Journal of Neurochemistry, 2005, 92, 790–797
doi:10.1111/j.1471-4159.2004.02906.x
Attenuated microglial activation mediates tolerance to the neurotoxic effects of methamphetamine David M. Thomas*,à and Donald M. Kuhn*, ,à *Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Neurosciences and Center for Molecular Medicine and Genetics, Wayne State University School of Medicine, Detroit, Michigan, USA àJohn D. Dingell VA Medical Center, Detroit, Michigan, USA
Abstract Methamphetamine causes persistent damage to dopamine nerve endings of the striatum. Repeated, intermittent treatment of mice with low doses of methamphetamine leads to the development of tolerance to its neurotoxic effects. The mechanisms underlying tolerance are not understood but clearly involve more than alterations in drug bioavailability or reductions in the hyperthermia caused by methamphetamine. Microglia have been implicated recently as mediators of methamphetamine-induced neurotoxicity. The purpose of the present studies was to determine if a tolerance regimen of methamphetamine would attenuate the microglial response to a neurotoxic challenge. Mice treated with a low-dose methamphetamine tolerance regimen showed minor reductions in striatal dopamine content and low levels of microglial
activation. When the tolerance regimen preceded a neurotoxic challenge of methamphetamine, the depletion of dopamine normally seen was significantly attenuated. The microglial activation that occurs after a toxic methamphetamine challenge was blunted likewise. Despite the induction of tolerance against drug-induced toxicity and microglial activation, a neurotoxic challenge with methamphetamine still caused hyperthermia. These results suggest that tolerance to methamphetamine neurotoxicity is associated with attenuated microglial activation and they further dissociate its neurotoxicity from drug-induced hyperthermia. Keywords: dopamine, hyperthermia, methamphetamine, microglia, neurotoxicity, tolerance. J. Neurochem. (2005) 92, 790–797.
The amphetamines are powerful stimulant drugs of abuse. Illicit use of these drugs, including methamphetamine (METH) and 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA, Ecstasy), has increased to the point that they have eclipsed cocaine and heroin abuse on a global scale (UN Office on Drugs and Crime report http://www.unodc.org/unodc/en/ global_illicit_drug_trends.html). The medical, legal and societal problems associated with any rampant drug of abuse are compounded in the case of the amphetamines because many members of this pharmacological class cause persistent damage to the dopamine (DA) neuronal system. The shortterm consequences of METH abuse are manifested as depletions of DA, inhibition of tyrosine hydroxylase, inactivation of the DA transporter (DAT) and reduction in function of the vesicle monoamine transporter (VMAT) (Gibb et al. 1997; Brown et al. 2000; Fleckenstein et al. 2000; Kita et al. 2003a). Additional complications emerge with longer-term use of the amphetamines including degeneration of fine, unmyelinated axons (Molliver et al. 1990) and neuronal apoptosis (Davidson et al. 2001; Cadet et al. 2003).
Chronic METH use is associated with the development of tolerance (Segal et al. 2003) and behavioral sensitization (Itzhak and Ali 2002), two factors that are thought to sustain its addictive properties. Schmidt et al. (1985a,b) first showed that the METH-induced depression of monoamine synthesis was mitigated in rats previously treated with a ‘binge’ dosing regimen that simulates the pattern of intoxication characteristic of human amphetamine abusers. In addition to METH (Gygi et al. 1996; Stephans and Yamamoto 1996; Riddle
790
Received August 2, 2004; revised manuscript received October 1, 2004; accepted October 6, 2004. Address correspondence and reprint requests to Donald M. Kuhn, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Neurosciences, Wayne State University School of Medicine, 2125 Scott Hall, 540 E. Canfield, Detroit, MI 48201, USA. E-mail:
[email protected]. Abbreviations used: DA, dopamine; DAB, diaminobenzidine; DAT, dopamine transporter; HRP, horseradish peroxidase; ILB4, isolectin B4; i.p., intraperitoneal; METH, methamphetamine; MDMA, 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine; MPTP, 1-methyl-4-phenyl-1,2,3,6-tetrahydropyridine; PBS, phosphate-buffered saline; VMAT, vesicle monoamine transporter.
2005 International Society for Neurochemistry, J. Neurochem. (2005) 92, 790–797
Attenuated microglial activation in METH tolerance 791
et al. 2002), tolerance also develops to the neurotoxic effects of MDMA (Shankaran and Gudelsky 1999) and other amphetamines (Abekawa et al. 1997; Frederick et al. 1998), and cross-tolerance occurs between METH and MPTP neurotoxicity (Sziraki et al. 1994; Kita et al. 2003b). The mechanisms underlying the development of tolerance to METH neurotoxicity are not understood. Johnson-Davis et al. (2003, 2004) have established that tolerance to METH toxicity is mediated by adaptive changes in VMAT trafficking and function within the nerve terminal. These investigators ruled out roles for drug-induced hyperthermia and alterations in METH penetration into brain in the development of tolerance (Johnson-Davis et al. 2003, 2004). It has been proposed recently that microglia participate in the neurotoxicity associated with METH intoxication (Escubedo et al. 1998; Pubill et al. 2002; Guilarte et al. 2003; Pubill et al. 2003; LaVoie et al. 2004; Thomas et al. 2004a,b). Microglia are the primary antigen-presenting cells in the central nervous system. Once activated, these immune-like cells (Aloisi 2001; Streit 2002) can secrete a variety of factors such as reactive oxygen species, reactive nitrogen species, proinflammatory cytokines and prostaglandins, each of which can lead to neuronal damage (Kreutzberg 1996; Hanisch 2002). In the course of characterizing microglial involvement in METH neurotoxicity (Thomas et al. 2004b), we noted an attenuated microglial response in mice previously treated with drug, suggesting the possibility that tolerance to METH toxicity could be mediated at the level of microglia. The present studies confirm this possibility and show that tolerance does not develop to the hyperthermic effects of METH.
