Oct 20, 2016 - through interaction with other individuals in a social network. The adoption of ... shifts an individ-. arXiv:1509.01502v2 [physics.soc-ph] 20 Oct 2016 ... individuals decide whether to accept new beliefs transmitted by their peers.
Collective dynamics of belief evolution under cognitive coherence and social conformity Nathaniel Rodriguez, Johan Bollen, and Yong-Yeol Ahn
arXiv:1509.01502v1 [physics.soc-ph] 4 Sep 2015
The Center for Complex Networks and Systems Research, Informatics Department, Indiana University Human history has been marked by social instability and conflict, often driven by the irreconcilability of opposing sets of beliefs, ideologies, and religious dogmas. The dynamics of belief systems has been studied mainly from two distinct perspectives, namely how cognitive biases lead to individual belief rigidity and how social influence leads to social conformity. Here we propose a unifying framework that connects cognitive and social forces together in order to study the dynamics of societal belief evolution. Each individual is endowed with a network of interacting beliefs that evolves through interaction with other individuals in a social network. The adoption of beliefs is affected by both internal coherence and social conformity. Our framework explains how social instabilities can arise in otherwise homogeneous populations, how small numbers of zealots with highly coherent beliefs can overturn societal consensus, and how belief rigidity protects fringe groups and cults against invasion from mainstream beliefs, allowing them to persist and even thrive in larger societies. Our results suggest that strong consensus may be insufficient to guarantee social stability, that the cognitive coherence of belief-systems is vital in determining their ability to spread, and that coherent belief-systems may pose a serious problem for resolving social polarization, due to their ability to prevent consensus even under high levels of social exposure. We therefore argue that the inclusion of cognitive factors into a social model is crucial in providing a more complete picture of collective human dynamics.
Ideological conflict has been a major challenge for human societies [1]. For instance, when post World-War I Germany was marked by economic depression and social trauma, ideological fringe groups like the NationalSocialist Party and the Communist Party of Germany made their way into mainstream politics to eventually dominate the political landscape [2–4]. This process of ideological upheaval eventually led to World War II, one of the deadliest conflicts in human history. Similarly, 14th and 15th century Europe was torn by sharp religious transitions and accompanying widespread conflicts, such as the Thirty Years’ War [5]. The abundance of such ideological transitions in history raises questions: are they driven by common psycho-social mechanisms? Do specific peculiarities of human psychology play a role in ideological dynamics? Although these questions have been tackled by numerous studies, most existing models of belief system dynamics focus on either social or cognitive factors rather than integrating both aspects. Social models focus on how social interactions transmit and shape beliefs. For instance, Axelrod’s cultural dissemination model considers social influence and homophily as key drivers of cultural polarization (see Fig. 1(a)) [6]. In this model each agent is represented as a vector of independent traits that can be modified through social influence. Axelrod’s model and the study of spin systems in physics have inspired a number of opinion models, such as the voter model and Sznajd model [7–9]. Cognitive modeling approaches mainly focus on information processing and decision making (see Fig. 1(b)) [10]. Psychological research has revealed that individuals strive for internal consistency, which leads
to cognitive mechanisms such as confirmation bias [11] and cognitive dissonance [12]. The existence of these cognitive forces and biases in belief formation questions the underlying assumptions—a set of independent beliefs [13, 14]—of spin models or opinion vector-based models [15, 16]. Recent attempts to combine these forces have used mechanisms like social- and anti-conformity [9], foundational beliefs [17], confirmation bias [18], and social network formation [19–21]. However, modeling beliefsystems that consist of interacting beliefs, and the study of such systems under social influence, is yet to be studied. Here we introduce a novel framework that can incorporate both social and cognitive factors in a coherent way (see Fig. 1(c)). We show that the integration of social and cognitive factors produces elementary features of collective social phenomena—societal transitions, upheavals, and existence of fringe groups. Our framework represents a society as a network of individuals—a social network—where each individual possesses a network of concepts and beliefs. Social influence takes place via the social ties [22–25] and each individual carries a belief network of interconnected “predicates” that represents the individual’s belief system [12, 16, 26–30]. We evaluate the internal coherence of each individual’s belief network by applying the balance theory [31, 32]. The coherence shifts an individual’s willingness to integrate new beliefs, thus simulating cognitive traits such as confirmation bias and cognitive dissonance [11, 15, 23–25, 33]. At the same time, an individual’s belief network is affected by social influence; repeated exposure to new ideas through social ties [34– 37] increases their likelihood of adopting even incongru-
2
FIG. 1. (a) Social models, such as the voter or Sznajd models, focus on the assimilation process through social pressure. Beliefs are usually simplified as independent states. (b) Cognitive models, such as the SKS model, focus on the interaction and coherence of beliefs of a single individual and how individuals make decisions and change their minds. The effect of social networks is often unaddressed. (c) Our model incorporates both forces, recognizing not only social pressures but also the connected nature of human beliefs. The social network acts as a conduit for belief transmission between individuals. We model a belief as a signed relationship between two concepts. We express the internal coherence of a network of such beliefs in terms of social balance theory where relationship triads can be either stable or unstable. The belief networks evolve over time as individuals decide whether to accept new beliefs transmitted by their peers.
