Changes in and factors related to loneliness in ... - Semantic Scholar

5 downloads 0 Views 189KB Size Report
EDITH J. M. FESKENS, DAAN KROMHOUT. 2. Department for Chronic Diseases ..... Jones DA, Victor CE, Vetter NJ. The problem of loneliness in the elderly in ...
q 1999, British Geriatrics Society

Age and Ageing 1999; 28: 491–495

Changes in and factors related to loneliness in older men. The Zutphen Elderly Study MARJA A. R. TIJHUIS, JENNY DE JONG-GIERVELD1, EDITH J. M. FESKENS, DAAN KROMHOUT2 Department for Chronic Diseases Epidemiology and 2Division of Public Health Research, National Institute of Public Health and the Environment, PO Box 1, 3720 BA Bilthoven, The Netherlands 1 Netherlands Interdisciplinary Demographic Institute, Den Haag, The Netherlands Address correspondence to: M. A. R. Tijhuis. Fax: (+31) 30 2744407. Email: [email protected]

Abstract Aim: to investigate (i) whether loneliness increases in old age, and if so, whether it relates to ageing itself, to time trends or to cohort effects and (ii) the relationship between changes in institutionalization, partner status and health and loneliness. Methods: 939 men born between 1900 and 1920 completed the De Jong-Gierveld Loneliness Scale, and answered questions about their partner status, health and institutionalization in 1985, 1990 and 1995. Results: for the oldest group (born between 1900 and 1910) loneliness scores increased, but not for the younger groups. The increase in loneliness was attributable to ageing. No birth cohort or time effects were found. Loneliness was related to changes in institutionalization, partner status and subjective health but not to limitations in activities of daily living or cognitive function. Conclusions: the increased loneliness experienced by very old men is influenced by loss of a partner, moving into a care home or not feeling healthy. Keywords: activities of daily living, age, cognitive function, institutionalization, loneliness, longitudinal study, marital status, subjective health

Introduction

Methods

Very old people appear to be most prone to loneliness [1–3], perhaps because of loss of close ties and increasing dependency. Results from large-scale surveys give contradictory results on the relationship between loneliness and age [4–13]. Most studies are crosssectional and not designed to study change. Reported changes in loneliness between different age groups were possibly due to cohort effects. In the longitudinal studies, age effects cannot be distinguished from time trends. In the process of loneliness [14, 15], it is not the status of health and social relationships that is important, but a change in these characteristics. Losing a partner, deterioration of health and institutionalization influence loneliness among elderly people [9]. We have analysed to what extent feelings of loneliness are related to age, cohort effects or time trends, and investigated the relationship between loneliness and changes in situational factors like institutionalization, health and partner status [16].

We used data collected in the Zutphen Elderly Study [17]. Zutphen is a small industrial town in the Netherlands with 32 000 inhabitants. In 1985 a randomly selected sample of all men living in Zutphen and born between 1900 and 1920 was asked to participate. The response was 74% (n = 939, Table 1). In 1990, a follow-up survey was conducted. Of the respondents in 1985, 221 had died and 560 participated (78%). In the next follow-up, in 1995, 463 men were invited to participate and 343 (74%) did so. A questionnaire on psychosocial items, including measures of loneliness, partner and health status, was sent to the participants in each study year. Feelings of loneliness were measured by the De Jong-Gierveld Loneliness Scale, developed for use in survey research [18]. The scale meets the criteria of the Rasch measurement model [19] and consists of 11 items describing a discrepancy between the social relations a person has and wants. Both positive (e.g. ‘There are enough people that I feel close to’) and negative

491

M. A. R. Tijhuis et al. Table 1. Results of longitudinal analyses: changes between survey years in loneliness in respondents from the 1985 cohort, and in respondents participating up until 1995 Respondent group ................................................................................

1985 cohort Measure/ survey year

...............................

n

Mean (SD)

Up until 1995 ......................................

n

Mean (SD)

.......................................................................................................

Age (years) 1985

939

72.6 (5.4)

343

70.6 (4.5)

1990

560

76.2 (4.7)

343

75.6 (4.5)

1995

343

80.6 (4.5)

343

80.6 (4.5)

1985

738

2.6 (2.6)

295

2.2 (2.3)

1990

433

2.8 (2.7)

274

2.5 (2.5)

1995

331

2.7 (2.8)

331

2.7 (2.8)

Loneliness

F (P)

0.23 (0.79)

2.32 (0.10)

None of the trends appeared significant when tested with ANOVA F-test.