Materials and methods Materials (+) Methamphetamine hydrochloride, 1-methyl-4-phenyl-1,2,3, 6-tetrahydropyridine (MPTP), pentobarbital, horseradish peroxidase (HRP)-conjugated isolectin B4 (from Griffonia simplicifolia), 3,3¢-diaminobenzidine (DAB), paraformaldehyde, Triton X-100, dopamine, methanol, EDTA, all buffers and HPLC reagents were purchased from Sigma-Aldrich (St Louis, MO, USA). CitriSolv and Permount were products of Fisher Scientific (Pittsburgh, PA, USA). Animals Female C57BL/6 mice (Harlan, Indianapolis, IN, USA) weighing 20–25 g at the time of experimentation were housed five per cage in small shoe-box cages in a light- and temperature-controlled room. Mice had free access to food and water. The Institutional Care and Use Committee of Wayne State University approved the animal care and experimental procedures. All procedures were also in compliance with the NIH Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals. Pharmacological and physiological procedures Mice were exposed to two different pre-treatment conditions, followed in both cases with the identical neurotoxic challenge
regimen of METH. In the first case, mice were treated with a neurotoxic regimen of METH [four intraperitoneal (i.p.) injections of 5 mg/kg at 2-h intervals], which is manifested as a significant depletion of DA and robust microglial activation (Thomas et al. 2004b) and were subsequently challenged with the same METH neurotoxic regimen 7 days later, a time when the METH-induced microglial activation has returned to control levels (Thomas et al. 2004b). Controls for the drug pre-treatment and challenge conditions received i.p. injections of physiological saline on the same schedule used for METH. Injection volumes were 0.1 mL/10 g body weight. For ease of presentation, the pre-treatment/challenge conditions described above are referred to as follows: (i) control pre-treatment/control challenge (Sal/Sal); (ii) METH neurotoxic pretreatment/control challenge (METHNT/Sal); (iii) control pre-treatment/METH challenge (Sal/METHNT); (iv) METH neurotoxic pre-treatment/METH challenge (METHNT/METHNT). In the second design, mice were pre-treated with a tolerance regimen of METH (four i.p. injections of 2 mg/kg with a 2-h interval between each injection). This tolerance regimen was given three times (i.e. Monday, Wednesday and Friday) with a 48-h interval between each treatment. Mice receiving the tolerance regimen were subsequently challenged with the METH neurotoxic regimen 72 h later (i.e. on the following Monday). This lower dose of METH was selected for tolerance induction because it results in minimal microglial activation and has only minor effects on striatal DA content (Thomas et al. 2004b). These pre-treatment/challenge conditions are referred to as follows: (i) control pre-treatment/control challenge (Sal/Sal); (ii) METH tolerance/control challenge (METHTol/Sal); (iii) control pre-treatment/METH challenge (Sal/METHNT); (iv) METH tolerance/METH challenge (METHTol/METHNT). Finally, in some experiments, mice were challenged with 1-methyl-4-phenyl-1,2,3,6-tetrahydropyridine (MPTP; four i.p. injections of 20 mg/kg with a 2-h interval between injections) 7 days after pre-treatment with the neurotoxic regimen of METH. These groups are referred to as METHNT/MPTP and controls for this group are Sal/MPTP. Body temperature was monitored by telemetry using IPTT-200 implantable temperature transponders from Bio Medic Data Systems Inc. (Seaford, DE, USA). Core body temperatures were recorded every 20 min non-invasively using the DAS-5001 console system from Bio Medic. Lectin histochemical staining and stereology Microglial activation was assessed by staining fixed brain sections with HRP-conjugated isolectin B4 (ILB4) as developed by Streit (Streit 1990) and as previously described in our studies with METH (Thomas et al. 2004b). All mice were killed 48 h after the last injection of the METH or MPTP neurotoxic challenge for microglial analyses. Mice were deeply anesthetized with pentobarbital (120 mg/kg) and perfused transcardially with ice-cold 4% paraformaldehyde. Brains were removed and stored overnight in fixative at 4C. Sections of 50 lm thickness were cut through the striatum (+1.2 through )0.1 mm with respect to bregma). Sections were floated into phosphate-buffered saline (PBS) containing 0.3% H2O2 for 30 min, washed once in PBS + 0.1% Triton X-100, then incubated in fresh PBS + 0.1% Triton X-100 for an additional 30 min. Microglia were labeled with HRP-conjugated ILB4 (10 lg/mL in 0.1% Triton X-100) overnight at 4C. Excess ILB4 was removed by
2005 International Society for Neurochemistry, J. Neurochem. (2005) 92, 790–797
792 D. M. Thomas and D. M. Kuhn
three washes with PBS + 0.1% Triton X-100 (5 min each), followed by a single wash in PBS before exposure to DAB substrate (0.1 mg/ mL) in PBS for 25 min. Following a single wash in PBS, sections were transferred to glass slides, air dried and dehydrated through a series of graded ethanol washes. Sections were incubated in Citrisolv for 5 min then cover-slipped under Permount. For all pharmacological studies presented below, brain sections from test drug-treated mice were processed simultaneously with controls and METH-treated mice to normalize staining among treatment groups. Microglial reactivity was viewed under the light microscope and the number of stained cells observed after various treatments was quantified by stereological analysis using NIH Image. Cell counts were sampled from a 1.2 mm2 area of the striatum by a person blinded to the treatment conditions. Cells were counted from three independent sections from all like-treated mice, bilaterally, generating an average count for each treated subject. DA tissue content Depletion of striatal DA after METH treatment is widely used as an index of METH-induced toxicity to DA nerve endings. DA depletion from striatum faithfully reflects other measures of DA nerve ending damage caused by METH, such as reduced tyrosine hydroxylase immunoreactivity or reduced ligand binding to the DAT. Striata were dissected from brain at the times indicated below after METH treatments and stored frozen at )80C. Tissues were weighed and sonicated in 5 vol of 0.1M perchloric acid at 4C. Insoluble protein was removed by centrifugation and catecholamines were adsorbed to alumina using standard protocols. After elution from alumina, the amount of DA in samples was determined by HPLC with electrochemical detection by comparison with a standard curve of authentic DA. Values for DA are reported as ng/mg tissue (wet weight) and are corrected for recovery. Data analysis Dose–response effects of METH on striatal DA content and microglial counts were tested for significance by ANOVA. Individual treatment groups were compared with appropriate controls for DA and microglial counts with a one-way ANOVA followed by Dunnett’s multiple comparison test in GraphPad Prism 4 (GraphPad Software Inc., San Diego, CA, USA). Differences were considered significant if p < 0.05. The effects of drug treatments on core body temperature were analyzed by two-way ANOVA.
