Effects of motor preparation and spatial attention on corticospinal excitability in a delayed-response paradigm
Rogier B. Mars1,2, Sven Bestmann1,3, John C. Rothwell1, & Patrick Haggard2,4
1
Sobell Department of Motor Neuroscience and Movement Disorders, Institute of
Neurology, University College London, London WC1N 3BG, United Kingdom 2
Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, London WC1N 3AR,
United Kingdom 3
Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, Institute of Neurology, University College
London, London WC1N 3BG, United Kingdom 4
Department of Psychology, University College London, London WC1E 6BT, United
Kingdom
This is a preprint of the paper to appear in Experimental Brain Research DOI 10.1007/s00221-007-1055-4 The original publication is available at www.springerlink.com
Keywords: movement preparation, spatial attention, delayed-response task, corticospinal excitability, TMS, motor preparation, response preparation Correspondence: Rogier B. Mars Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience 17 Queen Square London WC1N 3AR Tel:
+44 (0)20 76781151
Fax:
+44 (0)20 79168517
E-mail:
[email protected]
Abstract (max 250 words)
The preparation of motor responses during the delay period of an instructed delay task is associated with sustained neural firing in the primate premotor cortex. It remains unclear how and when such preparation-related premotor activity influences the motor output system. In this study, we tested modulation of corticospinal excitability using single-pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) during a delayed-response task. At the beginning of the delay interval participants were either provided with no information, spatial attentional information concerning location but not identity of an upcoming imperative stimulus, or information regarding the upcoming response. Behavioral data indicate that participants used all information available to them. Only when information concerning the upcoming response was provided did corticospinal excitability show differential modulation for the effector muscle compared to other task-unrelated muscles. We conclude that modulation of corticospinal excitability reflects specific response preparation, rather than non-specific event preparation.
Introduction
Humans and other primates are able to use prior information to prepare their motor system for a later response (Rosenbaum 1980). This motor preparation is evident in a shorter reaction time and is reflected in preparatory neural activity. An important question to ask when trying to understand the nature of preparatory neural processes is with what type of information a certain neural structure is dealing. For instance, in the domain of arbitrary or symbolic visuomotor associations (Wise and Murray 2000), the dorsal premotor cortex (PMd) has consistently been shown to exhibit strong preparatory activity (e.g., Kurata and Wise 1988; Toni et al. 1999). However, the type and locus of activity within PMd differs depending on the amount of information concerning the upcoming movement that is available to the participant (Hoshi and Tanji 2000; Mars et al. 2005). When preparatory information is given about only some attributes of the imperative stimulus, activity can be found in the rostral part of PMd. Conversely, when the information about the imperative stimulus is sufficient to specify the response in advance, preparatory activity is present in the most caudal parts of PMd which have access to the primary motor cortex and the spinal cord (Mars et al. 2005). Consistent with this finding, modulation of activity of spinal neurons during movement preparation has been observed in monkeys (Prut and Fetz, 1999). Studies probing human corticospinal excitability in delayed-response paradigms using transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS), however, have yielded inconsistent results. A number of studies have reported a decrease in corticospinal excitability during instructed delay periods, but no response-specific change in corticospinal excitability
(e.g., Touge et al., 1998; Hasbroucq et al., 1999). Conversely, some recent studies reported increases in corticospinal excitability, specific to the prepared response (Van den Hurk et al., 2007; Van Elswijk et al., 2007). These conflicting results can be partly explained by differences across studies in the timing of the response, and therefore in the degree of preparation, or in the predictability of the TMS pulse. Both of these factors have been shown to influence corticospinal excitability (Van Elswijk et al., 2007; Takei et al., 2005). Interestingly, Van den Hurk et al. (2007) employed a range of delay periods (varying between one and nine seconds) resulting in a low predictability of both response time and timing of the TMS pulse. However, these types of delays are quite unusual in preparation studies using TMS and may result in fluctuations in the level of motor preparation during the delay period. In the present study, we aim to investigate whether a specific increase in corticospinal excitability with selective response preparation can be found in a delayed response paradigm with delays in the ranges normally studied using TMS. Furthermore, we employ spatially compatible cues to instruct movement preparation. Since previous studies (e.g., Van den Hurk et al., 2007) cued movement preparation using arbitrary cues (cf. Wise and Murray, 2000), it remains to be seen whether these results generalize to other types of movement cueing. Participants were required to respond by pressing a button with either the left or right hand in response to a trigger cue. The trigger cue was presented either to the left of the right side of fixation and consisted of a symbolic cue instructing one of the two possible responses. At the beginning of each trial, an instruction cue could give participants prior information on the location of the upcoming trigger cue or the
movement it would instruct. We probed corticospinal excitability during the delay period between the instruction and the trigger cues using single-pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation. This allowed us to investigate whether the processing of advance motor information has distinct effects on corticospinal excitability over and above effects of prior information and general preparation. Critically, the timing of both response time and TMS pulse varied and was equally (un)predictable between conditions.
Materials and Methods
Participants Eleven healthy right-handed volunteers (4 women; age range 18-31 years) participated in the experiment. All participants gave their informed consent prior to participation. Experimental procedures were approved by the local ethics committee and performed according to the ethical standards laid down in the Declaration of Helsinki. No participants reported any adverse effects of TMS. The data of the last two (out of four) experimental blocks of one participant were discarded due to excessive head movements.