ent ideas. Individuals thus experience both cognitive and social forces: (i) they prefer to have coherent belief networks and prefer beliefs that will increase their internal coherence; but (ii) may accept conflicting beliefs under the influence of strong social pressure. Our approach considers a network of concepts and beliefs where the nodes represent concepts and signed edges between them represents binary associative beliefs that capture the relation between two concepts (cf. Social Knowledge Structure (SKS) model [26]). This formulation allows us to define internal coherence through the principle of triad stability in social balance theory [38– 41]. For instance, Simon is a devoted member of the Grey Party. Herbert is also a member of the Grey Party, but has been charged with corruption. There is a positive link between Herbert and the Grey Party, and between Herbert and corruption, while there is a negative link between Grey Party and corruption. Such pressured configurations are considered unstable (incoherent), and are analogous to frustrated states in spin-systems or unstable social triads. To resolve the frustration Simon may dissociate Herbert from the allegations of corruption, drop
Herbert’s association with the party, or change the relationship between the Grey Party and corruption. It has been shown that people tend to quickly resolve such inconsistency when provided the opportunity to choose dissonance reduction strategies [42]. Yet, in the presence of social pressure or more complicated concept associations, a concept may remain pressured. Each triad in a belief network can be either stable or unstable, as shown in Fig. 1(c). The incoherence of an entire belief system can thus be captured by an energy function [43]: (i) Em =−
1 X ajk akl ajl , N∆
(1)
j,k,l
where N∆ is the total number of triads in the belief network of individual m, ajk is the association connecting nodes j and k of the belief network, which can be +1 (positive association) or −1 (negative association). The sum is taken over all triads in the network. A single association’s contribution to the energy depends on the state of adjacent associations, providing interdependence and rigidity to the belief system. Beliefs
3 do not necessarily reflect reality. They may be fabricated or completely false. It is, however, the interaction between beliefs that gives them their strength, reflective of psychological factors like confirmation bias and cognitive dissonance. The evolution of belief systems is also driven by social interactions, through which people communicate their beliefs to others. We represent such societies as social networks whose nodes are individuals and edges represent social relationships through which ideas are communicated. We define a second, “social” energy term, inspired by energy in the spin-based models which capture the degree of alignment between connected individuals. The ‘local’ energy that an individual m feels can be defined by: (s) Em =−
X q∈Γ(m)
~m · S ~q , S
(2)
where the sum is taken over the set of m’s neighbors, ~ is a belief state vector where each denoted by Γ(m). S element corresponds to an edge in the belief network. ~ = N (N −1)/2, where N is the number of nodes in the |S| belief graph. For our simulations N is the same for each person and everyone possesses the same set of concepts (nodes). We combine the internal energy with the social energy for all individuals to define the total energy (Hamiltonian) as follows: H=−
i X h (i) (s) JEm + IEm
(3)
m∈M
individual has an identical set of concept nodes. Figure 1(c) shows the selection and emission process on a graph. The receiver accepts the association if it makes its belief system more coherent by decreasing the energy (∆E < 0). Even if ∆E > 0 (incoherent), the receiver may still accept on social grounds with the probability −∆E of e T . This term is analogous to the Boltzmann factor [44]. T , which we refer to as susceptibility, serves a similar purpose as temperature in physical systems for the belief network. As T increases, an individual is more likely to accept their neighbor’s opinions that conflict with their own. We characterize the status of the whole society by defining two global energy functions. First, the mean individual energy hE (i) i measures the average internal coherence of individuals. It is expressed by the following equation: 1 X (i) Em . (4) hE (i) i = |M | m∈M
The average is taken over the energies of all individuals and it can take values between +1 and −1. hE (i) i = −1 means that every individual in the society possesses a completely coherent belief system, with no pressured beliefs. The other extreme (+1) represents a society where every individual has completely incoherent beliefs. Yet, this measure does not give us any indication of how homogeneous a society is, as belief systems can vary widely while still being coherent. We have a second energy measure inspired from spin systems: X (s) E (s) = Em , (5) m∈M
