calories and nutrients in foods with intense sweeteners could have a variety ..... increases in hunger ratings after ingestion ofaspartame. Ofcourse, negative.
Effects of intense sweeteners and body weight: a review13 Barbara
food intake,
J Rolls
ABSTRACT
The
acesulfame-K has and, after saccharin ever,
on hunger,
most
investigators
have
that
aspartame
or unchanged increases has
little
weight.
Aspartame
intake;
indeed,
both
shown
foods or drinks in food intake.
that
with
Preliminary
clinical
ratings of hunger. ratings of hunger in on the
has
not
short-term
consumption
is associated
either
and
consumption
impact
and body
food
saccharin,
ratings of hunger food intake. How-
and
on various
in a group
found
for body
weight
reported
to decrease
self-rated increase
stomach self-rated
long-term
adox”
in their
suggest
that aspartame
ratings ratings
with consumption of the pleasantness
trials
program
in
sweeteners
have
never
been
compared
Am
J Clin
Nutr
portant
maintenance.
found to cause 199 1;53:872-8.
Intense
weight
in humans.
gain
or
not
offered
WORDS
food
intake,
Appetite, hunger,
aspartame,
intense
body
sweeteners,
weight,
dieting,
Although
be dissociated,
be ignored.
presumably
intense
sweeteners
such as aspartame
charm,
and saccharin
and
pared
intake,
and body much
weight. This uncertainty play in the popular press
only of no benefit in calorie control, but appetite. There have been several recent of intense
propriate,
sweeteners
in view
and
Effects
food
on eating
ofboth
of this issue, to review knowledge concerning petite
is fueled by suggestions that intense sweeteners
the popular
that they may studies on the
behavior. and
Thus,
scientific
it is
ap-
state of on ap-
sweeteners
on
hunger,
appetite,
subjects
Reports The
controversy
ofaspartame
compared sumed.
increase
began
in
in a preliminary in water
with Specifically,
Id, or 188 kcal),
872
ratings
intake
were
not
is extremely
im-
is an unintended
side
group has provided sweeteners aspartame,
further sac-
consumption.
can
all increase in food
intake
ofsolutions ofsweeteners is not only unfamiliar but the
personal unflavored
but
increased
consumption
was not associated
compared
with
aspartame
in solution
do diet
water.
drinks,
1 h later
could
He found appetite
drinks
There
are have
including
possible robust
differences
a key
compared
effect
sowith
with
increase
several a more
been
unflavored
sweetened
with a significant
may
has data of having
have
that
ratings
of soft
(3).
1990) condition
aspartame
ratings.
com-
of these
in water with no other also may be unpleasant.
experimental
appetite
ratings
communication,
unnatural
of aspartame water,
hunger
that solutions
as-
in appetite reasons
why
on appetite
in perceived
sweetness
intake that sweeteners
suggested
hunger
were decreases in The subjects were
so these
be unpleasant)
to an increase
consume
lutions
than
and food
aspartame The “par-
the increased
but they have not found
(M Tordoff,
partame
consumption.
of intense
lead
suggest
plain
to in both
that
ratings ofhunger are not always (2). Although hunger and food this does not mean that reported hunAt issue is whether increased hunger
acesulfame-K
to the increased
importance
this literature and to clarify the the effects of intense sweeteners
Tordoff that
were
to increase
referred
there taste.
Measuring
sweetener
with water,
Consumption added flavor
effects
despite
Since their initial report, Blundell’s results suggesting that the intense
sweeteners
are not increase
effect
preloads,
would
of intense
are widely used, some consumers may feel bewildered about the effects that these sweeteners will have on their appetite, food receiving
the intake.
can
effect
Introduction
to eat and
ofaspartame, of the sweet
after
screened
of glucose
has come to mean food consumption.
was that
actual
ger should (which
saccharin
preloads
in such studies, because index of energy intake
a valid intake
KEY
food
not
The
paradoxical
findings
with
consumption
were
motivation
popular press and, therefore,
or a reduction
after
who
whereas aspartame was reported to to eat and to decrease such ratings
so-called
of aspartame-sweetened
the hour
students
status.
fullness, motivation
The
over
female
self-rated
no change
diet-and-exercise
ratings
and
or dieting
the scientific and increases hunger
may be a useful aid in a complete weight
appetite
of male
of fullness.
controls
been
water
1986
report resulted
the control they
aspartame
t
appetite
that
when
Blundell
in an increase
condition, compared
solutions
and
consumption in ratings
in which the
ofa
effects
water
Hill
of appetite
was con-
of glucose
(I 3 Id, or 3 kcal),
(1)
solution
(786
and plain
Am J C/in Nutr
Johns 2
From
the Department
Hopkins Supported
of Psychiatry
University by NIDDK
School grants
and
Behavioral
Sciences,
The
of Medicine, Baltimore. DK39177 and DK40968.
3 Address reprint requests to BJ Rolls, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Meyer 1-108, The Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, 600 North Wolfe Street, Baltimore, MD 21205. Received April 25, 1990. Accepted for publication August 29, 1990.
l991;53:872-8.
Printed
in
USA.
© 1991 American
Society
for Clinical
Nutrition
Downloaded from www.ajcn.org at Institute of Atmospheric Physics,CAS on July 10, 2011
studies
of aspartame,
to increase to increase
found
it apparently
intake
to increase
have decreased consumption
situations,
of food
taste
reported consumption,
is associated with Even if aspartame some
sweet
been
SWEETENERS or the
expectation
of drinks
based
the explanation, familiar and and
subjects on their
such palatable
intake
food
might
have
previous
findings foods
if the data
from
hunger
They
through
suggest
satiety
value
No
matter
what
studies
that
an
that the sweet taste
cephalic-hepatic
reflexes.
