Document not found! Please try again

control of optomotor responses by descending deviation detector ...

2 downloads 495 Views 2MB Size Report
Summary. Neural control of optomotor responses in Locusta migratoria was studied using a newly developed preparation of intact, tethered, flying locusts.
J. exp. Biol. 149, 191-205 (1990) Printed in Great Britain © The Company of Biologists Limited 1990

191

CONTROL OF OPTOMOTOR RESPONSES BY DESCENDING DEVIATION DETECTOR NEURONES IN INTACT FLYING LOCUSTS BY K. HENSLER AND C. H. F. ROWELL Zoological Institute, University of Basel, Rheinsprung 9, 4051 Basel, Switzerland Accepted 23 October 1989 Summary Neural control of optomotor responses in Locusta migratoria was studied using a newly developed preparation of intact, tethered,flyinglocusts. The preparation could perform normal flight behaviour and head movements while neurones in the neck connectives were recorded and stimulated intracellularly. Course deviations simulated by an artificial horizon caused optomotor reactions, e.g. steering by the wings (monitored as steering reactions in M97 and M127, first basalar muscles of fore- and hindwings, respectively) and compensatory head movements. Intracellular recordings were made from two identified descending deviation detector neurones, PI(2)5 and DNC. Both neurones coded direction specifically for course deviations. Electrical activation of either neurone in tethered flight at frequencies of up to 230 Hz elicited steering in M97 and M127 and head rolling with latencies of less than 20 ms. These reactions were of the same quality and strength as compensatory head rolling and steering in M97 and M127 following horizon rolling of about 40°. This demonstrates directly a role of PI(2)5 and DNC in course control. Introduction Flying locusts detect unintended course deviations by means of exteroreceptors of the head (eyes, ocelli, wind-sensitive hairs), and counteract the deviations by optomotor reactions which include corrective steering movements by wings, legs and abdomen, and compensatory head movements (e.g. Mohl and Zarnack, 1977; Zarnack and Mohl, 1977; Taylor, 1981a,6; Thuring, 1986; Arbas, 1986; Baader, 1988; reviewed by Rowell, 1988; Mohl, 1989; Hensler, 1989). The sensory information about course deviations is coded and transmitted to the motor centres by a population of descending deviation detector neurones (DNs) which project from the brain to the fused abdominal ganglia. Approximately 15 pairs of DNs have been identified physiologically; six of them have also been identified morphologically. DNs are uni- or multimodal, and respond best to course deviations of different directions (Bacon and Tyrer, 1978; Mohl and Bacon, 1983; Rowell and Reichert, 1986; Griss and Rowell, 1986; Hensler, 1988a, 1989; fcvey words: locust,flightsteering, intracellular stimulation, compensatory head movement.

192

K. H E N S L E R A N D C. H . F . R O W E L L

Baader, 1989; Rowell, 1989); a subpopulation additionally codes for movements and/or position of the head (Hensler, 1988a; K. Hensler, in preparation). For three ocellar DNs, the major pathway to wing motoneurones (MNs) has been demonstrated (Reichert and Rowell, 1985, 1986). It involves a class of thoracic interneurones (TINs) which receive direct input from DNs and synapse onto various subsets of wing motoneurones (MNs). These data strongly suggest that DNs are involved in course control. However, the connectivity between DNs, TINs and wing MNs has been unravelled in dissected locusts, either at rest or during fictive flight (rhythmic activation of wing MNs in locusts with denervated thoracic ganglia, with a basic pattern similar to that in intact locusts). We know that proprioceptive feedback, as it occurs during normal behaviour, is important in generating the motor output (Wolf and Pearson, 1988), and that reflexes operating in quiescent animals may be changed, or inverted, during active movements (e.g. Skorupski and Sillar, 1986; Bassler, 1988). Hence, the behavioural relevance and functioning during normal behaviour of the pathway from DNs to wing MNs is not proven, and also there is no indication of the importance of single DNs within the entire population. Both questions can only be solved by individual stimulation of DNs in intact flying locusts. Stimulation during flight behaviour has been achieved for the wind-sensitive TCG (tritocerebral commissure giant) neurone, which indeed elicits steering by wing muscles when activated electrically (Mohl and Bacon, 1983). However, TCG is anatomically a special case: it is easily stimulated individually with an extracellular electrode mounted at the tritocerebral commissure, whereas individual stimulation of all other DNs requires penetration with intracellular electrodes. In a first attempt we tried to reveal effects of single DNs on wing MNs during fictive flight in the type of highly dissected preparations normally used for intracellular recordings from thoracic neurones (Robertson and Pearson, 1982; Rowell and Reichert, 1986; Hensler, 1988a). Neither the stimulation of single DNs nor the correlation of DN activity with muscle activity provided convincing results. The main reason is that steering is expressed as shifts in the relative timing of action potentials in the different wing muscles (Mohl and Zarnack, 1977; Zarnack and Mohl, 1977; Taylor, 1981ft; Thiiring, 1986; Schmidt and Zarnack, 1987; Hensler and Robert, 1990). Generally, these shifts range from 0.5 to 5ms, but during fictive flight the standard deviation of normal fluctuations usually exceeds 5 ms, and may thus obscure the small effect to be expected by stimulating the DN. Another problem is that sequences of fictive flight normally last for only a couple of seconds so that on- and off-effects may mask the influence of the stimulation. These problems have been overcome in this study with a new preparation requiring only minimal dissection: locusts with intact thoracic box and neck joint are able to move their head and flap their wings in a normal fashion during tethered flight while DNs are recorded or stimulated intracellularly in the neck connectives. Flight sequences are much longer under these conditions, and fluctuations in the relative latency are less pronounced. We show that electricaJi