where M is set of people in the social network and the last sum is taken over all links on the network. The parameters J and I, which we refer to as the coherentism and peer-influence respectively, control the relative contribution of the internal energy and the social energy to the total. The dynamics is dominated by internal belief coherence if J I and by social consensus when I J. Each individual is endowed with their own internal belief network and may transfer some of their beliefs to their social contacts. A receiver of a belief either accepts the incoming belief or not based on the context of their own belief system (internal coherency) and similarity to their neighbors (social conformity). A belief is more likely to be accepted if it increases the coherence of an individual’s own belief system, social pressure will also increase the odds of a belief being accepted, even if it conflicts with their belief system. We implement these ideas by creating the following rules: at each time step t, a random pair of connected individuals is chosen and one of the individuals (sender) randomly chooses a belief (association) from its internal belief system and sends it to the other individual (receiver), as illustrated in Fig. 1(c). We assume that each
which is minimized if the society is in consensus. Most opinion models exhibit a phase transition from a disordered state to an ordered one [22], where the ordered state represents consensus. As our model includes the two conflicting forces—personal belief rigidity and social influence—we first ask how the relative strength of these two forces governs consensus dynamics. We assign each individual a random belief system and let the society evolve. Through social interaction, the society may or may not reach a consensus, depending upon the relative strength of peer-influence (I) and coherentism (J). Figure 2 shows a set of phase diagrams for various combinations of J, I, and T . Since belief systems evolve locally, individuals may have to pass through incoherent states before fully converting. As the coherentism (J) increases individuals tend to cling to their own beliefs rather than make such incoherent transitions. This can prevent consensus, but small local consensus can still occur. As the susceptibility (T ) increases individuals readily accept incoherent beliefs, allowing individuals to traverse through the belief space. It facilitates spreading of ideas and consensus. By making individuals more susceptible to belief spreading rather than to random switching,
4 T plays the opposite role to temperature in standard spin models [22]. T acts as a temperature for individual’s beliefs, and as an inverse-temperature for the whole system. As the peer-influence (I) increases individuals again become more prone to consensus. Novel dynamics occur when the effect of coherentism and social influence become comparable. Figure 2(b) shows that complete consensus is not guaranteed when the forces exerted by J and I remain comparable in size. The competition leads to a situation where belief systems can coexist and where less coherent systems can dominate. In our model the energy contribution of belief networks can have a major impact on system stability as individuals seek more coherent beliefs and resolve dissonance. What happens when key beliefs are upset through an external shock or perturbation? Imagine two independent systems, both homogeneous with the same social energy, E (s) . In traditional social models, with the absence of the individual belief system (cognitive factor), these systems are identical. By contrast, the internal system of interconnected beliefs introduces a new force that drives people to seek coherence in the structure of their own belief systems. Given a homogeneous population of people with highly coherent belief systems, society remains stable. However, given a homogeneous population of low coherent belief systems, society will become unstable and spontaneously break down (see Fig. 3). In our simulation, the society is initialized with a near homogeneous state where 99% of the population is in consensus, but with conflicting beliefs. Individuals are driven to reduce the energy of their own belief systems and leave consensus. This society eventually re-converges at a more coherent belief-system that is completely different from the original consensus after undergoing catastrophic changes (Fig. 3(c)). Consensus in a society cannot guarantee the future stability of society. Such