kat bil fu l sut k t bi l fu l. âpacifierâ. âcatâ. âcarâ. âbirdâ. Mispronunced sat .... Kuhl, P. K., Williams, K. A., Lacerda, F., Stevens, K. N., & Lindblom, B. (1992).
Background
Phonological specificity of vowels and consonants in 20-month-olds’ word representations
• Infants discriminate phonetic contrasts already before 4 months of age (e.g., Eimas, Siqueland, Jusczyk, & Vigorito, 1971).
• Discriminate both native and nonnative contrasts from the beginning • Sensitivity to nonnative contrasts decline around 12 months (Werker & Tees, 1984) • Sensitivity to native contrasts increases around 12 months
Anders Højen Center for Child Language Department of Language and Communication University of Southern Denmark
1
Discrimination of Hindi and Salish by Englishlearning infants 6-8 mo. Discriminate contrasts in both languages 8-10 mo. Half of the infants could discriminate 10-12 mo. Few could discriminate (Werker, 1989)
2
Discrimination of the English /r/-/l/ distinction: Sensitivity increases for American infants but decreases for Japanese infants between 6-12 mo. (Kuhl et al., 2006)
3
Background
4
Background
• High capability of perceiving native phonetic distinctions at around 1 year • At this time word learning begins • Word learning requires encoding of sound for later recognition • Expect infants to encode the sound pattern of words accurately because of their perceptual abilities at 1 year of age
• BUT an early report indicated that at around 1 year, infants confuse minimal pairs (Shvachkin, 1973) • They could discriminate • …but could not associate the right word with a referent • Access to phonetic detail hampered by word-object association? 5
6
1 (c) Center for Børnesprog
Background
Background
• The effect of word-object association on perception shown again later in habituation-switch task (Stager & Werker, 1997) • 14-month-olds were taught novel “words”:
• Reduced phonological sensitivity at 14 mo. when associating words with objects? – I.e., words phonologically underspecified?
• Only novel words • Sensitivity to mispronunciations of wellknown words at 14 mo. shown with switch task (Fennel & Werker, 2003) and inter-modal preferential looking task or IPL (Swingley & Aslin,
– very different word pair: Lif – Neem – minimal pair: Bih – Dih
• In test phase, infants noticed word-object switch for Lif-Neem pair, but not Bih-Dih • But infants could discriminate Bih-Dih
2002) 7
8
Background
Background
• Sensitivity to phonological detail of well-known words at 18-23 months (Swingley & Aslin, 2000) • “Recovery” of sensitivity to phonological detail of novel words at 17-20 months (Werker et al. 2002)
Hypothesis (Fennel & Werker, 2003) • Word-learning (novel) too computationally demanding at 14 mo. but not 20 mo. to encode words accurately • Word recognition (well-known) less demanding – can be done already at 14 mo.
14 mo. 20 mo.
Well-known
Novel
√ √
÷ √ 9
10
Background
Background
• The just-reviewed studies examined sensitivity to consonants • But vowels might have either stronger or weaker representation than consonants – Stronger: Vowel perception language specific sooner than consonants perception (Kuhl, 1991; Kuhl et al. 1992; Polka & Werker, 1994; but cf. Polka & Bohn, 1996)
– Weaker: Vowels constrain lexical access less than consonants in adults (Cutler et al., 2000)
• More or less detail for vowels than consonants in early word representions? 11
• Conflicting results • 20-month-olds showed sensitivity mispronunciations in the consonant but not the vowel in novel words (Nazzi, 2005) • 15 to 24-month-olds sensitive to mispronunciations in both vowels and consonants in well-known words (Mani & Plunkett, 2007)
12
2 (c) Center for Børnesprog
This study
Methods
Tested sensitivity to vowel and consonant mispronunciations in well-known words using IPL Purpose • To pit sensitivity to vowel mispronunciations against consonant mispronunciations to determine if vowels constrain lexical access less than do consonants • To examine sensitivity to consonant mispronunciations in the first vs. last consonant in CVC words
Participants • Thirty-two 19 to 21-month-olds – 16 each in experiment 1 and 2 • All were healthy full-term infants born to native speakers of Danish
13
Methods – Exp. 1
Methods – Exp. 1