Electromyographic recording Surface electromygraphic (EMG) was recorded from the right first dorsal interosseous (FDI) muscle with a belly-tendon montage using Ag/AgCl surface electrodes. Raw signals were amplified using Cambridge Electronic Design 1902 amplifiers with a band-pass filter of 1-2000 Hz. Signals were stored on a personal computer for later analysis at a sampling rate of 5 kHz. Participants were asked to maintain relaxation of the target muscle throughout the experiment.
Stimulation procedure Transcranial magnetic stimulation was applied over the hand area of the left motor cortex with a figure-of-eight coil (70 mm outer wing diameter) using a Magstim 200 stimulator (Magstim Co., Whitland, U.K.). The coil was held tangentially over the left side of the scalp with its handle pointing backwards at an angle of approximately 45º
from the midsagittal axis. The site of stimulation was optimal to elicit motor-evoked potentials (MEPs) in the right FDI muscle. The coil position was marked on the participants head so that it could be maintained at that location during the experiment. Stimulation intensity for the main experiment was set to evoke an MEP of 1 mV peak-topeak amplitude, corresponding to a stimulator output of 30-60% for all participants.
Experimental procedure The experiment consisted of a training and an experimental part. During the first training session (30 trials), participants were trained on the associations between two visual stimuli and two motor responses (Fig. 1A). Motor responses consisted of button presses with the index finger of the left and right hands, respectively. On each trial, participants were presented with one of two visual shapes (300 ms, centrally presented). Following a variable delay (1500-2000 ms) participants heard an auditory beep (1000 Hz, 300 ms) after which they responded as fast as possible to the movement instructed by the visual shape. Visual feedback was presented centrally 200 ms later to indicate correct (‘V’), incorrect (‘X’), or too late (‘!’) responses. In a second training session, participants were familiarized with the main experimental task (Fig. 1B). Participants fixated on a cross at the centre of the screen throughout the task. At trial onset, a central instruction cue was presented for 300 ms, followed after a delay (varying between 1500-2000 ms, uniform distribution) by a target stimulus (‘trigger cue’, 200 ms). The trigger cue was one of the previously trained visual shapes and was presented to the left or right of the fixation cross at approximately 14.5˚
visual angle. Participants were instructed to press the button associated with the visual shape as quickly as possible. The instruction cue distinguished between the three experimental conditions: (1) NEUTRAL:
a question mark with an arrow pointing both left and right, providing no
information regarding the forthcoming trigger cue; (2)
SPATIAL:
a picture of an eye with
an arrow pointing to the left or the right indicating with 100% validity where the subsequent trigger cue would appear; or (3) MOTOR: a hand with an arrow pointing to the left or the right indicating with 100% validity which response (left or right press) the subsequent trigger cue would require. Consequently, the three different conditions provided different degrees of certainty regarding the required motor response. Participants had either no advance information (NEUTRAL), information regarding the spatial location of the imperative stimulus (SPATIAL), or full information regarding the required motor response irrespective of the spatial location of the trigger stimulus (MOTOR). Participants trained each of the different conditions separately for 16 trials each. They then performed one block of 50 trials with all three conditions intermixed. Performance feedback was provided as in the first training session. An additional training block of 50 trials was performed without performance feedback, and with TMS applied unpredictably on 50% of trials. The TMS pulse was applied 200 ms prior to the presentation of the trigger cue, to ensure that any movement preparation was maximal at the moment of stimulation. Following the training block, reaction times were inspected to ensure similar performance with and without TMS being
present. If necessary, this procedure was repeated until participants were able to perform the task equally well independent of the presence of TMS. Four experimental blocks of 50 trials each were then run. Each block contained 10 NEUTRAL
20
trials, 20
MOTOR
SPATIAL
trials (10 cueing the left side, 10 cueing the right side), and
trials (10 cueing left hand movement, 10 cueing right hand movement). On
half of the trials, participants received a single TMS pulse 200 ms prior to presentation of the trigger cue. Both trial type and application of TMS were randomized.
Data analysis Trials with incorrect, multiple, or premature (RT < 80 ms) responses were discarded from the analysis. Reaction times for correct trials were analyzed in a two-way repeated measures ANOVA with factors TMS (two levels: TMS and no TMS) and CONDITION (three levels: NEUTRAL, SPATIAL, and MOTOR cues). Peak-to-peak amplitude of each MEP was quantified using custom software in Matlab (The Mathworks, Natick, MA). Trials in which participants failed to relax the target muscle fully prior to the TMS pulse (maximum EMG amplitude > 75 µV in the 200 ms prior to the TMS pulse) were discarded from the analysis. The size of MEPs was expressed as the percentage of the mean MEP amplitude in the
NEUTRAL
condition in
each block in order to account for between-block differences. MEPs were analyzed in a two-way repeated measures ANOVA with factors CONDITION (2 levels: MOTOR
SPATIAL
and
cues) and SIDE (2 levels: left and right). For the RT and MEPs analyses,
significant effects in the ANOVAs were taken as justification for post-hoc pairedsamples two-tailed t-tests.
Although we discarded trials in which there was precontraction of the muscle (see above) we further assessed whether participants were able to relax the target muscle during the delay interval by calculating the root mean square EMG signal recorded in the interval 200-50 ms prior to the TMS pulse. As with the MEP amplitude, this value was normalized to the mean root mean square value of the NEUTRAL condition in each block.
Results
Behavioral results Participants made few errors during the experiment, averaging 97.5% correct responses. Reaction times on correct trials (Fig. 2A) decreased with increasing information regarding the required motor response (main effect of CONDITION: F(2,20)=61.72, P