD inpatients
TAS20 MINI LAT
Cross sectional
DSMIV-TR SADQ
Family history of alcoholism associated with significantly higher alexithymia and DIF scores 1 .
PT
274, age (45.1±10.3). 220 males, 54 females. Combined.
Sig higher TAS-20 Total (d=.31) and subscale scores (DIF: d=.40, DDF d=.047, EOT: d=.64) for Type IV than Type III (TAS20 Total. Sig higher TAS-20 (d=.70) and subscale scores (DIF: d=.31, DDF: d= .42, EOT: d=.61) for Type IV than Type II. Type IV and Type I ns. Relationship between alexithymia and risky alcohol consumption mediated by depersonalisatio n. Not investigated
EP T
ED
MA
NU
SC
RI
Pombo et al. (2015)
TAS20 MBIHSS
Cross sectional
AUDIT
University
TAS20 UPPSP
Cross sectional
DDQ-M YAAC Q
1841, age not reported. 875 males, 966 females. Combined.
Shishido et al. (2013)
429, age (19.92±1.45). 160 males, 269 females. Combined.
Physicians
AC C
Pedersen et al. (2016)
than nonalexithymic s. No relationship between alexithymia and risky drinking. No sig correlation between alexithymia and onset of alcohol use, level of alcohol dependence , or current drinking status.
Direct effect of alexithymia on risky alcohol consumptio n1. Relationshi p between alexithymia and alcohol consumptio n mediated by positive urgency (f2 =.25). Alexithymi a and alcohol problems mediated by negative urgency
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 11
Cross sectional
DSMIV IDTS SADD DrInC TLFB
Alexithymia was predicted emotion dysregulation as measured by subscales of the DERS (f2 range: 31-.92). and ERQ suppression (f2 = .47)
PT
TAS20 MINI ERQ DERS NMR Q KIMS MAAS CRI
RI
AD outpatients
SC
77, age (45.5±11.07). 38 males, 39 females. Combined.
NU
Stasiewic z et al. (2012)
EP T
ED
MA
Alexithymia was a sig predictor of avoidance coping (f2 =.23) and mindfulness domains (f2 range=.51-1.56)
159, age (37.60±11.44) . 111 males, 48 females. Combined.
AD outpatients
TAS20 OAS
Prospective
DSMIV-TR AUDIT bMAST
Not investigated
Thorberg et al. (2010 b)
210, age (38.17±10.82) . 144 males, 66 females. Combined.
AD outpatients
TAS20
Cross sectional
DSMIV-TR AUDIT bMAST
Not investigated
Thorberg , et al.
210, age (38.17±10.82)
AD outpatients
TAS20
Cross sectional
DSMIV-TR
Adult attachment
AC C
Thorberg et al. (2010 a)
(f2 =.56). Positive urgency and alexithymia mediated alcohol problems at high levels of alexithymia (f2 =.12) Alexithymi a was a sig predictor of alcohol problem severity (f2 =.26) Alexithymi a was a sig predictor of drinking situations (unpleasant emotions, physical discomfort, conflict with others, social pressure, urges/tempt ations) (f 2 range= .19.30) DIF (f2 =.46) and OAS subscale UNS (f2 =.39) were significant predictors of total alcohol related problems. CFA identified 3 factor model (DIF, DDF, EOT) as best fit 2 Not investigated
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 12
. 144 males, 66 females. Separate.
RASS
AUDIT bMAST
security predicted alexithymia (f2 =.35), with insecure styles of close and anxiety accounting for sig additional variance. Both Alexithymic and borderline alexithymic sig higher attachment insecurity than non-alexithymic (Close: d=.84; Depend: d=.53, Anxiety: d=.96) Sig greater insecure attachment for alexithymic vs. non-alexithymic AD (d=.78)
AD outpatients
TAS20 OCDS RAAS Anxiet y
Cross sectional
NU
254, age (36.84±11.34) . 176 males, 78 females. Separate.
DSMIV-TR AUDIT
Thorberg , et al. (2011c)
AC C
EP T
ED
MA
Thorberg , et al (2011b)
SC
RI
PT
(2011a)
230, age (36.82±38.77) . 158 males, 72 females. Combined.
AD outpatients
Relationship between alexithymiaand craving partially mediated by insecure attachment 1 .
TAS20 DEQ
Cross sectional
DSMIV-TR AUDIT
Relationship between alexithymia and alcohol related problems partially mediated by drinking expectancies of assertion (f2 = .173) and affective change (f2 =.011)
Sig greater alcohol problem severity for alexithymic than nonalexithymic AD (d=.69). Sig difference between alexithymic and non alexithymic AD for total craving (d=.53), resistance/c ontrol (d=.48 ) and obsession (d=.47) Alexithymi a predicted alcohol problem severity (f2 =.053)
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 13
Thorberg et al. (2013)
AD outpatients
OAS
Cross sectional
Not reported
Not investigated
AD outpatients
TAS20 DEQ
Cross sectional
DSMIV AUDIT
Not investigated
92, age (38.96±11.46) . 66 males, 26 females. Separate.
AD outpatients
TAS20 DEQ
AC C
EP T
ED
Thorberg et al. (2016b)
MA
NU
SC
RI
PT
Thorberg et al. (2016a)
192, age (37.23±11.42) . 136 males, 56 females. Combined 255, age (38.67±10.95) . 244 males, 111 females. Separate.