Auditory stimuli • Four correctly pronounced (CP) and four mispronounced (MP) Danish CVC • MP words differed from the correct words in either the initial consonant or the vowel. • Each MP stimulus could be “reconstructed” to two of the correct words by changing the vowel or initial consonant 15
Methods – Exp. 1 (Vowel target)
(Monitor) ”Sut”
”Kat” Change consonant
Change vowel
Kut
Table 1. Auditory stimuli, which were either correctly pronounced or mispronounced
Correct
Mispronunced
Danish
IPA
Enlish
sut kat bil ful
sut kt bil ful
“pacifier” “cat” “car” “bird”
sat kut bul fil
st kut bul fil
16
Methods – Exp. 1
Example of MP trial (Consonant target)
14
17
Procedure • Infant sits on parents lap in dim booth • On each trial, two pics shown for 5000 ms • At 2250 ms, word stimulus (CP or MP) is presented in carrier sentence: Se, en ___ • Offline examination of picture fixation after word stimulus, frame by frame from video recordings (show video) • Observation window: 1500 ms 18
3 (c) Center for Børnesprog
Methods – Exp. 1
Methods – Exp. 1
Procedure • In CP trials, the picture named by the auditory stimulus was the target • In MP trials, both pictures were possible targets. Auditory stimulus e.g., kut – Consonant target: Kat-picture (correct consonant, wrong vowel) – Vowel target: Sut-picture (correct vowel wrong consonant)
Procedure • Preference for looking at the consonant target in MP trials = consonants constrain lexical access more than vowels (weaker specification of vowels) • Preference for looking at the vowel target = vowels constrain lexical access more than consonants • 16 test trials (8 CP, 8 MP)
19
20
Results – Exp. 1
Small positive difference score in MP trials (looking time a consonant target minus vowel target),
not signific. different from 0 Non-significant preference for consonant target
Δ looking time, ms
preference in CP trials
• Preference for target in CP trials = experiment works • Non-significant preference for consonant target = no evidence that consonants and vowels differ in constraining lexical access • Does not suggest a difference in specificity for vowels and consonants in well-known word representations at 20 months
500 400 300 200 100 0
Target minus Distractor
Cons. target minus vowel target
21
22
Methods Exp. 2
Results – Exp. 2 Difference score (looking time at target minus distractor in MP trials) shows target
Examined sensitivity to mispronunciation in vowel vs. final consonant in CVC words Stimuli • Four CP words and four MP words, analogous to experiment 1 stimuli • Each MP stimulus could be reconstructed to two CP stimuli, e.g., Kot
preference in CP trials Small negative difference score in MP trials (looking time a consonant target minus vowel target),
not signific. different from 0
– Consonant target: Kat-picture – Vowel target: Kop-picture 23
Non-significant preference for vowel target
600
Δ looking time, ms
Difference score (looking time at target minus distractor in MP trials) shows target
Discussion
500 400 300 200 100 0 -100 -200 -300
Target minus Distractor
Cons. target minus vowel target 24
4 (c) Center for Børnesprog
Discussion
Discussion
• Non-significant preference for vowel target = no evidence that consonants and vowels differ in constraining lexical access • Does not suggest a difference in specificity for vowels and consonants in well-known words at 20 months
Interpretation problem • Lack of preference for either consonant or vowel target upon hearing MP word, e.g., kot, could mean two things: 1. Rejection of kot as either “kat” or “kop” – vowels and consonants both well-specified
– Supports Mani & Plunkett (2007)
2. Accept of kot as both “kat” and “kop” – vowels and consonants both phonologically underspecified 25
26
Discussion
Discussion
Interpretation problem • Under specification of both vowels and consonants in well-known words at 20 mo. is unlikely
Final consonant vs. vowel
Initial consonant vs. vowel 500
600
400
Non-significant preference for cons. target
300 200 100
Δ looking time, ms
Δ looking time, ms
– evidence of substantial detail for vowels and consonants in English infants aged 14-15 mo. – if both vowel and final consonant are underspecified, it should be hard to tell kat from kop in CP trials
• Possible effect of position of mispronunciations 500 400
Non-significant preference for vowel target
300 200 100 0 -100 -200
• Convinced?