(e.g. ‘I miss having a really close friend’) items were included. Scale scores were computed by adding the numbers of times a respondent reported loneliness [20], ranging from 0 (absence of loneliness) to 11 (severe loneliness). Reliability was in line with previous studies (Cronbach’s a = 0.81 in 1985, 0.77 in 1990 and 0.78 in 1995) [20]. Year of birth was categorized in three groups: 1900–10, 1911–15 and 1916–20. Men living in a nursing or residential home were considered to be dependent. Men living in their own house or flat were considered to be independent. Partner status was defined as having a partner (whether or not through marriage) or not. The first measure of health status is subjective health [21]. Respondents who felt healthy were scored as 0 (‘healthy’), respondents who felt rather, moderately or not healthy were scored as 1 (‘less healthy’). From 1990, 14 items on activities of daily living were included [22]. A sum score was computed and dichotomized into having no limitations and having one or more limitations. Mental health (in terms of cognitive functioning) was measured by the 30-point Mini-Mental State Examination [23] in a controlled setting by trained staff, in the surveys of 1990 and 1995. We used a score of 25 as cut-off point, considering scores below this point to indicate cognitive impairment [24–26].

Statistics Statistical tests (two-tailed, P < 0.05) were carried out using SAS (version 6.11). Loneliness was considered to be a continuous variable. Both parametric and non-

492

parametric tests were performed because loneliness score distributions were skewed. Since these tests revealed similar results, only parametric test results are shown. Three methods were used to disentangle the effects of ageing, birth cohorts and time [27]: (i) longitudinal method (comparison of loneliness scores between the years of study), (ii) cross-sectional method (comparison of loneliness scores between different age groups within each year of study) and (iii) time-series method (comparison of loneliness scores of similar age groups between the years of study). If both longitudinal and cross-sectional findings are consistent in direction, an effect of ageing is present. Consistent findings in both longitudinal analyses and time series suggest a time trend. Differences in loneliness scores between birth cohorts are present if both cross-sectional analyses and the time series reveal consistent findings. For each method, tests for trend (ANOVA, F-test) were performed. Longitudinal analyses were conducted for all respondents (n = 939) and for survivors up to 1995 (n = 343). Due to missing values in the loneliness measure, the numbers of respondents included in the analyses differ between the study years. Cross-sectional analyses included all respondents in a given survey year. For the time-series approach, differences between 1985, 1990 and 1995 in loneliness for those aged 75–79 or 80–84 were tested. These age groups were chosen to exclude overlap (one respondent being in the same group in two survey years). Age-, cohort- and time-related changes in loneliness were also estimated using a mixed longitudinal model (repeated effects model with compound symmetry covariance structure [28, 29]). To investigate the effects of change in situational factors a similar model was used.

Results Of all respondents in 1995, 20% (68) were born between 1900 and 1910, 31% between 1911 and 1915 and 49% between 1916 and 1920. The largest Pearson correlation coefficient between loneliness scores was found for 1985 and 1990 (R = 0.56). The correlations for other combinations of years ranged from 0.46 to 0.54. Average loneliness scores did not change between 1985 and 1995 (longitudinal analyses, Table 1). However, a non-significant trend in loneliness was observed for respondents participating up until 1995: loneliness scores increased from 2.2, to 2.5 and 2.7. Cross-sectional associations (Table 2) between age and loneliness were observed in 1990 for the respondents participating up until 1995 and in 1995. The oldest men reported more loneliness. Time-series analyses showed no significant trend in change in loneliness score over the survey years for either age group (Table 3).

Changes in loneliness Table 2. Results of cross-sectional analyses: age-related changes in loneliness in each survey year Age group (year of birth) ......................................................................................................................

1916–20

1911–15

.................................

Mean (SD)

n

1900–10

................................

n

Mean (SD)

.................................

n

Mean (SD)

.......................................................................................................................................

Respondents from the 1985 cohort 1985

281

2.7 (2.7)

236

2.4 (2.4)

221

2.9 (2.7)

1990

203

2.6 (2.6)

148

2.7 (2.6)

82

3.2 (3.2)

1995

163

2.5 (2.7)

102

2.4 (2.8)

66

3.5 (2.9)a

Respondents up until 1995 1985

149

2.4 (2.4)

91

2.0 (2.4)

55

2.1 (1.9)

1990

141

2.4 (2.4)

90

2.2 (2.2)

43

3.3 (3.3)a

1995

163

2.5 (2.7)

102

2.4 (2.8)

66

3.5 (2.9)a

a

Significant trend tested with ANOVA F-test (P < 0.05).