This
increased related
might
not be involved
since
not
increase
plasma
not
measure
food
intake,
in some
situations,
ratings
it is not
clear
that
it has
data
have
in drinks and
should
ofsome
intense
this
practical
with intense been
thought
an effect
of the
sweet
It is appropriate,
therefore,
foods
with
intense
however,
we
and
intake.
drinks
First,
sweeteners
affect
taste
on
appetite
in terms that
the
should
Blundell
and
sweeteners
and
suggest
that
this
“residual
food
intake,
they
also
food
intake
because
of the
they
suggested
that
aspartame,
argue
that
intake,
Blundell 235
or food
and
food
on the
intake. and
rats of
food
intense
the
lead
sweeteners effects.
the
ester
or 470
mg
sweet
taste
argued
that
sweet
taste
to increased can decrease For
ofthe
in capsules,
However,
much
higherdoses
(5.04
had
no significant
effect
1 3).
Also,
investigators and
weight
in
108
point
to note
ofdoses
ofaspartame
ratings,
food
intake,
(75 male
mg
and
is that have or body
that
ratings safety
.
kg
female
capsules
never
to the dose
been
weight.
of the
.
the
ingestion
or food had
volunteers
of
intake
large
(10,
doses
of
no effects
on
(14).
containing shown
fuels
The
a wide
to increase
im-
range hunger
animal
taste
did
not
these
in a 2-h test,
storage
to see how
intake
and
drinking
explanation alters
and away
only
be
which
sacthey the dis-
from
systematic
oxi-
investigaintense
sweet-
food intake. Equivalent aspartame or cyclamate has
it is still
studies because
robust
declined
important
the effects
drinks
sweetened
since
to follow
ofsaccharin
are in other laboratories. Most of the recent sweeteners have concentrated on aspartame. offoods
(sweet
it should
that an unflavored However, Tordoff
in rats. The temporarily
of saccharin
these
onfood
the en-
as sweet.
of aspartame,
Effects
to greater
Thus,
Also,
through
and with
yogurt. However,
lead
yogurt.
are the
mechanisms
plain
on intake.
saccharin.
toward
consumption
findings
the
at
of the
to the palatability
that
studies
introduction
in humans
studies
with
the up on
of intense
aspartame
A summary ofthe short-term studies ofthe effects of aspartame on hunger and food intake is shown in Table 1. Effects ofaspartame in children. Studies ofthe effects of conofaspartame that
intake.
Birch
ferent
this
in familiar sweetener
et al (24)
sweeteners
children
ened kcal),
consumed
the
effects
intake
volume
and
increase
drinks
30- and
offour
after
different
dif-
The drinks
foods (Table 1). beverages sweet(13 Id, or 3 (376 kJ, or
no delay
60-mm-delay
with
children.
a set of seven snack water or fruit-flavored
ate ad libitum the
or food
of drinks
of 2-5-y-old ofone
by children
appetite
(376 kJ, or 90 kcal), aspartame plus low-glucose maltodextrin
Children For
foods not
compared
a fixed
with sucrose or aspartame delays.
does
on the food
before freely consuming The drinks were either
mm
aspartame
study,
d’)
1) reported
both
consumption
with
related
amounts
greater
after
starch
to the
( 17). Sub-
ate fixed
et al (3) had not found increased food intake.
possible
do not
90 kcal).
of aspar-
g)ofencapsulated
(24-wk)
aspartame
portant
found
10.08
on hunger
in a long-term
encapsulated body
other
Rogers
Although
indicate
ingestion
have
than
of metabolic
sumption
reduction in food intake with taking a placebo
was not related
rather
These
example,
bypassing
plus
generaliza-
or glucose, with no and were then given
effects
led to increased food intake is that drinking saccharin
dipeptide
Nevertheless,
that
therefore
but significant when compared
but this reduction
tame.
(10).
(1 1, 12) reported
aspartame
sweet taste, caused a small in a subsequent test meal
capsule,
men
that
position
effects
whether
could
in young
recalled
may
could affect hunger not been conducted
intense
selection
of intake yogurt)
(15)
glucose-sweetened
of the
postingestive
his colleagues
saccharin
of
aspartylphenylalanine, could affect appetite because phenylalanine stimulates the release of cholecystokinin (CCK) (8). The gut peptide CCK has been postulated to be associated with satiety (9). However, phenylalanine was not found to affect hunger, food
these
equicaloric
16) and to another
day
compared
to food,
was not tested in different sweeteners
significantly
ofthe
yogurt
eners have
of the
methyl
than
Blundell
paralleled
tions
when
hunger”
containing
intake
and
intake
investigations
(13,
sweetener
was
rest
The
and
because
ratings
yogurt
effects one
intake
the
could could
(1, 3) have
appetite
over
intake.
on appetite
his colleagues
increase
Food
and
charm propose
in capsules
Although intense
1 h later.
vs sour
their
either with saccharin starch and saccharin,
solution of saccharin and Friedman (18-2
is bypassed.
Aspartame
sweetened or with
sweeteners effects that
food
foods
by Rogers
dation.
data
intake
in the study
from
and
consider
food
jects
physiological
be made
to
interest,
The problem
sweeteners
and
found
implications
hunger
to review
sweeteners
sweeteners. to imply
different
hancement
eating
is oftheoretical
drinks with intense sweeteners could increase calories and nutrients in foods with intense have a variety of postingestive physiological obscure
does (4) did
has been
cannot
saccharin-containing
not be interpreted
acesulfame-K)
any
Alleva
can affect subsequent
Although
and drinks
these
taste
and
ofhunger.
the use offoods
data
the taste
aspartame,
increase
ofaspartame 7). Tordoff
so these
that the sweet
(ie, saccharin,
is that
(6,
(15) extended
on food
or 30- or 60-
intervals,
the caloric
drinks, sucrose and maltodextrin plus aspartame, reduced snack intake relative to water. For the 30-mm delay, aspartame reduced snack consumption relative to water. Birch et al (24) replicated this result no effect
hanced
in a subsequent experiment; or decreased subsequent food
intake
either
but
had
never
en-
it.