Electrical stimulation

of locust deviation detectors

193

stimulation of the identified DNs PI(2)5 (Hensler, 1988a) and DNC (Rowell and Reichert, 1986) elicits both considerable steering by wing muscles and compensatory head movements. Both resemble the optomotor behaviour elicited by horizon movements which excite the neurones in question. Materials and methods Experimental apparatus and preparation Experiments were performed at 24-30 °C on adult Locusta migratoria of either sex, from a laboratory culture kept on a light: dark cycle of 14h:8h. The legs of a locust were removed and a small piece of balsawood (6 mmx5 mmx lmm) was waxed onto the pterothoracic sternum, its edges touching the ventral insertions of M97 and M127 on either side (first basalar muscles of fore- and hindwings, respectively=direct wing depressors; the insertions are recognized as hairless patches). Electromyograms were recorded from M97 and M127 on both sides, using steel pins, about 2 mm long, inserted through the edges of the balsa block into the muscles for 100-200 ^m. The animal was waxed at the lateral walls of the pronotum into the U-shaped part of a holder (Fig. 1A). Pro- and pterothorax were fixed to each other with lateral bridges of beeswax. The posterior part of the pronotum which projects over the mesothoracic wing hinges was removed, and a 3 mmx3 mm window was cut in the remaining posteriodorsal part. The anterior third of the pronotum remained untouched to leave the neck joint intact. The neck connectives were exposed by removing overlying tissue, including the anterior portion of the gut (the remaining posterior portion was ligatured), connective tissue, fat body and both neck muscles M61 which lie directly over the connectives. Sometimes the dorsal longitudinal neck muscles M51 were damaged by the dissection, but the other 13 pairs of neck muscles remained intact, and no obvious change in head movements was observed (for anatomy and nomenclature of neck muscles see Shepheard, 1974; Honegger etal. 1984). The stimulus device used for the characterization of sensory inputs to DNs is described elsewhere in more detail (Hensler, 1988a). Here we give only a brief account. The animals were positioned in front of an artificial semi-horizon, a hemisphere with a diameter of 70 mm (Rowell and Reichert, 1986). Its lower half was painted black. The translucent upper half was painted with black dots, and illuminated by a small tungsten bulb from the rear. Visual input from behind the animal was prevented by covering the rear halves of the compound eyes with black enamel. Rotational course deviations were mimicked by horizon movements around the roll, yaw and pitch axes by means of servo-motors. Wind of up to 4 m s " 1 could be blown onto the locust's head through a tube (diameter 12mm) in the middle of the horizon. Active head rolling (rotation around the longitudinal body axis) was measured using a capacitance device (Sandeman, 1968). This measured, between two lateral fcensors, the position of a steel pin glued vertically to the head (Fig. 1A). Passive

K. H E N S L E R AND C. H . F .

ROWELL

head rolling was imposed under servo control using the pin as a lever. The servo was mounted on a manipulator, and a fork connected to its axis could be engaged with and disengaged from the lever while an experiment was in progress. Recording and stimulation of single neurones The neck connectives were manipulated with a glass hook onto a rectangular platform (1.5mmx2.5mm) made of copper and galvanically coated with gold (Fig. IB). A row of three pegs, about 300 fun in height, protruded vertically from both the anterior and the posterior edges. The distance between neighbouring pegs in a row was 300-400 fan. The connectives were placed between the lateral pegs, with the median pegs between them, and were subsequently fixed with tiny droplets of tissue glue (histoacryl blue, Braun, Melsungen, FRG). This allowed stable intracellular recordings during active and passive head rolling of up to 30°, and while normal flight activity was performed at the usual frequency of 15-20 Hz. Neurones were penetrated with glass microelectrodes containing 5 % Lucifer