Relationships in the cross-sectional analyses were confirmed by the (non-significant) trend found in the longitudinal analyses. Thus, increasing loneliness was due to ageing effects. These results were confirmed by additional repeated measurements analyses. Overall change in loneliness was 0.02 per year (P = 0.06). With each year, loneliness scores increased by 0.04 (P = 0.02). Time and cohort effects were not statistically significant [estimates =¹ 0.02 (P = 0.46) and 0.004 (P = 0.09) respectively]. Similar repeated measurements analyses were performed to assess effects of changes in situational factors. For most men, institutionalization, partner status and cognitive function did not change. Because of small numbers moving into care or changing partner status, we had to combine groups. Results for multivariate models are shown in Table 4. Age at baseline was not related to loneliness in 1995 after adjustment

for change in situational factors. These analyses were not stratified by age because of small numbers, especially in the oldest group. Change in institutionalization, partner and subjective health status was related to loneliness, while change in cognitive function and in limitations in activities of daily living were not. In 1995 men who lived dependently or became dependent reported 1.72 times higher loneliness scores than those who lived independently or became independent. Being without a partner, partner loss, feeling less healthy and subjective health impairment were all related to higher loneliness scores (estimates = 1.83, 1.46, 1.69, 0.94 respectively). Repeated measurements analyses on change in institutionalization, partner and health status were also performed adjusting for loneliness at earlier points in time. These analyses (not shown) gave similar results.

Discussion Table 3. Results of time-series analyses: comparisons of loneliness scores between survey years within similar age groups Survey year Age group (in each survey year)

...............................................................

1985

1990

1995

.......................................................................................................

75–79 years n Mean score (and SD)

128 2.8 (2.6)

211 2.9 (2.8)

216 2.7 (2.7)

80–84 years n Mean score (and SD)

84 2.9 (2.6)

81 3.2 (3.2)

114 2.4 (2.9)

None of the differences between years of study appeared significant when tested with ANOVA F-test.

Loneliness does increase with age, but only in the very old. We found no birth cohort or time trends. Change in partner status, in subjective health and in institutionalization were clearly related to loneliness. Men who were or became dependent, men without a partner or who lost their partner and men who felt less healthy or experienced health impairment reported more loneliness than those who had a partner, lived independently or felt healthy. Changes in cognitive function and in limitations in activities of daily living were not related to loneliness. No longitudinal study has unravelled the relationship between loneliness and age, cohort and time trends. Studies of the relationship between loneliness and age have given contradictory results [5, 7, 8, 11, 12, 30]. Our study and that of Wenger et al. [30] show that age is not related to loneliness after adjustment for

493

M. A. R. Tijhuis et al. Table 4. Results of repeated measurements analyses (regression coefficients): frequency (%) and effects of change in institutionalization, partner status and health on loneliness in 1995 Frequency

Loneliness in 1995

.......................................................................................................

Intercept



1.21

Age in 1985



¹0.01

Change in institutionalization (vs independent at all times/ became independent, 92%) Dependent at all times/became dependent

8%

1.72a

9%

1.83a

21%

1.46a

Less healthy at all times

38%

1.69a

Health impairment

27%

0.94a

4%

0.74

Limitations at all times

36%

¹0.34

Became limited

23%

0.11

Lost limitations

7%

¹0.28

Change in partner status (vs partner at all times/partner found, 70%) No partner at all times Partner loss Change in subjective health status (vs healthy at all times, 31%)

Health improvement Change in limitations in activities of daily living (vs no limitations at all times, 34%)

Key points

Change in cognitive function (vs healthy at all times, 57%) Less healthy at all times

15%

¹0.06

Health impairment

16%

0.75

Health improvement

12%

¹0.21

a

Statistical significance: t-test via repeated measurements analyses (P < 0.05).

situational factors. The greater exposure to risk events is probably more important than age in itself [31]. Our results on relationships between loneliness and partner and health status confirm others’ reports [8, 12, 30, 32]. More loneliness was found in elderly people with disabilities or mobility problems [7]. Mixed results were found with marital status and cognitive function [5, 7, 11, 12, 32]. Longitudinal studies [33–35] report that changes in loneliness are associated with change in partner status, in well-being, in mood and in satisfaction with autonomy and life in general. Loneliness in old age often results in an increase in depression, sleeping problems and disturbed appetite [36], which may lead to institutionalization [37]. Our finding that elderly men living in institutions are more