In a study
ofolder
children
(9-10
sweetened with aspartame, sodium given 90 mm before a buffet lunch
ened
aspartame
drinks
were
given
both
with
y old),
fruit-flavored
cyclamate, (22). The
and without
drinks
or sucrose aspartame-sweet-
added
were
carbohy-
Downloaded from www.ajcn.org at Institute of Atmospheric Physics,CAS on July 10, 2011
as an indication behavior. Thus,
ingestion
insulin
have
Motivational
saccharin
as the sweetener; aspartame is important to note because
may
lunchtime
with
Blundell
tions
lunch by
the finding that chewing flavored, sugar-sweetened gum stimulates gastric acid secretion (5). The cephalic release of insulin may
sweetened
and
of yogurt, sweetener,
increase
is supported
offoods
Rogers
unflavored
of aspartame was monotonically
873
INTAKE
using saccharin this study. This
are to be extrap-
chewing
FOOD Effects
the importance of using in studies of appetite
these
various concentrations but not in a way that
to concentration.
the
experiences.
emphasize and drinks
olated to real-life situations. Tordoff and Alleva (4) reported gum base with ratings ofhunger,
about
AND
874
ROLLS
TABLE
1
Short-term
effects
of aspartame
ratings
and food intake
Subjects5
Study Anderson (22)
on hunger
Ct al 1989
Dose
Exp 1: 9-l0-y-olds (n = 20)
in a test meal Vehicle
1 1 19 mg (1)
Hunger ratings
300 mL flavored drink with
Type oftest
Food intake
meal
No effect compared with cyclamate
Lunch, 90-mm delay
No effect compared with sucrose No effect or decreased
Lunch,
90-mm
Lunch,
60-mm delay
No effect compared with cyclamate
polycose Exp 2: 9-10-y-olds
295 mg (1)
300 mL flavored
(n = 20) Black
al 1990 (23)
Ct
Males
Birch et al 1989 (24)
only (n
=
drink 280 or 560 mL diet
160 or 320 mg 140 mg
20)
Exp 1: 4-5-y-olds (n = 24)
soda
205 mL flavored
NCt
Snacks,
drink
Exp 2: 2-4-y-olds (n = 20)
102 mg
Body weight NC
162 mg
delay
0-, 30-, 60-mm
NC
150 mL flavored
delay
Snacks, 0-, 30-, 60-mm delay
drink
No effect compared with sucrose No effect compared with water No effect or decreased 30 mm compared with water
at
No effect or decreased 30 mm compared
at
with water
Blundell and Hill 1986(1)
1990 (25)
1 12 mg
70 g corn
water
NC
compared
Increased
withwater No effect or decreased
flakes
Next
meal
NC No effect compared with unsweetened
and total
daily
cereal cereal Rodin
1990 (6)
Rogers Rogers (12)
250 mg
ci al 1988 (3)
Overweight and normal (n 24) (n = 12)
162 mg
500 mL lemonflavored water 200 mL plain water
et al 1990
Exp 1 (n
234 mg
Capsules
12)
Exp2(n=15)
Rolls et al 1988 (17)
Females
Rolls et al 1989 (26)
(n
Rollsctal 1990 (27, 28)
Exp 1:malesonly (n = 14)
Ryan-Harshman
et al
Lunch, 38-mm delay
No effect compared
Increased compared
Lunch, 60-mm delay
No effect compared
Lunch,
Decreased
with water
1990(4)
with water
NC
Capsules
60-mm
delay
Decreased
IlOand 220 mg
Gelatin and pudding (ad libitum) 237and473mL lemonade
reased
With
Exp 2: males only (n = 14)
1 10 and 220 mg
237 and 473 mL lemonade
Decreased
Lunch, 30-mm delay
Exp 3: males only (n = 14)
1 10 and 220 mg
237 and 473 mL lemonade
Decreased
Lunch, 60-mm delay
5.04 and lO.O8g
Capsules
No effect
0.05%, 0.30%,
Chewing
Lunch, 60-105-mm delay NC
=
Males
=
12)
32)
21 1-391
only (n
=
13)
Dieters and nond.ieters (n= 120)
Gelatin mg
(ad libitum)
gum
Increased
compared
withwater Decreasedcompared with water No effect compared with sucrose No effect compared with sucrose Noeffectcompared with water or no drink No effect compared with water or no drink No effect compared with water or no drink No effect
Lunch, 60-mm delay
Decreased compared with water Decreased
Cheese on crackers, 60-rain delay Lunch, 120-mm delay
only (n
1987(10)
Tordoff and Alleva
235and 470 mg 77 mg (1)
or sucrose
NC
with water =
Downloaded from www.ajcn.org at Institute of Atmospheric Physics,CAS on July 10, 2011
Mattes
(n=95) (n = 24)
200 mL plain
lunch
NC
0.50%, 1.0% 5
Normal-weight,
nondieting
male
and female
adults
unless
otherwise
specified.
Exp, experiment.
t Not collected.
drate (polycose), and the cyclamate-sweetened cose added. None ofthe treatments differentially fullness, desire to eat, or lunch-time food intake. consumed with or without polycose did not food
intake
in children
when
compared
with
drinks
had poly-
affected Thus,
affect sodium
hunger,
aspartame hunger or cyclamate
or sucrose. Effects ofaspartame in adults. We (26) examined the effects ofconsumption ofcommercially available foods sweetened with either sucrose or aspartame on appetite ratings and food intake in adults.