Results
The microglial response to a neurotoxic regimen of METH abates within 7 days (Thomas et al. 2004b), so mice were treated with a neurotoxic challenge of METH 7 days after a neurotoxic pre-treatment regimen. The results in Fig. 1 show that the neurotoxic METH challenge in naı¨ve mice (Sal/ METHNT) causes a significant reduction in striatal DA (79% reduction, p < 0.01, Dunnett’s test). DA levels remain significantly depleted 9 days after the METH tolerance regimen (METHNT/Sal; 81% reduction, p < 0.01, Dunnett’s test). When mice are challenged with the neurotoxic METH regimen 7 days after the same treatment (METHNT/ METHNT), DA levels are not further depleted (18% of
Fig. 1 Effects of METH pre-treatment on DA depletion caused by a neurotoxic challenge with METH. Mice (n ¼ 5–8 per group) were pretreated with Sal or a neurotoxic METH regimen, followed 7 days later with the same METH challenge (Sal/METHNT and METHNT/METHNT, respectively). Controls (Sal/Sal and METHNT/Sal) for all METH treatments were injections of physiological saline on the same schedule used for drug injections. Striatal DA levels were determined 48 h after the neurotoxic METH challenge. Results are presented as mean ± SEM. *Significantly different from control (Sal/Sal) as determined by one-way ANOVA followed by Dunnett’s multiple comparison test (p < 0.01).
control) by comparison with mice treated with a neurotoxic regimen (Sal/METHNT) and remain substantially lower than controls (Sal/Sal). The effect of drug treatment on striatal DA was significant in all groups that received METH (p < 0.01, Dunnett’s test, for all three groups relative to control). Figure 2 presents data on the microglial response to different treatment conditions. Few activated microglia are observed in controls (Sal/Sal) in agreement with our previous results (Thomas et al. 2004b), whereas mice given a neurotoxic regimen of the drug (Sal/METHNT) show a robust microglial activation. Mice pre-treated with METH and then challenged with saline (METHNT/Sal) 7 days later show few activated microglia, confirming our previous data that microglia return to control levels within this time frame. When mice are challenged with the neurotoxic METH regimen after pre-treatment with the same regimen (METHNT/METHNT) and killed 48 h after challenge, there is little evidence of a microglial response. If the time between METH pre-treatment and challenge treatments is increased from 7 to 30 days, or if the challenge dose of METH is increased from 5 to 10 mg/kg, microglial activation is still not seen. These data are included in Fig. 2 (e and f, respectively) as well and indicate that challenge doses of METH that cause severe DA neurotoxicity and microglial activation do not provoke a subsequent microglial response after mice have been pre-treated with the same METH neurotoxic regimen. The drug effect on microglial activation was significantly different from control only in mice treated with the acute neurotoxic METH challenge (Sal/METHNT, p < 0.01, Dunnett’s test). Finally, mice treated acutely with MPTP (Sal/MPTP) show a robust microglial activation in striatum as shown in Fig. 2. Treatment of mice with MPTP
2005 International Society for Neurochemistry, J. Neurochem. (2005) 92, 790–797
Attenuated microglial activation in METH tolerance 793
Fig. 3 Effects of a METH tolerance regimen on DA depletion caused by a neurotoxic challenge with METH. Mice (n ¼ 5–8 per group) were pre-treated with Sal or a METH tolerance regimen and were challenged 72 h later with a neurotoxic METH challenge (Sal/METHNT and METHTol/METHNT, respectively) and striatal DA levels were determined 48 h after the acute METH challenge. Controls for the challenge treatments (Sal/Sal and METHTol/Sal) were injections of physiological saline on the same schedule used for drug injections. Results are presented as mean ± SEM. *Significantly different from control (Sal/ Sal) as determined by one-way ANOVA followed by Dunnett’s multiple comparison test (p < 0.01). The METHTol/METHNT group was not different from the METHTol/Sal group (p > 0.05) although the DA levels in the latter group were reduced (17% below the METHTol/Sal group).