Fig. 1. (A) Experimental apparatus used to simulate course deviations by moving an artificial horizon while neurones in the neck connectives of intact locusts were penetrated intracellularly. The locust was fixed into the U-shaped part of a holder. The neck connectives were supported by a metal platform inserted through a small window cut into the posteriodorsal part of the pronotum. A pin glued downwards onto the frontal part of the head served as a lever during imposed head rolling or, alternatively, as part of a capacitance device to measure the head position around the roll axis. Frontal wind was blown onto the locusts' head through a hole in the middle part of the horizon (arrow). For details see text. (B) Illustration of how the connectives were held in place by the metal platform.

194

K. H E N S L E R AND C. H . F .

ROWELL

head rolling was imposed under servo control using the pin as a lever. The servo was mounted on a manipulator, and a fork connected to its axis could be engaged with and disengaged from the lever while an experiment was in progress. Recording and stimulation of single neurones The neck connectives were manipulated with a glass hook onto a rectangular platform (1.5mmx2.5mm) made of copper and galvanically coated with gold (Fig. IB). A row of three pegs, about 300 /an in height, protruded vertically from both the anterior and the posterior edges. The distance between neighbouring pegs in a row was 300-400pan. The connectives were placed between the lateral pegs, with the median pegs between them, and were subsequently fixed with tiny droplets of tissue glue (histoacryl blue, Braun, Melsungen, FRG). This allowed stable intracellular recordings during active and passive head rolling of up to 30°, and while normal flight activity was performed at the usual frequency of 15-20 Hz. Neurones were penetrated with glass microelectrodes containing 5 % Lucifer

Fig. 1. (A) Experimental apparatus used to simulate course deviations by moving an artificial horizon while neurones in the neck connectives of intact locusts were penetrated intracellularly. The locust was fixed into the U-shaped part of a holder. The neck connectives were supported by a metal platform inserted through a small window cut into the posteriodorsal part of the pronotum. A pin glued downwards onto the frontal part of the head served as a lever during imposed head rolling or, alternatively, as part of a capacitance device to measure the head position around the roll axis. Frontal wind was blown onto the locusts' head through a hole in the middle part of the horizon (arrow). For details see text. (B) Illustration of how the connectives were held in place by the metal platform.

Electrical stimulation of locust deviation detectors

195

Yellow (Stewart, 1978) in the tip and 0.5moll" 1 lithium acetate in the shaft (resistance: 30-80 MQ). PI(2)5 and DNC were identified physiologically using the criteria described by Hensler (1988a) and Rowell and Reichert (1986), respectively. The appropriate tests included horizon movements, passive head movements, ocellar stimulation and frontal wind. DNs were penetrated at least 6-7 mm from their spike initiation zones which appear to be in the brain (Rowell and Reichert, 1986; Hensler, 1988a). Consequently, stimulation was not easily achieved by the injection of depolarizing current because the high electrode resistance in combination with the short length constants of DNs allowed excitation of neither the spike initiation zone nor of the axon itself. Stimulation was possible only by taking advantage of a post-inhibitory rebound effect: the axon was hyperpolarized with a current of 10-15 nA for 10-30 min. A burst of action potentials was elicited when the hyperpolarization was released. The frequency within the burst could reach more than 200 Hz when switching directly to depolarizing current. The doses of current depended on the quality of the penetration and on individual properties of the animal. Care had to be taken as this procedure easily led to serious damage of the axon if the hyperpolarization was too strong and/or the stimulation was repeated too often. All stimulation experiments were performed in complete darkness to prevent interference from visual feedback caused by the resulting head movement.

Results

Responses to simulated course deviations Tethered, flying locusts reacted to horizon rolling with compensatory head rolling, and with shifts of relative latency (for a definition see Fig. 2) in the first basalar muscles M97 and M127 (Fig. 2; see also Taylor, 1981a,b; Thiiring, 1986; Schmidt and Zarnack, 1987; Hensler and Robert, 1990; for changed timing in other wing muscles, see Mohl and Zarnack, 1977; Zarnack and Mohl, 1977). Shifts of latency in M127 were in the opposite direction to those in M97, and generally scatter was more pronounced. Both PI(2)5 and DNC (Fig. 3A) responded with bursts of action potentials to horizon rolling in the preferred direction (Fig. 3B, C). For PI(2)5 this was horizon rolling to the side opposite to the axon (thus simulating course deviation to the same side as the axon) (Fig. 3B). The preferred direction of DNC is horizon rolling to the same side as the axon (thus simulating course deviation to the side opposite to the axon) (Fig. 3C). The response properties of both neurones are described in detail elsewhere [PI(2)5, Hensler, 1988a; DNC, Rowell and Reichert, 1986]. Electrical stimulation of DNs Effect on wing motoneurones Electrically induced bursts of action potentials (200±30Hz for 200-300 ms) in