Normal-weight,
nondieting
males
and
females
were
given large portions of either a high- or low-calorie pudding or gelatin dessert and were instructed to eat as much as they liked. Subjects were either informed or not informed of the caloric content ofthe pudding or gelatin. All subjects ate similar weights
ofthe different caloric versions ofeach food. Despite the resulting difference in caloric intake ( 861 Id, or 206 kcal) after consumption of the’ high- or low-calorie version of the same food, subjects
did
not eat significantly
more
after
the low-calorie
food
when presented with a buffet lunch later. Over the 2 h after consumption
with a variety of foods 2 h ofthe puddings or gelatins,
ratings
amount
of hunger,
desire
to eat,
the
subjects
wanted
to
eat, and the pleasantness of the taste of the eaten food were similarly decreased and fullness was similarly increased by consumption ofthe different caloric versions ofthe foods. Knowing the caloric content ofthe pudding and gelatin did not influence intake sponded
both
or appetite: similarly
sucrose-
and
both
informed
and
in the tests. Thus, aspartame-sweetened
uninformed under foods
subjects
re-
these conditions, had similar effects
SWEETENERS
AND
FOOD
on hunger ratings over the 2 h after they were consumed, and food intake 2 h later was not differentially influenced by the sweeteners. We recently completed a study comparing the effects of as-
study
partame-
(1) that
or sucrose-sweetened
food intake nondieting
(27,
28).
males,
crose-sweetened a self-selection
lemonade
The
subjects,
consumed
237
on hunger
who or 473
were mL
ratings
and
normal-weight, aspartame-
receiving
the lemonade,
had been
used,
manipulated.
subjects
were
nor did they suspect When
the
drinks
not
told
which
or su-
taken
with
normal-weight,
There
is little because
studies
contribute
tam
to the warnings taste
sweet
outcome
may
restricting
energy
including
patterns
ratings
findings
are
ofeating
not
found
laboratories
after
ingestion
always
among
Several
laboratories,
not
a problem.
increases
Ofcourse,
They
lead
cer-
increases
found
ofaspartame.
in
aids “may
aspartame
have
Hill
found
prevalent that
also
and
been
such dietary
individuals.”
have
Other
has
ofhunger,
weight
Blundell’s,
intake.
of Blundell
of aspartame
ratings
of normal
hunger
sig-
The
are actively
the
to disordered
groups
was being meal,
people.
who
substance
to increase
food
the
nondieting
in individuals
intake.
sweetener
that the sweetener
were
were
well be different
their
lemonade, plain water, or no drink, either with lunch or 30 or 60 mm before the lunch. When
875
INTAKE
in
negative
to questions
about
nificantly more overall calories (lunch plus the drink) were consumed when subjects drank the sucrose-sweetened lemonade. This trend remained when the drinks were consumed 30 and 60 mm before the meal, but it was statistically significant only for the 473-mL sucrose-sweetened lemonade consumed 30 mm before lunch. There were never any differences in food intake between the aspartame and water conditions. Thus, aspartamesweetened drinks were not associated with increased food intake nor were there differences in hunger ratings among any of the conditions. Another recent study showed no effect ofaspartame in drinks on subsequent food intake (6). Male and female normal-weight and overweight subjects received 500 mL plain water or lemonflavored water mixed with fructose, glucose, or aspartame 38 mm before being offered a buffet lunch. The results showed that the fructose had the largest suppressant effect on food intake and that the aspartame-sweetened drinks and plain water did not differ. Black et al (23) also showed that aspartame-sweetened soft drinks and water had similar effects. Twenty normal-weight
whether particular features of the study’s design could account for the negative findings. However, in relation to the effects of
males
ment
tested
in four
conditions:
280
mL
mineral
water
consumed in 2 mm, 280 mL aspartame-sweetened diet soda (their choice of flavor) consumed in 2 mm, 280 mL diet soda consumed in 10 mm, and 560 mL diet soda consumed in 10 mm. All drinks were consumed 60 mm before lunch. The 560mL drink decreased hunger ratings over the 60 mm after consumption, but the other conditions had no significant effects on ratings of hunger or satiety. Intake in the lunch did not differ among conditions. Thus, aspartame consumption again did not increase ratings of hunger or affect food intake. Mattes (25) suggests that the effects ofdifferent sweeteners on hunger and food intake should be evaluated independently of their energy value and taste. Thus, he held the energy content and the rated pleasantness of a test meal constant. The effects of equicaloric breakfasts of unsweetened cereal or cereal sweetened with either sugar or aspartame on subsequent hunger and food intake over 5-d test periods were assessed using diet records. There were no significant differences in hunger ratings. Also, there were no significant differences in intake of the next meal, total daily intake, or the types of foods selected. Informing the subjects nificantly
about affect
the
caloric
intakes.
content There
of the was
breakfast
a nonsignificant
did
not
sig-
trend
for
the subjects who knew that they had consumed aspartame to eat more during the rest of the day, perhaps because they mistakenly believed that the aspartame-sweetened cereal had fewer calories. Mattes suggested that the value of intense sweeteners as aids in weight management will depend on responses to the expected energy savings they provide. Although this may turn out to be the case, it should be stressed that the subjects in this
there
ferent
laboratories
ofthe
studies
showed
have
of the
foods
drinks
(Table
on
as whether
it could
rather,
they
of as-
1). Nevertheless,
ratings
with
be ofinterest
food
dif-
in some
consumption
appetite
control
hunger
if
intense
in the study
intake.
on appetite
and
the sweet
taste taste
in ordinary
intake; and drinks
foods
the studies
about
the types
and
subsequent
interval the
the
age, body
have
intake;
have
effects
of sweetener food intake.
not been they
best
in agreesuggested
have
that may affect the outcome.