collapses may explain mass movements, revolutions, major shifts in religious or party identification, or schisms within political parties or religions. For instance, traumatic events such as war or depression, can shift some of the most fundamental beliefs that people are holding. Such changes may increase the internal conflicts of individuals, which in turn may make the whole society more prone to catastrophic collapse or paradigm shifts. Throughout history, major external perturbations in the form of war, depression, and crippling inflation has frequently disrupted the world-views of a society’s citizens, inducing subsequent social upheavals. More coherent belief systems, which otherwise fail to gain adherents in the presence of a dominant stable societal values, can gain the upper-hand by recruiting among a population of disturbed citizens as Hoffer suggests [2]. Given that the coherence of personal beliefs fundamentally impacts collective behavior, we investigate the impact of “true believers” or zealots in a population. In the context of our framework we define zealots as indi-
viduals who will never alter their own belief systems, but will continue to attempt to convert others to their own. Zealots can play an important role in shaping collective social dynamics [2, 22, 45]. To study the impact of zealots, we prepare a homogeneous society with highly coherent beliefs and introduce zealots with varying internal coherence. Figure 4 shows that the internal coherence of zealots has a strong impact on their effectiveness in converting society. Low coherence zealots require much higher densities in order to convert the whole population (see squares in Fig. 4) because converted individuals revert back to more coherent belief-systems at a higher rate, making it difficult for the zealot’s belief-system to retain converts. Highly coherent zealots pull the whole population out of consensus and convert it to their belief-systems more easily. Coherent zealots require almost half the density, in this particular case, to convert the entire population. This suggests that coherence of a set of beliefs plays a vital role in determining how well the set of beliefs spread through society. Our model may also offer a framework for explaining and studying the co-existence of many seemingly invalid or impractical cults and fringe groups in our society. The beliefs of cults and other fringe groups may frequently contradict reality, yet they continue to thrive in spite of being surrounded by large majorities of other belief systems. In our model, this is explained by the degree to which the coherence of belief systems can out-balance social pressure in addition to social isolation. To investigate the influence of belief rigidity and social isolation on social dynamics we create a society with two communities, a large mainstream community and a smaller cult community (Fig. 5(a)). The mainstream community acts like a reservoir of mainstream beliefs. The other community consists of people following a different coherent belief system. By controlling social exposure and the strength of belief coherence, we investigate what effect belief coherence has on the capacity for mainstream society to invade the cult. The parameter µ controls the fraction of edges in the cult community that are shared with the mainstream community [46]. When socially isolated (low µ) the cult can resist invasion regardless of mainstream coherence (Fig. 5(b)). Groups of like-minded individuals can resist outside influence by reducing social contact with non-members (decreasing µ) and enhancing internal social interactions, both of which are common in many fringe groups and even in religious communities. On the other hand, the internal coherence also plays a key role. Less coherent mainstream beliefs have greater difficulty in converting the cult, even at high levels of exposure (high µ). Compared with the dogma of cults, the truth or the reality can be more complex and less coherent. In such case, even a belief-system firmly grounded on truth may struggle to convert cults that possess a highly coherent set of beliefs. Interlocked beliefs turns tightly-knit communities into bastions of resistance
5
FIG. 2. (a-c) Phase diagrams of various combinations of the three parameters J, I, and T . Along with corresponding slices through the phase space (bottom row). (a,d) peer-influence I and susceptibility T conflict creating a regime where multiple belief systems with various coherences can coexist. We see a similar regime appear in (b, e) where peer-influence and coherentism contend for dominance. More traditional disorder-to-order transitions as in other opinion models also take place when I is small and fixed (c, f).