494

lonely than those living independently may therefore be a matter of selection: the most lonely in the community had moved into care homes. However, institutional residents with contacts with former neighbours reported lower levels of loneliness than institutional residents without such contacts [38]. Moving into a nursing home may also lead to social interaction and increased activity which result in less loneliness [39]. We only studied men. Results on gender differences in loneliness are conflicting [5, 7, 8, 11, 12, 30]. Reasons for possible gender differences are that women may have lower self-esteem (low self-esteem is associated with being lonely). Also, it is culturally more acceptable for women to express their emotional difficulties than it is for men. Further, women consider interpersonal relations more important than men do [40]. Women generally grow older and live without their partner for longer than men. Loneliness in men tends to be associated with an evaluation of the relationship with their partner, whereas in women the association tends to be with an evaluation of their overall network or relationships [41]. Therefore, effects of change in partner status on loneliness might be less for women than for men. We have shown that institutionalization and partner and health status are related to loneliness. Age, however, was only related to loneliness in the very old. Further longitudinal research is needed to confirm our results and help understand the process and determinants of loneliness.

• Loneliness increased over time in very old men but not in the younger old. • Men living in or moving into a nursing home were more lonely than those living independently. • Men without a partner were more lonely than men with a partner. • Men who felt less healthy were more lonely than men who felt healthy.

References 1. Dean LR. Aging and the decline of affect. J Gerontol 1962; 17: 440–6. 2. De Jong-Gierveld J, Kamphuis F, Dykstra PA. Old and lonely? Compr Gerontol 1987; 1: 13–7. 3. World Health Organisation. The Elderly in Eleven Countries. A sociomedical survey. Public Health in Europe 21. Geneva: WHO, 1983. 4. Andersson L. Interdisciplinary study of loneliness—with evaluation of social contacts as a means towards improving competence in old age. Acta Sociol 1982; 25: 75–80. 5. Baum SK. Loneliness in elderly persons: a preliminary study. Psychol Rep 1982; 50: 1317–8. 6. De Jong-Gierveld J. Eenzaamheid: een meersporig onderzoek

Changes in loneliness (Loneliness: a multi-facetted approach). Deventer: Van Loghum Slaterus, 1984.

25. Siu AL. Screening for dementia and investigating its causes. Ann Intern Med 1991; 115: 122–32.

7. Jones DA, Victor CE, Vetter NJ. The problem of loneliness in the elderly in the community: characteristics of those who are lonely and the factors related to loneliness. J R Coll Gen Pract 1985; 35: 136–9.

26. Kalmijn S, Feskens EJM, Launer LJ, Kromhout D. Polyunsaturated fatty acids, antioxidants, and cognitive function in very old men. Am J Epidemiol 1997; 145: 33–41.

8. Mullins LC, Sheppard HL, Andersson L. A study of loneliness among a national sample of Swedish elderly. Compr Gerontol B 1988; 2: 36–43. 9. Perlman D. Loneliness: a life-span, family perspective. In: Milardo RM ed. Families and Social Networks. London: Sage, 1988; 190– 220. 10. Van Tilburg TG. Verkregen en Gewenste Ondersteuning in het Licht van Eenzaamheidservaringen (Received and Desired Support with Regard to Feelings of Loneliness). Dissertation. Amsterdam: Vrije Universiteit, 1988. 11. Page RM, Cole GE. Demographic predictors of self-reported loneliness in adults. Psychol Report 1991; 68: 939–45. 12. Holme´n K, Ericsson K, Andersson L, Winblad B. Loneliness among elderly people living in Stockholm: a population study. J Adv Nursing 1992; 17: 43–51. 13. Tornstam L. Loneliness in marriage. J Soc Personal Relat 1992; 9: 197–217. 14. Marangoni C, Ickes W. Loneliness: a theoretical review with implications for measurement. J Soc Personal Relat 1989; 6: 93–128. 15. De Jong-Gierveld J. Developing and testing a model of loneliness. J Pers Soc Psychol 1987; 53: 119–28. 16. Young JE. Loneliness, depression and cognitive therapy: theory and application. In: Peplau LA, Perlman D eds. Loneliness: a sourcebook of current theory, research and therapy. New York: Wiley Interscience, 1982; 379–405. 17. Feskens EJM, Bloemberg BPM, Pijls LTJ, Kromhout D. A longitudinal study on elderly men: the Zutphen study. In: Schroots JJF ed. Aging, Health and Competence. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science, 1993; 327–33. 18. De Jong-Gierveld J, Kamphuis F. The development of a Rasch-type loneliness scale. Appl Psychol Measurement 1985; 9: 289–99. 19. De Jong-Gierveld J, Van Tilburg T. Het meten van persoonlijke ervaringen en gevoelens in vragenlijstonderzoek (The measurement of personal experiences and feelings in questionnaires). In: De JongGierveld J, Van de Zouwen J eds. De Vragenlijst in het Sociaal Onderzoek (The Questionnaire in Social Research). Deventer: Van Loghum Slaterus, 1987; 67–83. 20. Van Tilburg T, De Leeuw E. Stability of scale quality under various data collection procedure: a mode comparison on the ‘De JongGierveld Loneliness Scale’. Int J Publ Opin Res 1991; 3: 69–85. 21. Pijls LTJ, Feskens EJM, Kromhout D. Self-rated health, mortality, and chronic diseases in elderly men. The Zutphen Study, 1985–1990. Am J Epidemiol 1993; 138: 840–8. 22. Hoeymans N, Feskens EJM, Van den Bos GAM, Kromhout D. Measuring functional status: cross-sectional and longitudinal associations between performance and self-report (Zutphen Elderly Study 1990–1993). J Clin Epidemiol 1996; 49: 1103–10. 23. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. ‘Mini-Mental State’. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiat Res 1975; 12: 189–98. 24. Fillenbaum GG, George LK, Blazer DG. Scoring nonresponse on the Mini-Mental State Examination. Psychol Med 1988; 18: 1021–5.