196

K. H E N S L E R AND C. H . F . R O W E L L

either PI(2)5 or DNC had no effects on wing muscles in non-flying locusts. During flight activity, however, considerable shifts of relative latency were elicited in both M97 and M127 (Fig. 4). The shifts were of similar sign and intensity to those caused by horizon rolling of about 40° (see Fig. 2). The maximum shift was not reached immediately but developed within the first 100 ms after onset of the stimulation, i.e. within two wingbeat cycles (Fig. 5). On average, 50% of the effect was reached within the first 30ms, i.e. well within one wingbeat cycle. Scatter of the individual values prevented exact determination of the minimum latency, but the data suggest that it is in the range of 10-20 ms. Spiking activity in Relative latency=rr-fi

M971

M97r

mm Illlllllllllll

Head roll

20 ms

Horizon roll

Fig. 2. Steering responses of a tethered, flying, intact locust, following sinusoidal horizon rolling from the normal position to 42° right and back. This simulated unintended roll deviation of the locust to the left. As a measure of steering we use the relative latency between the right and left first basalar muscles of the meso- and metathorax (M97 and M127). The relative latency is defined as the time difference between the first action potentials within a wingbeat cycle in the right and left muscle (relative latency=fr—1\, illustrated for M97 between arrowheads in the inset). It increases when the right M97 fires earlier with respect to the left M97 and vice versa. Horizon movements are followed by a compensatory head movement of up to 15°, and the relative latency in M97 and M127 shifts by several milliseconds. Individual dots are averaged from N single experiments (repetition rate: 0.25s"1). The start of horizon rolling was used as a reference for averaging the cycles. The line connects the means between two neighbouring values. Note that shifts in M97 and M127 go in opposite directions. Arrowheads indicate the absolute value of the relative latency which has, however, no meaning in this context.

Electrical stimulation of locust deviation detectors PI(2)5

DNC

197

Left PI(2)5

I!!

I III

!I

M1121

I

!

I |]l!

LJJlLi L 4-U-

Horizon roll '42° right

M127r

Horizon roll

3°°

left

Fig. 3. (A) Dorsal view of the descending deviation detector neurones PI(2)5 and DNC in the brain, suboesophageal, thoracic and fused abdominal ganglia. According to the position of the axon, PI(2)5 is defined as left and DNC as right (structure after Hensler, 1988a; Griss and Rowell, 1986). (B, C) Responses of PI(2)5 and DNC, respectively, to horizon rolling while the locusts perform flight activity (monitored as rhythmic activation of a wing muscle) in the presence of frontal wind (3 m s " 1 ) . Note that both neurones are excited by horizon rolling to the right (simulating unintended course deviations of the locust to the left) with a maximum frequency of 50-60 Hz. (C) DNC is also tonically excited by frontal wind. This input is modulated by the horizon position (Rowell and Reichert, 1986). This explains the decreasing excitation during anti-preferred horizon rolling (bar).

DNs ceased immediately when they were again hyperpolarized, but after-effects were observed in M97 and M127 which lasted for 3-5 wingbeats (Fig. 4). This shows that PI(2)5 and DNC are both involved in the control of corrective steering via short-latency pathways, and that single DNs are capable of producing considerable steering effects. However, single DNs are not designed to control steering alone, as the system is not suited to transmit for long the high-frequency bursts evoked by electrical stimulation. These evoked bursts consist of 40-60 action potentials at 200±30Hz, which are unphysiologically high rates compared with bursts evoked by horizon rolling: these contain 10-50 action potentials at less than 50 Hz (Fig. 3B, C). The motor effect of electrical stimulation declined dramatically, due to fatigue, when bursts of more than 300 ms were repeated more often than once per second. A moderate decline of the response was seen for ^ursts exceeding 200ms (Fig. 5, point on extreme right).