Fig. 2 Effects of METH pre-treatment on microglial activation caused by a neurotoxic challenge with METH or MPTP. Mice (n ¼ 5–8 per group) were treated as described in Fig. 1 and analyzed for microglial activation in striatum 48 h after the METH challenge. Stereological counts of microglia were obtained with NIH Image and are included on each panel as mean ± SEM. Treatment conditions for each panel are (a) Sal/Sal, (b) Sal/METHNT, (c) METHNT/Sal and (d) METHNT/ METHNT. (e) Microglial staining in mice challenged with METH 30 days after pre-treatment. (f) Microglial staining in mice treated with a high-dose METH challenge (4· 10 mg/kg, i.p., 2-h interval between injections vs. 5 mg/kg) 7 days after METH pre-treatment. (g) Microglial staining after an acute challenge with MPTP (Sal/MPTP). (h) From mice challenged with MPTP after METH pre-treatment (METHNT/ MPTP). The Sal/METHNT and Sal/MPTP treatment groups were significantly different from controls (Sal/Sal) as determined by Dunnett’s multiple comparison test (p < 0.01). The horizontal scale bar represents 150 lm.
7 days after a METH neurotoxic regimen (METHNT/MPTP) did not activate microglia above control levels, as shown in Fig. 2, in agreement with results observed in METHNT/ METHNT-treated mice. The METH pre-treatment regimen used above caused substantial neurotoxicity and created a floor-effect with regard to DA depletion. Therefore, we used a less severe, low-dose regimen to generate tolerance to METH
neurotoxicity. The METH dose in these experiments (2 mg/ kg, 4 ·, 2-h interval between injections) was selected from our previous dose–effect studies as one that caused minimal DA toxicity and microglial activation (Thomas et al. 2004b). Figure 3 shows that three treatments with 2 mg/kg (i.p., 4 ·, 2-h interval between injections) with a 48-h time period between each regimen, followed 72 h later with an acute challenge, resulted in the development of tolerance. A neurotoxic challenge with METH (Sal/METHNT) resulted in a significant (i.e. 79%) DA depletion by comparison with controls (Sal/Sal). The lower dose of 2 mg/kg METH, when given on three separate occasions (METHTol/Sal), did cause a mild depletion of DA (72% of control). However, the effects of an acute METH challenge were significantly blunted in mice previously treated with the low-dose tolerance regimen (METHTol/METHNT). METH typically reduces striatal DA levels by about 80% without pre-treatment and this response is reduced to 17% depletion by comparison with the lowdose METH tolerance regimen. The microglial response to the lower-dose METH tolerance regimen is shown in Fig. 4. In agreement with our previous report (Thomas et al. 2004b), acute METH (Sal/ METHNT) provokes a substantial activation of striatal microglia by comparison with controls (Sal/Sal). Mice that received the tolerance regimen without an acute METH challenge (METHTol/Sal) showed very little microglial activation 9 days after drug treatment. Figure 4 shows that tolerance has also developed to the effects of METH on microglial activation. The extent of microglial activation is significantly lower in mice that received the tolerance
2005 International Society for Neurochemistry, J. Neurochem. (2005) 92, 790–797
794 D. M. Thomas and D. M. Kuhn
Fig. 4 Effects of a METH tolerance regimen on microglial activation caused by a neurotoxic challenge with METH. Mice (n ¼ 5–8 per group) were treated as described in Fig. 3 and analyzed for microglial activation in striatum 48 h after the acute METH challenge. Stereological counts of microglia were obtained with NIH Image and are included on each panel as mean ± SEM. Treatment conditions for each panel are (a) Sal/Sal, (b) Sal/METHNT, (c) METHTol/Sal and (d) METHTol/METHNT, as described in Materials and methods. The Sal/ METHNT and METHTol/METHNT treatment groups were significantly different from control (Sal/Sal) as determined by Dunnett’s multiple comparison test (p < 0.01). The horizontal scale bar represents 200 lm.
regimen before the neurotoxic challenge (METHTol/ METHNT), in agreement with neurochemical findings (Fig. 3 above). The only groups that differed from control with respect to microglial activation were the Sal/METHNT and METHTol/METHNT groups (p < 0.01 for each, Dunnett’s test). The effect of the two different METH pre-treatment regimens on the body temperature response to METH challenge is shown in Fig. 5. Mice pre-treated with the neurotoxic or the tolerance regimen (METHNT/METHNT and METHTol/METHNT groups, respectively) each developed hyperthermia to a greater extent than mice given only a neurotoxic METH challenge (Sal/METHNT). Core temperatures of all groups receiving the neurotoxic challenge of METH, regardless of the pre-treatment regimen used, were significantly elevated over those of controls (Sal/Sal; p < 0.0001 by two-way ANOVA). To quantify the differences in core temperatures among the four treatment groups throughout the 9-h time period when temperatures were monitored, the area under each curve in Fig. 5 was calculated against a baseline of 36.5C (body temperatures in all groups remained above this level). The areas under the curves (in arbitrary units) for each treatment condition were Sal/Sal (360), Sal/METHNT (573), METHNT/METHNT (831) and METHTol/METHNT (897). Taken together, these results indicate that tolerance does not develop to the hyperthermic
Fig. 5 Effects of a neurotoxic challenge with METH on core temperatures of mice pre-treated with a neurotoxic or tolerance regimne of METH. Mice (n ¼ 4–5 per group) were treated with tolerance/challenge conditions as indicated and core temperatures were monitored by telemetry every hour for 9 h on the day of challenge. Results are presented as mean body temperature (C) for each group at the indicated time points. SEM bars were omitted for the sake of clarity and were less than 10% of the mean in all groups. Core body temperatures in all groups receiving a METH neurotoxic challenge were significantly different from the Sal/Sal control (p < 0.0001, two-way ANOVA).