consumption,
gender,
and food
of sweetener
the
is no
asked. The sweeteners
sweeteners
to date
dose
administration,
appetite
sweeteners,
variables
the type
is administered,
sweetener
There being about
and whether the type affects appetite and
of intense
ofexperimental
sweetener
affects
conducted
the effects
include
intake.
is bypassed,
and food
Although
food
must fit the question a variety of questions
the sweet
when
sated),
and
that
The experiment reviewed asked
whether,
and
negative;
after
to increase
sweeteners
These
simply
from
Also,
has been much discussion (1 1, 17, 29) about the apexperimental design for the study of the effects of in-
design. studies
such
not
was characterized,
There propriate tense
were
findings
protocols.
ratings
paradigm
mechanisms
negative
different
in hunger
partame-sweetened sweeteners
several
used
the findings
a decrease
a robust
been
that
of the
offoods
with
learn
to adjust
should
attempt
termine
how
appetite
and food
Effects
of long-term
offered
subjects
the after
(hungry
or
are
or weeks
tense
sweeteners
ulatory
in the
energy
are required can
seen
have
throughout
subjects
these effects
can
experiments variables
to de-
of sweeteners
on
or sugar
content
controls manipulations
to determine
whether
affect
body
since
could
obscure
several the
density
on the short-term studies ofcaloric
in short-term
been
sweeteners
of sweeteners
shed light long-term
mechanisms intake
There sugar
manipulate
use of intense
ofthe
days
food
and involved
manipulation
or meals However,
content,
(30, 3 1). Future
intake.
Manipulations foods intake.
restraint,
energy
intake
to isolate
Experimental
reduced
subsequent they
dietary
way
sweetener
and whether or not the subject knows about the sweetener ( 17). Also, the familiarity ofthe subjects with the sweetener and the food could be important since some studies have shown that with repeated consumption
weight,
the the
of meal
type
state
used,
between
day
weight
of single of food over
the use of inweight regof sweeteners on
body
the effects
studies.
studies over
in which
several
days.
aspartame
replaced
Porikos
et al (32-
Downloaded from www.ajcn.org at Institute of Atmospheric Physics,CAS on July 10, 2011
were
aspartame,
876
ROLLS
35) studied
both
normal-weight
and
obese
individuals
confined
to a hospital room. Subjects were not told that the caloric content of the foods and drinks was being manipulated. Foods were served from platters with large portions available and subjects were encouraged to consume sweet drinks (they were required to drink at least two sodas a day). In the study of normal-weight individuals,
which
lasted
for
24 d, the
foods
and
drinks
were
kcal), or no soda the soda sweetened with the
higher
than
7, when
for the large
Subjects
consumed
the
enough
sugar,
of the
the
menu
weight
gain
items
stopped.
to produce
an
average decrease in energy intake of 24% when aspartame replaced sucrose. Initially, the subjects continued to eat the same weight of food they had been consuming during the sucrose baseline
lation.
and,
therefore,
their
customary
diet.
showed
imprecise
caloric
regu-
dilution, subjects compensated for and they did this by eating more of
However,
total
caloric
intake
significantly below N 15%) baseline levels over the rest of the 12-d dilution period. When, at the end ofthe study, sucrose was again consumed, energy intake rose back to the level eaten in the baseline period. Although Porikos et al (33, 35) suggest that aspartame may have some efficacy in weight control, more work is needed to substantiate
this
conclusion.
These
studies,
although
pioneering
other
No condition
volume
(ie,
of soda
study
of diet
sugar-sweetened
or drinks
products
are
run
to control
of both
in weight their
control, findings
surreptitious
substitution
sweetened
with high-fructose
studies
in which
are knowingly
normal
A recent
clinical
partame-sweetened
diet affected and women
study
explored whether the addition of asand beverages to a low-fat, hypocaloric weight loss (39). Fifty-nine obese, free-living men were assigned randomly to a hypocaloric, balanced foods
their
exchanges
supplementation with in the experimental
as aspartame-sweetened
aspartamegroup were
pudding
also included support groups, ercise instruction. There were
behavior modification, no significant differences
switched
weight
and
and
then
were
switched
back
to sucrose.
of aspartame
ment, drinks
In another
accurately reduced manipulation The
was
associated
residential
study
in which
caloric
intake
monitored, the carbohydrate content by 1.67 MJ (400 kcal) for 3 d (37). Most
subjects,
was due who
to the substitution
were
six
could
normal-weight,
be
for sugar.
nondieting
males,
were naive about the manipulation. Apart from the required lunch and for a 1-h period after lunch when no food was available, subjects could eat freely from a wide variety offoods. They made up for the difference in calories in the lunches every day of the experiment and this compensation was seen by dinner time. Total daily energy intake was the same regardless of the energy
content
of the
and unaware
ofthe
in the energy
content
energy intake. In a recent study, ofconsumption or aspartame cessive
daily soda
3-wk
Thus,
manipulation,
in subjects
not
trying
a 1.67 MJ (400-kcal)
oflunch
had
no significant
effect
Alleva
sweetened intake and subjects
with
body
(9 females
1.2 L aspartame-sweetened sweetened with high-fructose
soda
corn
(38) compared high-fructose
to diet
reduction on daily
weight. and (1 3 Id,
syrup
the effects corn
Over 2 1 males)
three
to use other group was told
or saccharin-sweetened
loss
of the
groups
The
In a follow-up period
after
products.
with
males
syrup
suc-
received
or 3 kcal), 1.2 L (2.22 Mi, or 530
study
the
The
without
lost slightly the
(40) of the
the
more
the
12-wk
or
with all asprogram
and cxin the
aspartame
weight
females
12-mo
12 wk of treatment,
products to avoid
sup-
without
showed
as-
the
op-
weight-maintenance patients
decided
them-
selves whether or not to use aspartame. By the end of the 12 mo, both groups were using similar amounts ofaspartame. Fac-
tors associated
with
maintenance
sumption, correlation was found
and decreased desire for sweets. A strong negative between aspartame consumption and weight regain for the males but not the females. For males larger
aspartame
intake weight
aspartame
at the end
maintenance.