Prospective
DSMIV AUDIT
Not investigated
CFA could not find any support for any tested model. Alexithymi a and borderline alexithymia sig higher affective change expectancy scores than nonalexithymic (d=.67) Alexithymi c sig higher assertion expectancy than borderline (d= .37) and nonalexithymic (d=.75) Sig decrease in total alexithymia (d=.64), DIF (d=.77) and DDF (d=.56) from baseline to 3 month follow up. Assertion expectancy partially mediated the relationship between total alexithymia (f2 =.62), DIF (f2 =.39) and DDF (f2 =.47) change T1 to T2.
Note: Adult Attachment Style Questionnaire (AAQ), Alcohol Perceived Risks Assessment (APRA), Alcohol Use Identification Test (AUDIT), Assessment of DSM-IV Personality Disorders (ADP-IV), Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI),Beck Depression Inventory (BDI), Bermond-Vorst Alexithymia Questionnaire
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 14
MA
NU
SC
RI
PT
(BVAQ), Big Five Inventory (BFI), Brief Michigan Alcohol Screening Test (bMAST), Children of Alcoholics Screening Test (CAST), Clinical-Administered PTSD Scale (CAPS),Cognitive Emotional Preoccupation (CEP) with alcohol, Coping Orientation to Problems Experienced (COPE-NVI), Coping Response Inventory (CRI),Daily Drinking Questionnaire Revised (DDQ-R), Difficulties in Emotion Regulation (DERS), Drinking Expectancies Questionnaire (DEQ), Drinking Motives Questionnaire (DMQ), Drinker Inventory of Consequences (DrInC), Drinking Motives Questionnaire-Revised (DMQ-R), Defense Style Questionnaire (DSQ), Early Trauma Inventory Short Form (ETISR-SF), Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ), European Addiction Severity Index (EuropASI), Frontal Systems Behavior Scale (FrSBe), Impulsive Behavior Scale (UPPS-P), Inventory of Drug Taking Situations (IDTS), Inventory of Parent and Peer Attachment (IPPA), Kentucky Inventory of Mindfulness Skills (KIMS)Lesch Alcoholism Typology (LAT), Lifetime Drinking History (LDH), Maskach Burnout Inventory Human-Services-Survey (MBIH), Michigan Alcohol Screening Test (MAST), Mindfulness Awareness Scale (MAAS), Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI), Modified Daily Drinking Questionnaire (DDQ-M), Multivariate Analysis of Covariance, Multivariate Analysis of Variance (MANOVA), (MANCOVA), Negative Mood Regulation Expectancies Questionnaire (NMRQ), Obsessive-Compulsive Drinking Scale (OCDS), Observer Alexithymia Scale (OAS),[ OSA subscales: Distant (DIS), Uninsightful (UNS), Somatising (SOM) Humourless (HUM), Rigid (RI)], Positive Affectivity Negativity Schedule (PANAS), Revised Adult Attachment Scale (RAAS)-Anxiety, Short Alcohol Dependence Data Questionnaire (SADD), Sensitivity to Punishment and Sensitivity to Reward Questionnaire (SPSRQ), Severity Alcohol Dependence Questionnaire (SADQ), State Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI), Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID-I), Structural Equation Modelling (SEM), Temptation and Restraint Inventory (TRI), Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI), The Short Form 36 (SF-36), Time Line Follow Back (TLFB), Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS-20) [TAS-20 Subscales Difficulty Identifying Feelings (DIF), Difficulty Describing Feelings (DDF), Externally Oriented Thinking (EOT)], Traumatic Experiences Checklist (TEC), Young Adult Alcohol Consequences Questionnaire (YAACQ) Insufficient information available to calculate effect size for significant results. 2 χ2 > 1df, effect size could not be calculated.
ED
1
EP T
3. Results
The following section integrates the results of alexithymia and alcohol use/disorder research
AC C
published since Thorberg et al.’s (2009) review. This section is divided into subcategories to provide a research update on the prevalence estimates of alexithymia in AD, relationship between alexithymia and alcohol problem severity, and the relationship between alexithymia and both psychological drinking constructs and more well established risk factors for AD. The following section also reviews current evidence pertaining to the psychometric measurement of alexithymia in AD and the absolute and relative stability of alexithymia in AD populations. 3.1 Prevalence of alexithymia in alcohol dependence
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 15
The prevalence rate of alexithymia among AD populations has been estimated between 45-67% (Thorberg et al., 2009). Review of relevant studies published since 2009 suggest that the prevalence rate of alexithymia among AD populations is much lower than originally reported, ranging between 30-49% (Coriale, Bilotta, Leone, Cosimi, Porrari, De Rosa, et al., 2012; Craparo, Vittoria, Alessio, & Vincenzo, 2014; Evren, Dalbudak, Cetin,
PT
Durkaya, & Evren, 2010a; Evren, Dalbudak, Durkaya, Cetin, & Evren, 2010b; Evren, Evren,
RI
& Dalbudak, 2009; Pombo, da Costa, Ismail, Cardoso, & Figueira, 2015; Thorberg, Young,
SC
Sullivan, Lyvers, Connor, & Feeney, 2011b). One exception was a substantially lower prevalence rate (21%) reported by Thorberg, Young, Sullivan, Lyvers, Hurst, Connor, et al.
NU
(2016b) who attributed this finding to sample characteristics, arguing that the self-referring sample may have exhibited greater emotional awareness. The prevalence rates outlined in
MA
Thorberg et al.’s (2009) review are argued to be inflated based on the small sample sizes among the studies reviewed (N