effects of METH. It should also be noted that the dose of METH used presently (5 mg/kg) causes a mild hyperthermia by comparion with doses of 10 mg/kg or higher (Bowyer et al. 1992, 1994; Miller and O’Callaghan 1994; O’Callaghan and Miller 1994). Discussion
METH causes a sustained neurotoxicity within DA nerve endings of the striatum. The mechanisms by which METH causes neurotoxicity are not understood, but a great deal of evidence points to oxidative and/or nitrosative stress as mediators of this effect (Wrona et al. 1997; Davidson et al. 2001; Cadet et al. 2003; Kita et al. 2003a). The cellular source of the reactive oxygen species and reactive nitrogen species that contribute to METH toxicity has eluded identification so far, but the recent implication of microglia as participants in this process has helped narrow the search (Escubedo et al. 1998; Pubill et al. 2002; Guilarte et al. 2003; Pubill et al. 2003; LaVoie et al. 2004; Thomas et al. 2004a,b). In fact, microglia can produce virtually all reactive species that have been identified as factors in METH neurotoxicity including nitric oxide and superoxide, and many of the transcriptional factors (e.g. NFjB, AP-1, cFOS) that are involved in METH neuronal toxicity (Davidson et al. 2001; Cadet et al. 2003) are also required for microglial activation (Pocock and Liddle 2001; Wyss-Coray and Mucke 2002). Chronic amphetamine use is associated with the development of pharmacological tolerance and behavioral sensitization, two factors that are thought to sustain the addictive properties of these drugs. It is well known that tolerance develops to the neurotoxicity of METH (Gygi et al. 1996; Stephans and Yamamoto 1996; Riddle et al. 2002; Johnson-
2005 International Society for Neurochemistry, J. Neurochem. (2005) 92, 790–797
Attenuated microglial activation in METH tolerance 795
Davis et al. 2003; Johnson-Davis et al. 2004) and crosstolerance is observed between the neurotoxic effects of METH and MPTP (Sziraki et al. 1994; Kita et al. 2003b). Tolerance also develops to MPTP-induced astrogliosis (O’Callaghan et al. 1990). Johnson-Davis et al. (2004) showed recently that the effects of a neurotoxic regimen of METH on VMAT trafficking and function developed tolerance and these investigators ruled out adaptive changes in METH pharmacokinetics and transport to the brain and reductions in drug-induced hyperthermia as mediators of this effect, at least in rats (Johnson-Davis et al. 2004). Additional studies are required to determine if altered METH pharmacokinetics would account for tolerance in mouse models. Beyond this, the mechanisms that mediate tolerance to METH neurotoxicity are not known. In view of emerging data implicating microglia as participants in this process, we decided to analyze the microglial response under conditions where tolerance to the neurotoxic effects of METH has developed. Two different treatments were used presently to study the microglial response to METH tolerance. Previous studies from our lab showed that microglial activation was maximal 48 h after a neurotoxic regimen of METH and returned to control levels after 7 days (Thomas et al. 2004b). Therefore, mice were treated with a neurotoxic challenge of METH 7 days after pre-treatment with the same neurotoxic regimen and microglial activation was not observed. If the dose of the second METH challenge was increased from 5 mg/kg to 10 mg/kg, or if the time between METH pre-treatment and challenge treatment was lengthened to 30 days, mice were still unable to mount a microglial response. The neurotoxic pretreatment regimen used in these experiments caused severe, persistent DA depletion, so the failure to see further reductions in DA after the second METH challenge may reflect a floor effect more so than a development of tolerance. Therefore, a lower-dose METH tolerance regimen was tested with the intent of minimizing DA depletion and microglial activation. Treatment of mice with 2 mg/kg METH for 3 days, with a 48h interval between each regimen, followed 72 h later with an acute neurotoxic challenge, revealed that tolerance to drug neurotoxicity and microglial activation had developed. The two pre-treatment regimens used differ significantly in that the neurotoxic regimen caused extensive microglial activation, which was allowed to subside prior to the neurotoxic challenge with METH, whereas the tolerance regimen did not result in significant microglial activation. Therefore, the inability of a METH neurotoxic challenge to provoke microglial activation after a tolerance regimen is not determined by the prior activation status of the microglia. Assuming that microglial activation precedes and contributes to METH-induced nerve ending damage, it follows that microglial tolerance could result in tolerance to METH neurotoxicity. Bowyer and colleagues were the first to make the important observation that METH intoxication results in