0 27
Discussion
-300
Target minus Distractor
Cons. target minus vowel target
Target minus Distractor
Cons. target minus vowel target
28
Discussion
• Suggests that infants process words from linearly, “from one end” – If they hear kut, they look more for a picture whose name begins in /k/ (kat) than a name whose rhyme is right (sut) – If they hear kot, they look more for kop than kat
• Hypothesis: lexical access constrained more by “early” sounds in words than late sounds
• Could this effect solve interpretation problem? • If the effect of position of mispronunciation is significant, how could both vowels and consonants be underspecified?
– But they eventually discard MP forms resulting in lower looking times than in CP trials 29
30
5 (c) Center for Børnesprog
Conclusion
Thank you!
• Vowels and consonants do not differ in constraining lexical access to well-known words at 20 months • Suggests that vowels are as phonologically well-specified as consonants are in word representations at 20 months • Future: difference between vowels and consonant specificity at 14 months?
• Presentation will soon be available from – www.andershojen.dk – www.sdu.dk/cfb
• These people contributed to the research: – Thomas O. Madsen, Werner Vach, Torkil Østerbye, Karina Faber, Hans Basbøll, Sueli Caporali, Dorthe Bleses
• Research sponsored by – University of Southern Denmark – Widex A/S
31
32
References
References
Cutler, A., Sebastián-Gallés, N., Soler-Vilageliu, O., & van Ooijen, B. (2000). Constraints of vowels and consonants on lexical selection: Cross-linguistic comparisons. Memory and Cognition, 28(5), 746-755. Eimas, P. D., Siqueland, E. R., Jusczyk, P. W., & Vigorito, J. (1971). Speech perception in infants. Science, 171, 303-306. Fennel, C. T., & Werker, J. F. (2003). Early word learners' ability to access phonetic detail in well-known words. Language and Speech, 46(2/3), 245-264. Kuhl, P. K. (1991). Human adults and human infants show a 'perceptual magnet effect' for the prototypes of speech categories, monkeys do not. Perception and Psychophysics, 50(2), 93-107. Kuhl, P. K., Stevens, E., Hayashi, A., Deguchi, T., Kiritani, S., & Iverson, P. (2006). Infants show a facilitation effect for native language phonetic perception between 6 and 12 months. Developmental Science, 9(2), F13-F21. Kuhl, P. K., Williams, K. A., Lacerda, F., Stevens, K. N., & Lindblom, B. (1992). Linguistic experience alters phonetic perception in infants by 6 months of age. Science, 255(5044), 606-608. Mani, N., & Plunkett, K. (2007). Phonological specificity of vowels and consonants in early lexical representations. Journal of Memory and Language, 57, 252-272. Nazzi, T. (2005). Use of phonetic specificity during the acquisition of new words: differences between consonants and vowels. Cognition, 98, 13-30. Polka, L., & Bohn, O.-S. (1996). A cross-language comparison of vowel perception in English-learning and German-learning infants. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 100(1), 577-592.
Polka, L., & Werker, J. F. (1994). Developmental-Changes in Perception of Nonnative Vowel Contrasts. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance, 20(2), 421-435. Shvachkin, N. K. (1973). The development of phonemic perception in early childhood. In C. A. Ferguson & D. I. Slobin (Eds.), Studies of child language development (pp. 92-127). New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston. Stager, C. L., & Werker, J. F. (1997). Infants listen for more phonetic detail in speech perception than in word-learning tasks. Nature, 388(6640), 381-382. Swingley, D., & Aslin, R. N. (2000). Spoken word recognition and lexical representation in very young children. Cognition, 76(2), 147-166. Swingley, D., & Aslin, R. N. (2002). Lexical neighborhoods and the word-form representations of 14-month-olds. Psychological Science, 13(5), 480-484. Werker, J. F. (1989). Becoming a native listener. American Scientist, 77(1), 55-59. Werker, J. F., Fennel, C. T., Corcoran, K. M., & Stager, C. L. (2002). Infants’ ability to learn phonetically similar words: Effects of age and vocabulary size. Infancy, 3(1), 1-30. Werker, J. F., & Tees, R. C. (1984). Cross-language speech perception: Evidence for perceptual reorganization during the first year of life. Infant Behavior and Development, 7(1), 49-63.
33
34
6 (c) Center for Børnesprog