to ideological invasion from outside. Figure 6(a) shows a phase diagram of the case when I J, where conversion is determined solely by social exposure (µ). This is expected by most spin-based social models. However, if the strength of social influence is comparable to belief strength, social exposure is not the sole determinant anymore (see Fig. 6(b)). Notably, by increasing the coherentism (J) of its members and maintaining a coherent set of beliefs, cults can continue to thrive even with complete mixing with society. This contradicts, while underlining common intuition, the traditional exposure models where enough exposure is sufficient to convert populations. These results also hint at common characteristics of surviving cults. We do not expect to find successful cults that have low belief coherence and high mixing because they would be quickly converted by the mainstream society. We expect to see more cults that utilize a combinations of policies that minimize their member’s social contacts with outsiders, emphasize the importance of their dogma (increasing J), and maximizing the coherence of their beliefs. The latter could mean incorporating explanations for beliefs that contradict empirical evidence or belittling mainstream methods of reasoning. Coherence can be, but is not nec-
essarily aligned with logical consistency, rather coherence is based on the strength of associations between concepts and valence concepts (see SKS model [26]), that is derived from a connectionist cognitive framework. We have shown that our model exhibits a disorder-toorder transition similar to other opinion models, while also exhibiting unique dynamics that explain common processes observed in the real world, such as the spontaneous breakdown of a homogeneous society driven by shocks to individuals’ beliefs, the dependence of zealotry on belief coherence, and the successful entrenchment of fringe groups. Each of these results arises from the fact that our framework integrates belief interdependence with social influence. The breakdown of homogeneous societies can manifest through changes in the connectivity of concepts at the concept-network level (cf. SKS model). Abrupt shocks to these conceptual connections can impact the coherence of beliefs. That impact is reflected in our model as an unstable belief system. When people share a common shock, one could expect that many of those people’s belief-systems will enter a frustrated state. In order for this socially frustrated state to be resolved, members of the population partake in societal-dissonance resolu-
5 ×10 4
1.0
3
0.8
3 2
hSz /N i
(s)
(a)
E (s) − Emin
6
0.6
1 0
0.4
(b) 1.0 0.5
Ez = −1.0 (coherent) Ez = 0.0 Ez = 1.0 (incoherent)
hE (i)i
0.2
0.0
0.0 0.00
−0.5 −1.0
(c) 1.0
hSg /N i
0.8 0.6
So (E = 1)
0.4
Sf (E = −1)
0.2 0.0 0
50
100
150
200
t FIG. 3. Spontaneous social transition. Strong societal consensus does not guarantee a stable society in our model. If major paradigm shifts, such as wars or economic crisis, occur and make individual belief systems incoherent, a society may undergo a catastrophic transition. (a) The plot shows the evolution of social energy E (s) over time. The system starts in a homogeneous state where 99% of the population is in a consensus with highly coherent beliefs. We then introduce a shock by flipping beliefs, making the dominant belief system incoherent. Individuals leave consensus, searching for more coherent sets of beliefs, until society re-converges at a stable configuration. The social energy of the system, as coherence of beliefs plays a critical role. (b) Decreasing mean individual energies hE (i) i over time illustrates individual stabilization during societal transition. (c) As society is upset, the original dominant belief system So (solid black) is replaced by an emerging alternative Sf (dashed red).
tion strategies by collectively transitioning toward more coherent belief systems. This lets us explain paradigm shifts in culture and thinking as the result of societalwide coping strategies. Our model is a minimalistic approach toward introducing individualist concept-networks into a systematic social dynamics model. Though simple, the framework can be expanded to include more realistic features. For instance, communicated beliefs are chosen randomly, but this feature can be replaced with other models of belief communication, such as imitation, where someone will be more likely to communicate recently accepted beliefs. Additionally, our mapping from the SKS concept model could be expanded to included weighted relation-
0.02
0.04
0.06
0.08
ρo
0.10
0.12
0.14
FIG. 4. Impact of the internal consistency of zealot beliefs. We define zealots as a group of individuals who share an identical, immutable belief system. Such belief systems can, however, vary in terms of their coherency. The dynamics of hSz /N i over ρo , the density of zealots introduced into the population, reveals that zealots with more coherent beliefs can convert a population much more efficiently. In converting the whole population, the coherent set of beliefs (circles) require only less than half the density of zealots compared with incoherent beliefs (squares). Bars show standard error.