does not increase some improvement
levels,
loss at I 2 mo were
physical
better
activity
of weight
increased
increased
aspartame
ofmaintenance This
study
was associated indicated
weight gain and suggested in weight loss and weight
is substituted
that there maintenance
takes
were
study
obtained
from
suggests (41).
that
aspartame
Seven-day
I 6 women
who
may baseline
had
who had successfully lost weight and maintained average of 13 y. The subjects were then instructed aspartame aspartame
their consumers
from
diets and
stress
may be when that
maintenance
authors
with
aspartame
their
clinical
The
that
that
efit for weight
for sugar.
con-
sample size was too small for definitive conclusions and more long-term investigations of the effects of aspartame body weight in obese men and women are needed. Another
Tordoffand
ofsodas on food periods,
lunch.
were encouraged The control
partame (10.4 vs 12.2 kg) whereas posite trend (7.4 vs 5.8 kg).
of lunch was ofthis dietary
of aspartame
partame-
plementation.
with a reduction, not an enhanceconsumed in a variety of foods and
of food intake when over several days.
for
required.
milk shake and intense sweeteners.
to aspartame,
of
substituted
6 d when the baseline was being established, the sucrose intake was well above normal levels so that the manipulations took place under abnormal conditions. Furthermore, the experiment was not counterbalanced for order of sweetener presentation. Subjects always started with sucrose for the first 6 d, were The experiments should also be conducted so that the rotation goes from aspartame to sucrose and then back to aspartame. Despite these criticisms, the studies indicated that consumption
the too
investigations
milk
first
was
in intake data imply
naturalistic
foods
given
the
the
ofsoda
36) argues
during
in both
with a reduction Although these
involved
Their
aspartame for large volumes corn syrup. Clearly, more
diets
that
of the
intake
Consumption
broadly.
with or without products. Patients
(29,
1.2 L water)
be of some benefit about extrapolating
diet either containing
Booth
caloric
consumed.
that aspartame may authors are cautious
A criticism is that the baseline did not represent the normal
subjects.
were higher
Consumption
daily
was associated other sources.
and precise, had some problems. diet encouraged overeating and of the
the soda
significantly
conditions.
decreased
types of sweet drinks of refined sugar from
Clinical
from
and
body
females.
quantities
remained
calories
of
was associated
with the no-drink condition and was associated weight in the males and no change in weight
decreased
in the
(the
Consumption
syrup
been
on
be of bendietary
in-
obese
but
the loss for an to exclude all
for 3 wk. Five subjects were 1 1 were regular consumers.
occasional The base-
Downloaded from www.ajcn.org at Institute of Atmospheric Physics,CAS on July 10, 2011
40%
period
After 4-6 d of the ofthe missing calories,
in the
corn
intake)
soda
sexes compared with
replaced
intakes
additional
aspartame-containing
sweetened with sucrose over the first 6 d of the test. With free access to a variety of foods, the subjects gained weight. On day aspartame
caloric
of this
gains
order.
with high-fructose
daily
source
weight
in a counterbalanced
SWEETENERS line
caloric
stention regular
intakes
of these
from aspartame aspartame users
aspartame,
and
there
two
did found was
groups
did
not
differ,
AND
and
ab-
not affect intake. However, the dietary control difficult without
a tendency
for caloric
intakes
to in-
FOOD
INTAKE
situations,
there
controls been
These
clinical
problem normal
studies
of knowing environment
plied
with
are
difficult
to interpret
whether participants, and were self-reporting
dietary
modifications.
because
of the
who were in their intake, really com-
Also,
in
multicomponent
weight-loss programs it is difficult to attribute single component of the intervention (42).
weight
loss to a
found
showed
to
that
The (43)
study
1982
benefit
in long-term designed
survey
as evidence
weight
to look
by Stellman
that
intense
maintenance.
Garfinkel
That
from
Data based
study,
however,
in daily
the
energy intake sweeteners are
relevant
only
to saccharin
sweetener
was
not
in weight
changes
dieters
women been
lost
ofthe
saccharin
users
at all,
it is
because
survey. and
that
Differences
nonusers
were
kg). The study specifically excluded women their dietary habits (including sweetener use) 10 y, and this would have eliminated many
from
the survey.
more
ignored
not aspartame,
at the time
between
very small(0.23-0.68 who had changed over the previous serious
use and
available
it is relevant
weight
in most
The
fact that
if they
reviews
intense
used
of these
the most
data.
overweight
sweeteners
Stellman
has
and
Gar-
finkel (44) state that the weight changes they observed probably had liule clinical meaning. They also point out that the saccharin users may have gained weight ifthey had not used the sweetener. They conclude that casual long-term use of intense sweeteners in the
absence
itselflead is the help
of other
to weight key
with
here.
major
loss. The
Because
weight
changes
need
intense
loss only
in lifestyle
for other
changes
sweeteners
through
dietary
does
can
not
changes,
to
the
Future methods body
result
changes.
can
show
as establishing
mind
very
epidemiologic for determining
weight
surveys
of the
It should
retrospective
randomized
and
In addition,
(45)
trolled,
size
be stressed,
correlations
causality.
ducting
sample
of this
epidemiologic
that
such
be interpreted should
about
the dangers
studies
instead
bear
in
of con-
of well-con-
trials.
is much
more
on food and
one
to learn
intake
study
and
investigators
associated aspartame
hunger.