microglial activation (Bowyer et al. 1994), and these investigators suggested that microglial activation occurred in response to neuronal damage. Therefore, it could be argued that microglial activation seen after acute METH administration does not contribute to its neurotoxicity. The same rationale could be applied to tolerance induction and would assert that microglia do not show activation because nerve endings have developed tolerance to the damaging effects of METH. However, a number of emerging results are consistent with the possibility that microglia participate in acute METH toxicity as well as in the development of tolerance to it, including the following: (i) microglia rapidly develop tolerance to various provocative stimuli (Alves-Rosa et al. 2002; Nadeau and Rivest 2002; Ajmone-Cat et al. 2003; Nakajima et al. 2003) so it is certainly possible that microglia made tolerant to METH would produce lesser amounts of those reactive species that are thought to mediate drug-induced neuronal damage (e.g. reactive oxygen species or reactive nitrogen species); (ii) METH intoxication increases the expression of numerous microglia-associated genes within hours of drug administration, well before toxicity is seen (Thomas et al. 2004b); (iii) microglial activation precedes the emergence of nerve ending toxicity (LaVoie et al. 2004; Thomas et al. 2004b) and develops along the same time course with it (Baucum et al. 2004); (iv) microglial activation caused by the amphetamines is restricted to those drugs known to cause neurotoxicity (e.g. METH, MDMA, parachloramphetamine), minimizing a role for pharmacological effects such as hyperthermia and hyperactivity as causes (Thomas et al. 2004c). It is probably not possible to classify the participation of microglia in METH neurotoxicity as either direct (i.e. causal) or indirect (i.e. reactive to damage). Clearly, considerable cross-talk is exchanged between distressed neurons and microglia to fuel the very complex process of neuronal damage (Bruce-Keller 1999; Polazzi and Contestabile 2002; Kerschensteiner et al. 2003). Therefore, we conclude that microglia participate in the acute phase of METH neurotoxicity and that an attenuation in microglial activation after repeated, intermittent METH administration mediates the adaptive tolerance to its neurotoxic effects. The development of tolerance to METH neurotoxicity and microglial activation could be viewed as a neuroprotective response in view of the fact that tolerant animals show much less neurotoxicity after METH intoxication than mice challenged acutely with the same treatment regimen. However, the ability of prior METH exposure to arrest microglial activation (for up to 30 days in mice) could have potentially dangerous effects on the central nervous system that are not readily apparent. For example, lesions of the brain, stroke or infection are associated with microglial activation (Kreutzberg 1996; Moore and Thanos 1996; Stoll and Jander 1999). Under these conditions, microglia are thought to serve neuroprotective roles by removing damaged cell fragments
2005 International Society for Neurochemistry, J. Neurochem. (2005) 92, 790–797
796 D. M. Thomas and D. M. Kuhn
or invading pathogens and by secreting various growth factors and cytokines that limit further damage or even participate in neuronal recovery (Hanisch 2002; Streit 2002). The signaling pathways within microglia that direct their responses toward neuronal protection or damage are not fully understood (Elward and Gasque 2003; Kerschensteiner et al. 2003). However, it seems clear that METH intoxication creates conditions within the brain that are unlike stroke or infection where the protective, immune-like functions of microglia are evoked. Prior, intermittent exposure to subneurotoxic doses of METH to an extent that provokes tolerance could hamper the adaptive immune response of the central nervous system (Nguyen et al. 2002) and worsen the consequences associated with subsequent neuronal damage or disease. This possibility is currently under investigation in our laboratory. The present finding that tolerance does not develop to the hyperthermic effects of METH, regardless of the drug pretreatment condition used (i.e. neurotoxic or tolerance regimen), agrees well with other studies (Johnson-Davis et al. 2003, 2004) and provides an additional degree of dissociation of this response from drug-induced neurotoxicity. The hyperthermic response to METH challenge observed presently was rather mild (Fig. 5) and could be explained by the use of a lower dose (i.e. 5 mg/kg per injection · 4). Higher doses (e.g. 10 mg/kg per injection · 4) and elevated ambient temperature enhance METH neurotoxicity (Miller and O’Callaghan 2003) and it is well known that the extent of DA nerve-ending damage caused by METH is highly correlated with the degree of druginduced hyperthermia (Bowyer et al. 1992, 1994; Miller and O’Callaghan 1994; O’Callaghan and Miller 1994). The observation that METH elevates core body temperature significantly in pre-treated mice, without provoking microglial activation, further supports our finding that hyperthermia does not appear to be the signal that prompts microglial activation after METH intoxication (Thomas et al. 2004b). Hyperthermia is a clinically significant sideeffect of METH ingestion (Urbina and Jones 2004). The finding that tolerance does not develop to METH-induced hyperthermia, even after it has developed to its neurotoxic effects, reinforces the dangers associated with this drug of abuse. Acknowledgements This research was supported by the National Institute on Drug Abuse grants DA10756 and DA14692 and by a VA Merit Award.
References Abekawa T., Ohmori T. and Koyama T. (1997) Tolerance to the neurotoxic effect of methamphetamine in rats behaviorally sensitized to methamphetamine or amphetamine. Brain Res. 767, 34–44.