I '

198

K. HENSLER AND C. H. F. ROWELL

Effect on neck motoneurones Stimulation of PI(2)5 in quiescent locusts caused head rolling away from the axon of the stimulated neurone (Fig. 6). The head movement also contained yaw and pitch components but these were weak compared with roll. The latency between stimulus and head movement was about 30 ms. The delay between activity in neck MNs and detectable head movements is known to range between 10 and 20 ms (K. Hensler, unpublished results; A. Baader, personal communication); hence, DN excitation elicited the first action potential in neck motoneurones (MNs) after 10-20 ms. PI(2)5 frequencies of about 200 Hz normally led to head rolling of 5-10°, a similar range to compensatory head movements elicited by horizon rolling of 30-40°. The direction of evoked head rolling coincided with the direction of compensa-

—4 ms

Current

.... I...I

0 ms

-lOnA 0.5s

Fig. 4. Shifts of the relative latency in M97 (A, C) and M127 (B, C) evoked by current injection into the left PI(2)5 (A, B) and the right DNC (C). Bursts of action potentials (about 200±30Hz) were elicited for 200-300 ms taking advantage of a post-inhibitory rebound effect (see Materials and methods). Dots are averaged from N single experiments which were repeated every 2-3 s. The lines connect the means of three neighbouring values. Arrowheads indicate absolute values of relative latency. A, B and C are from different animals. Note that shifts of the relative latency are of similar strength and of identical sign to those caused by horizon rolling (Figs 2, 3).

Electrical stimulation of locust deviation detectors

199

100-

JS 50-

100 200 Time after commencement of electrical stimulation (ms)

300

Fig. 5. Mean shifts of the relative latency in M97 (7s£iV^18) within successive time windows of 30 ms (0-210 ms) and 90 ms (furthest right dot), starting with the first action potential of a burst evoked electrically in PI(2)5. Shifts are normalized with respect to the maximum value. Electrical stimulation was not synchronized with the wingbeat; hence, action potentials in M97 (and therefore the relative latency) occurred erratically within the respective time window. The abscissa represents the mean time. Error bars are 1 S.E.M. for shifts and 1 S.D. for the time after the start of the stimulus.

300 ms Right

Head roll

Fig. 6. (A) Electrical stimulation of a left PI(2)5 for 300 ms at about 200 Hz. This causes head rolling to the right of up to 10°. Successive bursts elicit similar head movement as long as the repetition rate does not exceed 0.5-1 Hz. (B) The response is suppressed during active head movements (first and second stimulus) but returns immediately when these stop (third to fifth stimuli). Note that evoked head rolling goes in the same direction as during compensatory head rolling which excites the left PI(2)5 (Figs 2, 3).

200

K. HENSLER AND C. H. F. ROWELL

tory head rolling caused by horizon movements in the preferred direction of the stimulated PI(2)5 (Figs 2, 3). For example, horizon rolling to the right, excited the right PI(2)5 and elicited compensatory head rolling to the right. Electrical stimulation of the same PI(2)5 also caused head rolling to the right. Analogous results have been observed for DNC (K. Hensler, unpublished results; A. Baader, personal communication), i.e. stimulation of the left DNC, which responds to horizon rolling to the right, was seen to elicit head rolling to the right. Thus, PI(2)5 and DNC are both involved in the control of compensatory head rolling via a pathway of short latency. Recognizable head movements were only elicited in quiescent locusts. Active head movements performed by the locust during flight or other behaviour suppressed the electrically evoked head movements (Fig. 6B). Even in averaged responses (up to five single events) no head movement was detected under these circumstances. Discussion We show for the first time in intact flying locusts that the identified descending deviation detector neurones (DNs) PI(2)5 and DNC are capable of eliciting motor activity which closely resembles the optomotor responses following simulated course deviations around the roll axis (Figs 2, 4). This strongly indicates that PI(2)5 and DNC are directly involved in corrective course control. Similar motor effects have been observed for other visual DNs which have not been identified morphologically (K. Hensler, in preparation). In the same context Baader (1989; and reviewed by Rowell, 1989) describes DNs which evoke head rolling and rudder-like movements of abdomen and hindlegs. Abdominal movements were never recorded when stimulating PI(2)5, although the projections of PI(2)5 into the fused abdominal ganglion (Fig. 3A) might provide subthreshold input to the abdominal motor system. Baader (1989) also observed antennal movements upon stimulation of some DNs. Although it is not clear whether PI(2)5 and DNC provide input to the antennal motor sytem, both send blebbed branches into the deutocerebral neuropile containing the arborizations of antennal MNs (Fig. 3A; Hensler, 1988a; Griss and Rowell, 1986; antennal MNs, Bauer, 1987). Action potentials which we evoked electrically in axons propagate in both directions from their point of origin and, in principle, the motor effects shown here could be caused by secondary, descending neurones driven by ascending action potentials. However, the short latency between stimulus and motor responses (

Suggest Documents