have
with aspartame is associated
Even
if aspartame
about
body
in humans
sumption may be followed tiating this finding requires
eral
Laboratory-
sugar
indicate
that
if
The
caveat
here
intake
Ifthe
food is not
will
actively
studies
depend
uses
the consump-
to restrict
may remain unchanged. part ofa weight-control
but
these
with measuring regimens. and
because
be run in they lead
sweeteners
on food
intake
field studies
and
energy. that
sweeteners
pro-
can
to know
be extrapolated lack
naturalistic
difficult
and with ensuring
precision,
the laboratory and to similar conclusions intake
suggesting
long-term
intrinsically
it is difficult
studies
daily
with reduced data
More
are
Because
laboratory-based
food,
intake,
on how the use of intense patterns and body weight
individuals.
needed,
on
However, if intense program, they could aid
of foods and drinks with intense intake and weight gain in dieters.
are
should whether
Ifan individual for one higher
to eat a high-calorie
trying
Little information is available sweeteners affects food-consumption
these
nondieting
probably
individual
as an excuse
in community-dwelling
is that
the
because
compliance whether
rig-
to natural parallel
field about
studies
to determine the effects
of
U
weight.
body
References 1. Blundell
JE,
Hill AJ. Paradoxical effects of an intense sweetener on appetite. Lancet l986;l: 1092-3. Hunger ratings are not a valid proxy measure of reported in humans. Appetite l990;15:l03-l3. Carlyle J, Hill AJ, Blundell JE. Uncoupling sweet taste comparison of the effects of glucose and three intense on hunger and food intake. Physiol Behav 1988;43:547-
2. Mattes RE. food intake 3. Rogers PJ, and calories: sweeteners 52. 4. TordoffMG, Alleva AM. Oral stimulation with aspartame increases hunger. Physiol Behav 1990;47:555-.9. 5. Helman CA. Chewing gum is as effective as food in stimulating cephalic phase gastric secretion. Am J Gastroenterol 1988;83:6402.
sweeteners in rats
limited.
in normal-weight,
on caloric
low-calorie
(aspartame)
Conclusions There
intake.
motivation.
consumption motes food
study.
however,
cannot
researchers
admonitions
or
in food
the exclusion
studies should employ more rigorous actual sweetener consumption and
only
Feinstein’s
large
very
replaced
primarily
individual
situations
in lifestyle
studies foods
or a reduction
calorie control by providing palatable foods It needs to be stressed that there are no
orous
of subjects who made such changes could be the reason for the failure to find clinically significant weight loss. The statistically significant, but clinically irrelevant, weight change of ‘-0.45 kg is likely
ofa
or if the
to dietary
be expected
energy
effect
individual’s
ofproblems
by
the not
several
subjects who were unaware ofthe manipulation. voluntarily substitutes a reduced-calorie product
ability
that
are
aspartame
were conducted
in calories,
obesity.
on has
the calorie reduction is substantial, compensation will be incomplete. Thus, there will be some reduction in daily energy intake, but substitution of only a few foods may not result in a
tion
extent
not
impact
indeed,
no change
studies
in which
women aged 50-69 y, it relied on recalled body weight and sweetener use and there was no check for the validity and reliTo the
and
any
Aspartame
aspartame-sweetened
either
long-term
studies
the
recall.
incidence
are of no
of
with
it has weight.
intake;
food
consumption
In
of this
at cancer
and
sweeteners
increase
that body
the effects
weight. suggest
Short-term that
by increased food further investigation.
reported
increases
consumption, with decreased
increases
l990;5
studies
saccharin
con-
tion
in ratings
of hunger
of hunger found that ratings of
in some
with
LC, Stegink and
without
LD, Brummel
carbohydrate
Cantor
P. Cholecystokinin
in plasma.
MC, et al. Aspartame
in phenylketonuric
subjects: effect on plasma concentrations and insulin. Metabolism l990;39:39l-6. 8.
aspartame, glucose, and intake. Am J Clin Nutr
1:428-35.
7. Wolf-Novak
intake; substanAlthough sev-
most have or unchanged
ratings
of intense
6. Rodin J. Comparative effects of fructose, water preloads on calorie and macronutrient
of amino Digestion
inges-
and
acids,
normal
glucose,
l989;42:181-201.
9. Smith GP, Gibbs J. The satiety effect of cholecystokinin: recent progress and current problems. Ann NY Acad Sd 1985;448:41723.
Downloaded from www.ajcn.org at Institute of Atmospheric Physics,CAS on July 10, 2011
was
cited
and
intake.
studies
epidemiologic
is often
evidence
intake
is associated
reduction Epidemiologic
is no
of food
drinks
crease.
877
878
ROLLS
10. Ryan-Harshman M, Leiter LA, Anderson GH. Phenylalanine and aspartame fail to alter feeding behavior, mood and arousal in men.
Physiol Behav l987;39:247-53. 1 1. Blundell JE, Rogers PJ, Hill methodological
Appetite 12.
aspects
1988;lI(suppl
Rogers
PJ,
Pleming
30.
intake
Blundell
and satiety.
Springer-Verlag,
1987:161-72.
Rolls
studies
Si.
Human
JE.
Aspartame
ingested
In: Dobbing
J, ed. Sweetness.
and intake. Appetite
MG,
food intake
Appetite
Friedman
24.
Drinking
saccharin
solution
food intake, appetite 1989;1 3:93-103.
and
hedonic
increases
response
in
Black RM, lanaka subjective hunger,
PA, Crombie CA, Anderson GH. Diet soft drinks, and food intake. FASEB J 1990;4:A940(abstr). Birch LL, McPhee L, Sullivan S. Children’s food intake following drinks sweetened with sucrose or aspartame: time course effects. Physiol Behav 1989;45:387-95.
25.
Mattes intake
26.