Ajmone-Cat M. A., Nicolini A. and Minghetti L. (2003) Prolonged exposure of microglia to lipopolysaccharide modifies the intracellular signaling pathways and selectively promotes prostaglandin E2 synthesis. J. Neurochem. 87, 1193–1203. Aloisi F. (2001) Immune function of microglia. Glia 36, 165–179. Alves-Rosa F., Vulcano M., Beigier-Bompadre M., Fernandez G., Palermo M. and Isturiz M. A. (2002) Interleukin-1b induces in vivo tolerance to lipopolysaccharide in mice. Clin. Exp. Immunol. 128, 221–228. Baucum A. J. 2nd, Rau K. S., Riddle E. L., Hanson G. R. and Fleckenstein A. E. (2004) Methamphetamine increases dopamine transporter higher molecular weight complex formation via a dopamine- and hyperthermia-associated mechanism. J. Neurosci. 24, 3436–3443. Bowyer J. F., Tank A. W., Newport G. D., Slikker W. Jr, Ali S. F. and Holson R. R. (1992) The influence of environmental temperature on the transient effects of methamphetamine on dopamine levels and dopamine release in rat striatum. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 260, 817–824. Bowyer J. F., Davies D. L., Schmued L., Broening H. W., Newport G. D., Slikker W. Jr and Holson R. R. (1994) Further studies of the role of hyperthermia in methamphetamine neurotoxicity. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 268, 1571–1580. Brown J. M., Hanson G. R. and Fleckenstein A. E. (2000) Methamphetamine rapidly decreases vesicular dopamine uptake. J. Neurochem. 74, 2221–2223. Bruce-Keller A. J. (1999) Microglial–neuronal interactions in synaptic damage and recovery. J. Neurosci. Res. 58, 191–201. Cadet J. L., Jayanthi S. and Deng X. (2003) Speed kills: cellular and molecular bases of methamphetamine-induced nerve terminal degeneration and neuronal apoptosis. FASEB J. 17, 1775–1788. Davidson C., Gow A. J., Lee T. H. and Ellinwood E. H. (2001) Methamphetamine neurotoxicity: necrotic and apoptotic mechanisms and relevance to human abuse and treatment. Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev. 36, 1–22. Elward K. and Gasque P. (2003) ‘Eat me’ and ‘don’t eat me’ signals govern the innate immune response and tissue repair in the CNS: emphasis on the critical role of the complement system. Mol. Immunol. 40, 85–94. Escubedo E., Guitart L., Sureda F. X., Jimenez A., Pubill D., Pallas M., Camins A. and Camarasa J. (1998) Microgliosis and down-regulation of adenosine transporter induced by methamphetamine in rats. Brain Res. 814, 120–126. Fleckenstein A. E., Gibb J. W. and Hanson G. R. (2000) Differential effects of stimulants on monoaminergic transporters: pharmacological consequences and implications for neurotoxicity. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 406, 1–13. Frederick D. L., Ali S. F., Gillam M. P., Gossett J., Slikker W. and Paule M. G. (1998) Acute effects of dexfenfluramine (d-FEN) and methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA) before and after shortcourse, high-dose treatment. Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 844, 183–190. Gibb J. W., Johnson M., Elayan I., Lim H. K., Matsuda L. and Hanson G. R. (1997) Neurotoxicity of amphetamines and their metabolites. NIDA Res. Monogr. 173, 128–145. Guilarte T. R., Nihei M. K., McGlothan J. L. and Howard A. S. (2003) Methamphetamine-induced deficits of brain monoaminergic neuronal markers: distal axotomy or neuronal plasticity. Neuroscience 122, 499–513. Gygi M. P., Gygi S. P., Johnson M., Wilkins D. G., Gibb J. W. and Hanson G. R. (1996) Mechanisms for tolerance to methamphetamine effects. Neuropharmacology 35, 751–757. Hanisch U. K. (2002) Microglia as a source and target of cytokines. Glia 40, 140–155. Itzhak Y. and Ali S. F. (2002) Behavioral consequences of methamphetamine-induced neurotoxicity in mice: relevance to the psy-
2005 International Society for Neurochemistry, J. Neurochem. (2005) 92, 790–797
Attenuated microglial activation in METH tolerance 797
chopathology of methamphetamine addiction. Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 965, 127–135. Johnson-Davis K. L., Fleckenstein A. E. and Wilkins D. G. (2003) The role of hyperthermia and metabolism as mechanisms of tolerance to methamphetamine neurotoxicity. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 482, 151–154. Johnson-Davis K. L., Truong J. G., Fleckenstein A. E. and Wilkins D. G. (2004) Alterations in vesicular dopamine uptake contribute to tolerance to the neurotoxic effects of methamphetamine. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 309, 578–586. Kerschensteiner M., Stadelmann C., Dechant G., Wekerle H. and Hohlfeld R. (2003) Neurotrophic cross-talk between the nervous and immune systems: implications for neurological diseases. Ann. Neurol. 53, 292–304. Kita T., Wagner G. C. and Nakashima T. (2003a) Current research on methamphetamine-induced neurotoxicity: animal models of monoamine disruption. J. Pharmacol. Sci. 92, 178–195. Kita T., Saraya T., Konishi N., Matsunari Y., Shimada K., Nakamura M., O’Hara K., Wagner G. C. and Nakashima T. (2003b) 1-Methyl-4phenyl-1,2,3,6-tetrahydropyridine pretreatment attenuates methamphetamine-induced dopamine toxicity. Pharmacol. Toxicol. 92, 71–80. Kreutzberg G. W. (1996) Microglia: a sensor for pathological events in the CNS. Trends Neurosci. 19, 312–318. LaVoie M. J., Card J. P. and Hastings T. G. (2004) Microglial activation precedes dopamine terminal pathology in methamphetamineinduced neurotoxicity. Exp. Neurol. 187, 47–57. Miller D. B. and O’Callaghan J. P. (1994) Environment-, drug- and stress-induced alterations in body temperature affect the neurotoxicity of substituted amphetamines in the C57BL/6J mouse. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 270, 752–760. Miller D. B. and O’Callaghan J. P. (2003) Elevated environmental temperature and methamphetamine neurotoxicity. Environ. Res. 92, 48–53. Molliver M. E., Berger U. V., Mamounas L. A., Molliver D. C., O’Hearn E. and Wilson M. A. (1990) Neurotoxicity of MDMA and related compounds: anatomic studies. Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 600, 649–661. Moore S. and Thanos S. (1996) The concept of microglia in relation to central nervous system disease and regeneration. Prog. Neurobiol. 48, 441–460. Nadeau S. and Rivest S. (2002) Endotoxemia prevents the cerebral inflammatory wave induced by intraparenchymal lipopolysaccharide injection: role of glucocorticoids and CD14. J. Immunol. 169, 3370–3381. Nakajima K., Tohyama Y., Kohsaka S. and Kurihara T. (2003) Suppression of lipopolysaccharide-dependent tumor necrosis factor a induction in rat microglia, in which protein kinase Ca is downregulated. Neurosci. Lett. 343, 33–36. Nguyen M. D., Julien J. P. and Rivest S. (2002) Innate immunity: the missing link in neuroprotection and neurodegeneration? Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3, 216–227. O’Callaghan J. P. and Miller D. B. (1994) Neurotoxicity profiles of substituted amphetamines in the C57BL/6J mouse. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 270, 741–751. O’Callaghan J. P., Miller D. B. and Reinhard J. F. Jr (1990) 1-Methyl-4phenyl-1,2,3,6-tetrahydropyridine (MPTP)-induced damage of striatal dopaminergic fibers attenuates subsequent astrocyte response to MPTP. Neurosci. Lett. 117, 228–233. Pocock J. M. and Liddle A. C. (2001) Microglial signalling cascades in neurodegenerative disease. Prog. Brain Res. 132, 555–565. Polazzi E. and Contestabile A. (2002) Reciprocal interactions between microglia and neurons: from survival to neuropathology. Rev. Neurosci. 13, 221–242.