27. Elahi VK, Elahi D, Andres R, Tobin JD, Butler MG, Norris AH. A longitudinal study of nutritional intake in men. J Gerontol 1983; 38: 162–80. 28. Anderssen N, Jacobs R, Sidney S et al. Change and secular trends in physical activity patterns in young adults: a seven-year longitudinal follow-up in the Coronary Artery Risk Development in Young Adults Study (CARDIA). Am J Epidemiol 1996; 143–4: 351–62. 29. Weijenberg MP, Feskens EJM, Kromhout D. Age related changes in total and high density lipoprotein cholesterol in elderly men. Am J Public Health 1996; 86: 798–803. 30. Wenger GC, Davies R, Shahtahmasebi S, Scott A. Social isolation and loneliness in old age: review and model refinement. Ageing Soc 1996; 16: 333–58. 31. De Jong-Gierveld J, Van Tilburg T. Social relationships, integration, and loneliness. In: Knipscheer CPM, De Jong-Gierveld J, Van Tilburg TG, Dykstra PA, eds. Living Arrangements and Social Networks of Older Adults (pp. 59–82). Amsterdam: VU University Press, 1995; 155–72. 32. Stessman J, Ginsberg G, Klein M, Hammerman-Rozenberg R, Friedman R, Cohen A. Determinants of loneliness in Jerusalem’s 70year-old population. Israel J Med Sci 1996: 32: 639–48. 33. De Jong-Gierveld J, Dykstra P. Changes in loneliness: assessing the impact of changes in the social network and changes in health. Working paper, prepared for the symposium on Dynamics in Personal Relationships of the Elderly: changes in the household, the family, the network and loneliness at the 7th International Conference on Personal Relationships, Groningen, 1994. 34. Downey KI. An investigation of loneliness in adolescents: prevalence, stability and correlates. Diss Abstracts Int 1985; 45–7: 2306B. ˚ kerlind I, Ho 35. A ¨ rnquist JO. Stability and change in feelings of loneliness: a two-year prospective longitudinal study of advanced alcohol abusers. Scand J Psychol 1989; 30: 102–12. 36. Weidinger B. Einsamkeit und ihre Auswirkung auf das subjektive Krankheitsempfinden bei u ¨ ber 60 ja¨hrigen Patienten in der Allgemeinarztpraxis. (Loneliness and its consequences for subjective health in general practice patients of 60 years and over). Mu ¨ nchen: Medizinischen Poliklinik der Universita¨t Mu ¨ nchen, 1992. 37. Mor-Barak ME, Miller LS. A longitudinal study of the causal relationship between social networks and health of the poor frail elderly. J Appl Gerontol 1991; 10: 293–310. 38. Bondevik M, Skogstad A. Loneliness among the oldest old: a comparison between residents living in nursing homes and residents living in the community. Int J Aging Hum Dev 1996; 43: 181–97. 39. Rokach A. The subjectivity of loneliness and coping with it. Psychol Rep 1996; 79: 475–81. 40. Borys S, Perlman D. Gender differences in loneliness. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 1985; 11–1: 63–74. 41. De Jong-Gierveld J. Husbands, lovers, and loneliness. In: Lewis RA, Salt RE eds. Men in Families. New York: Sage, 1986: 115–25.

Received 3 December 1997; accepted in revised form 7 October 1998

495

Suggest Documents