ships that emphasize the uncertainty that an individual has in their beliefs (conviction) and its relations to other beliefs. While we used the same parameter values for all individuals, we expect them to vary within the population from person to person. Variations of this value could produce the natural occurrence of zealot-like behavior. Finally, the formation and maintenance of social ties is an important facet of social dynamics [47, 48] and could be implemented by allowing agents to choose their neighbors [49]. Our model expands upon previous social models by implementing an internal belief system that assumes that the interdependence among beliefs shapes their dynamics. In this framework we are able to incorporate known psychological forces into the model and show that a bottom-up approach can successfully link micro-level behaviors to global dynamics. By making this small jump to using internal belief-systems, while preserving key features of standard social models, such as percolation and global consensus, extends their dynamics and explanatory value significantly, which in-turn allows us to explain the co-existence and entrenchment of belief systems and the catastrophic dynamics of social upheaval. Future work will be directed towards the development of new opinion models for specific applications that more closely integrate our behavioral and cognitive understanding of humanity. The authors would like to thank Alessandro Flamini, Rob Goldstone, and Fabio Rojas for valuable feedback
7
FIG. 5. (a) The survival of cults and fringe groups depends on the coherence and strength of beliefs. We create a network with two communities with parameters T = 2.0, I = 0.001, and J = 2.0—putting the system in a regime where it will seek consensus. We vary the fraction of links that connect P the cult community to the mainstream community, denoted µ. eo is the number of social links between communities and ki is the total number of links in the cult (both shared and internal). The mainstream community attempts to convert the smaller cult. (b) At low µ the lack of exposure allows the cult to resist mainstream conversion. At higher µ there is sufficient exposure to the mainstream community to overcome the rigidity of the cult’s belief system. However, the process of conversion becomes more difficult as the cult’s beliefs become more coherent than mainstream beliefs. Cults are easily converted with highly coherent mainstream beliefs even at low exposure levels (black circles), while cults maintain their beliefs even at high exposure given low coherence of mainstream beliefs (red squares). Bars show standard deviation.
FIG. 6. (a) Exposure determines conversion resistance when peer-influence (I) is strong. (b) Fringe groups can sustain their beliefs, even at a very high level of social exposure, with high levels of individual coherentism (J).
and discussions.
[1] T. Sowell, A conflict of visions : ideological origins of political struggles (Basic Books, New York, 2002), ISBN 0465081428. [2] E. Hoffer, The true believer : thoughts on the nature of mass movements, Perennial library (New York : Harper and Row, 1966, c1951., 1966). [3] T. Rohkramer, A single communal faith? : the German Right from Conservatism to National Socialism (Berghahn Books, New York, 2007), ISBN 1845453689. [4] F. McDonough, Hitler and the Rise of the Nazi Party (Pearson, Harlow, England, 2012), ISBN 140826921X. [5] E. Britannica, history of europe (2014). [6] R. Axelrod, The Journal of Conflict Resolution 42(2), 203 (2009). [7] K. Sznajd-Weron and J. Sznajd, International Journal of Modern Physics C 11, 1157 (2000), ISSN 01291831. [8] P. Clifford and A. Sudbury, Biometrika 60, 581 (1973),
ISSN 00063444. [9] M. A. Javarone, Physica A: Statistical Mechanics and its Applications p. 19 (2014), ISSN 0378-4371. [10] A. Allahverdyan and A. Galstyan, PLoS ONE 9, 1 (2014), ISSN 19326203. [11] R. Nickerson, Review of General Psychology 2, 175 (1998), ISSN 1089-2680. [12] L. Festinger, A theory of cognitive dissonance (1962). [13] R. Goldstone and M. Janssen, Trends in cognitive sciences 9, 424 (2005), ISSN 13646613. [14] W. A. Mason, F. R. Conrey, and E. R. Smith, Personality and social psychology review 11, 279 (2007), ISSN 10888683(not verified). [15] C. McGarty and J. Turner, British Journal of Social Psychology 31, 253268 (1992). [16] R. Pechey and P. Halligan, Journal of Cognition and Culture 12, 81 (2012), ISSN 15677095. [17] N. Masuda and S. Redner, Journal of Statistical Mechanics-Theory and Experiment (2011), ISSN 17425468. [18] R. Nishi and N. Masuda, PHYSICAL REVIEW E 87 (2013), ISSN 15393755.