Pubill D., Verdaguer E., Sureda F. X., Camins A., Pallas M., Camarasa J. and Escubedo E. (2002) Carnosine prevents methamphetamineinduced gliosis but not dopamine terminal loss in rats. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 448, 165–168. Pubill D., Canudas A. M., Pallas M., Camins A., Camarasa J. and Escubedo E. (2003) Different glial response to methamphetamineand methylenedioxymethamphetamine-induced neurotoxicity. Naunyn. Schmiedebergs Arch. Pharmacol. 367, 490–499. Riddle E. L., Kokoshka J. M., Wilkins D. G., Hanson G. R. and Fleckenstein A. E. (2002) Tolerance to the neurotoxic effects of methamphetamine in young rats. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 435, 181–185. Schmidt C. J., Sonsalla P. K., Hanson G. R., Peat M. A. and Gibb J. W. (1985a) Methamphetamine-induced depression of monoamine synthesis in the rat: development of tolerance. J. Neurochem. 44, 852–855. Schmidt C. J., Gehlert D. R., Peat M. A., Sonsalla P. K., Hanson G. R., Wamsley J. K. and Gibb J. W. (1985b) Studies on the mechanism of tolerance to methamphetamine. Brain Res. 343, 305–313. Segal D. S., Kuczenski R., O’Neil M. L., Melega W. P. and Cho A. K. (2003) Escalating dose methamphetamine pretreatment alters the behavioral and neurochemical profiles associated with exposure to a high-dose methamphetamine binge. Neuropsychopharmacology 28, 1730–1740. Shankaran M. and Gudelsky G. A. (1999) A neurotoxic regimen of MDMA suppresses behavioral, thermal and neurochemical responses to subsequent MDMA administration. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 147, 66–72. Stephans S. and Yamamoto B. (1996) Methamphetamine pretreatment and the vulnerability of the striatum to methamphetamine neurotoxicity. Neuroscience 72, 593–600. Stoll G. and Jander S. (1999) The role of microglia and macrophages in the pathophysiology of the CNS. Prog. Neurobiol. 58, 233–247. Streit W. J. (1990) An improved staining method for rat microglial cells using the lectin from Griffonia simplicifolia (GSA I–B4). J. Histochem. Cytochem. 38, 1683–1686. Streit W. J. (2002) Microglia as neuroprotective, immunocompetent cells of the CNS. Glia 40, 133–139. Sziraki I., Kardos V., Patthy M., Patfalusi M. and Budai G. (1994) Methamphetamine protects against MPTP neurotoxicity in C57BL mice. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 251, 311–314. Thomas D. M., Francescutti-Verbeem D. M., Liu X. and Kuhn D. M. (2004a) Identification of differentially regulated transcripts in mouse striatum following methamphetamine treatment – an oligonucleotide microarray approach. J. Neurochem. 88, 380– 393. Thomas D. M., Walker P. D., Benjamins J. A., Geddes T. J. and Kuhn D. M. (2004b) Methamphetamine neurotoxicity in dopamine nerve endings of the striatum is associated with microglial activation. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 311, 1–7. Thomas D. M., Dowgiert J., Geddes T. J., Francescutti-Verbeem D., Liu X. and Kuhn D. M. (2004c) Microglial activation is a pharmacologically specific marker for the neurotoxic amphetamines. Neurosci. Lett. 367, 349–354. Urbina A. and Jones K. (2004) Crystal methamphetamine, its analogues, and HIV infection: medical and psychiatric aspects of a new epidemic. Clin. Infect. Dis. 38, 890–894. Wrona M. Z., Yang Z., Zhang F. and Dryhurst G. (1997) Potential new insights into the molecular mechanisms of methamphetamineinduced neurodegeneration. NIDA Res. Monogr. 173, 146–174. Wyss-Coray T. and Mucke L. (2002) Inflammation in neurodegenerative disease – a double-edged sword. Neuron 35, 419–432.
2005 International Society for Neurochemistry, J. Neurochem. (2005) 92, 790–797