Rolls BJ, Laster U, Summerfelt A. Hunger and food intake following consumption oflow-calorie foods. Appetite l989;13:l 15-27. Rolls BJ, Kim 5, Fedoroff IC. Effects of sucrose and aspartame on hunger, thirst and food intake. FASEB J 1990;4:A787(abstr). Rolls BJ, Kim S, FedorofflC. Effects ofdrinks sweetened with sucrose or aspartame on hunger, thirst and food intake in men. Physiol Behav 1990;48: 19-26.
27. 28.
RD. Effects ofaspartame and sucrose on hunger in humans. Physiol Behav 1990;47:l037-44.
Applied
Masher
and
eating
ed. Developments
Science,
P, Fuller
conditions
and energy
Louis-Sylvestre
Metab
1988;l 1:
and preference: IV. Cephalic phase and metabolic factors. l989;12:37-56. GH, Saravis 5, Schacher R, Zlotkin 5, Leiter L. Aspartame:
22. Anderson effect on lunch-time children. Appetite 23.
MI.
Elsevier DA,
of low-calorie
usefulness IH,
1987:287-3
J. Starch
human
pleasantness
16.
content
appetite
sweeteners in sweeteners.
foods
of ordinary
and
satiation
of starch-paired
indexed
flavours.
by
Appetite
J, lournier
A,
Verger
P. Chabert
M,
Delorme
B.
Learned caloric adjustment ofhuman intake. Appetite 1989;12:95103. 32. Porikos KP, Booth G, Van Itallie TB. Effect ofcovert nutritive dilution on the spontaneous intake ofobese individuals: a pilot study. Am J Clin Nutr 1977;30:l638-44. 33. Porikos KP, Hearer MF, Van Itallie TB. Caloric regulation in normal weight men maintained on a palatable diet of conventional foods. Physiol Behav l982;29:293-300. 34. Porikos KP, Pi-Sunyer FX. Regulation of food intake in human obesity: studies with caloric dilution and exercise. Gin Endocrinol
Berlin:
19. lordoff MG, Friedman MI. Drinking saccharin solution increases food intake and preference: II. Hydrational factors. Appetite 1989;12: 1 1-21. 20. Tordoff MG, Friedman MI. Drinking saccharin solution increases food intake and preference: III. Sensory and associative factors. Appetite 1989; 12:23-35. lordoff
31.
35.
ofsweeteners
of the
In: Grenby
l982;3: 163-84.
without
92-3. 18. IordoffMG, Friedman MI. Drinking saccharin increases food intake and preference: I. Comparison with other drinks. Appetite l989;l2: 1-10.
21.
Booth
Evaluation control.
l984;l 3:547-61.
Porikos reducing
KP,
Van
Itallie
food intake:
TB.
studies
U Jr, eds. Aspartame
Efficacy
of low-calorie
with aspartame.
physiology
Foltin
RW,
Fischman
MW,
Moran
for lunches in a residential
compensation
by humans
TH,
Rolls
in
LD, Filer
and biochemistry.
Marcel Dekker Inc, 1984:273-86. 36. Booth DA. Mechanisms from models-actual the zero-calorie drink-break option. Appetite 102. 37.
sweeteners
In: Stegink
New York:
effects from real life: 1988;1 1(suppl l):94RI, Kelly
IH.
varying in fat and carbohydrate laboratory. Am J Gin Nutr
Caloric
content 1990;52:
969-80. 38.
Tordoff
MG,
Alleva
AM.
Effect
of drinking
soda
sweetened
with
aspartame or high-fructose corn syrup on food intake and body weight. Am J Clin Nutr 1990;5 1:963-9. 39. Kanders BS, Lawn PT, Kowalchuk MB, Greenberg I, Blackburn GL An evaluation ofthe effect ofaspartame on weight loss. Appetite 1988;! 1(suppl 1):73-84. 40. Kanders BS, Lavin PT, Kowalchuk M, Blackburn GL Do aspartame (APM)-sweetened foods and beverages aid in long-term control of body weight?. Am J Clin Nutr l990;5 1:5 15(abstr). 41. Evans E. Effect ofwithdrawal ofartificial sweeteners on energy intake of stabilised post-obese women. Int J Obes 1989;13(suppl 1): 1 1 l(abstr). 42. Brownell KD, Wadden TA. Behavior therapy for obesity: modern approaches and better results. In: Brownell KD, Foreyt JP, eds. Handbook ofeating disorders. New York: Basic Books, 1986:18097. 43.
Stellman
SD,
Garfinkel
L.
Artificial
sweetener
use
and
weight change among women. Prey Med 1986;15:195-202. 44. Stellman SD, Garfinkel L. Patterns of artificial sweetener weight change in an American Appetite 1988; 1 l(suppl 1):85-9 45.
Feinstein
AR.
menace
of daily
Scientific
standards
life. Science
Cancer 1.
Society
in epidemiologic
1988;242:
1257-63.
prospective studies
one-year use
and
study. of the
Downloaded from www.ajcn.org at Institute of Atmospheric Physics,CAS on July 10, 2011
17.
DA.
associatively
l):54-61. HC,
Booth in weight
London:
Uncoupling sweetness and calories: of laboratory studies on appetite control. AJ.
tasting inhibits hunger and food intake. Physiol Behav 1990;47:l23943. 13. Anderson GH, Leiter L. Effects ofaspartame and phenylalanine on meal-time food intake of humans. Appetite 1988;l l(suppl l):4853. 14. Leon AS, Hunninghake DB, Bell C, Rassin DK, Tephly IR. Safety oflong-term large doses of aspartame. Arch Intern Med l989;l49: 23 18-24. 15. Rogers PJ, Blundell JE. Separating the actions ofsweetness and calories: effects of saccharin and carbohydrates on hunger and food intake in human subjects. Physiol Behav l989;45:1093-9. 16. Rolls BJ. Sweetness
29.