8 [19] M. Javarone and G. Armano, JOURNAL OF PHYSICS A-MATHEMATICAL AND THEORETICAL 46 (2013), ISSN 17518113. [20] V. Sekara, A. Stopczynski, and S. Lehmann, arXiv:1506.04704 [physics.soc-ph] (2015). [21] F. Shi, P. Mucha, and R. Durrett, Physical Review E - Statistical, Nonlinear, and Soft Matter Physics 88 (2013), ISSN 15393755. [22] C. Castellano, S. Fortunato, and V. Loreto, Reviews Of Modern Physics 81(2), 591 (2009). [23] C. Lord, L. Ross, and M. Lepper, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 37, 2098 (1979), ISSN 0022-3514. [24] C. Taber and M. Lodge, American Journal of Political Science 50, 755 (2006), ISSN 00925853. [25] E. Evans, Cognitive Psychology 42, 217 (2001), ISSN 00100285. [26] A. Greenwald, M. Banaji, L. Rudman, S. Farnham, B. Nosek, and D. Mellott, Psychological Review 109, 3 (2002), ISSN 0033-295X. [27] M. Schilling, Creativity Research Journal 17.2/3, 131 (2005). [28] W. Quine and J. Ullian, The web of belief (New York, Random House, 1970). [29] P. Thagard, Coherence in thought and action, Life and mind (Cambridge, Mass. : MIT Press, 2000), ISBN 0262201313. [30] R. Pechey and P. Halligan, PLoS ONE 7, 1 (2012), ISSN 19326203. [31] F. Heider, Journal of Psychology 21, 107 (1946). [32] H. Norman and D. Patrick, Social Networks 25, 17 (2003), ISSN 0378-8733. [33] A. Allahverdyan and A. Galstyan, PLoS ONE 9 (2014). [34] S. Asch, Effects of group pressure on the modification and
[35] [36] [37] [38] [39] [40] [41] [42] [43] [44] [45]
[46] [47] [48]
[49]
distortion of judgments. In Groups, leadership and men (1951). S. Asch, Psychological Monographs 70(9), 170 (1956). D. Centola, Science 5996, 1194 (2010). D. Matz and W. Wood, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 88, 22 (2005), ISSN 0022-3514. F. Heider, The Journal of Psychology 21, 107 (1946). T. Antal, P. Krapivsky, , and S. Redner, Physica D (2006). P. Doreian, Social Networks 24, 93 (2002), ISSN 03788733. D. Cartwright and F. Harary, Psychological Review 63, 277 (1956), ISSN 0033-295X. A. Elliot and P. Devine, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 67, 382 (1994), ISSN 0022-3514. S. Marvel, S. Strogatz, and J. Kleinberg, Physcial Review Letters 103 (2009), ISSN 00319007. S. Daniel, in Introduction to Thermal Physics, edited by S. Iwata (Addison Wesley Longman, 2000). M. Mobilia, A. Petersen, and S. Redner, Journal of Statistical Mechanics-Theory and Experiment (2007), ISSN 17425468. A. Lancichinetti and S. Fortunato, Physical Review E 80 (2008), ISSN 15393755. D. Lazer, Journal of Mathematical Sociology 25, 69 (2001), ISSN 0022250X. M. Rivera, S. Soderstrom, and B. Uzzi, Dynamics of dyads in social networks: Assortative, relational, and proximity mechanisms., vol. 36 of Annual Review of Sociology (2010), ISBN 03600572. M. McPherson, L. Smith-Lovin, and J. Cook, Annual Review of Sociology p. 415 (2001), ISSN 03600572.