Empirical Observations of Red Light Running at Arterial ... - CiteSeerX

6 downloads 0 Views 672KB Size Report
at Arterial Signalized Intersection. Liping ZHANG, Kun Zhou, Wei-bin Zhang and James A. Misener. California PATH Program, University of California, Berkeley ...
Empirical Observations of Red Light Running at Arterial Signalized Intersection Liping ZHANG, Kun Zhou, Wei-bin Zhang and James A. Misener

California PATH Program, University of California, Berkeley

1

Empirical Observations of Red Light Running at Arterial Signalized Intersection Abstract Red Light Running (RLR) has become an increasely major national safety issue at signalized intersections. Significant efforts have been made to understand the RLR related driver behaviors and develop countermeasures to reduce RLR and its related crashes. At high speed intersections, it has been well shown that drivers caught in the dilemma zone is an important reason of RLR. While at arterial intersections, there are still needs to further investigate into RLR related driver behaviors. We carried out our research at a well-designed arterial intersection (speed limit 35 mph to 30 mph, approach dependent) and collected data using multiple discrete point sensors at different distances-to-intersection from multiple approaches. Empirical data showed that for over 90% of the RLRs, the drivers were not trapped in dilemma zone at yellow onset. Instead, they could have stopped safely and comfortably. Further analysis of the empirical data showed that over 60% of the RLR were with a headway less than 2 seconds, or belonged to a platoon. The average headway of RLR vehicles were 5% − 10% less than that of vehicles going through yellow. The findings were used in the development of an all-red interval extension system for intersection collision avoidance.

Index Terms Red-light running, dilemma zone, driver behaviors, option zone

I. I NTRODUCION A. Red-Light Running Red-light-running (RLR) is an increasingly major national safety issue at signalized intersections. In 2004, 2.5 million or 40% of all police-reported crashes in the United States occurred at or near intersections [1]. Of these intersection-related crashes 22.5% were fatal and 46% resulted in injury. The RLR aspect of this problem is known and serious [1–3], as approximately 20% of all intersection crashes occur due to signal violation crashes [3]. Significant efforts, span from enforcement to engineering countermeasures, have been taken to counteract the issue caused by RLR. It is vitally important to understand how RLR and its related crashes happened and to build engineering countermeasure systems to reduce RLR and RLR caused crashes. In

2

fact, the US DOT and state DOTs have formed several programs to investigate such collision avoidance systems. The research is part of the U.S. Department of Transportation (DOT) initiated Collaborative Intersection Collision Avoidance System CICAS program (Signalized Left Turn Assistance −SLTA and Traffic Signal Adaptation − TSA) conducted under the auspices of the University of California, Berkeley effort[4–6].

B. Red Light Running and Arterial Intersections The dilemma zones are widely believed to contribute to the RLR behavior. As defined in the ITE hand book [7], a dilemma zone is the zone , in which a vehicle approaching an signalized intersection at yellow onset can neither safely / comfortably stop before the stop bar nor clear the intersection safely (equivalently enter the intersection before red onset). This is the classical definition of dilemma zone which is associated with the signal timing [8], and thus is also called yellow time dilemma zone [9]. Denote the duration of the yellow phase as Y, the average perceptive reaction time (PRT) as δ and the speed of the vehicle as v(t), a simple kinematics formulation of the dilemma zone [9] is that when the distance of the vehicle x(t) satisfies both x(t) > Y · v(t),

(1)

and x(t) < δ · v(t) +

v2 , 2a

(2)

where ”a” is the acceleration rate to stop safely / comfortably. Note that in the definition, we assume that the vehicle do not accelerate to proceed through the intersection, and this is based on our observations from field intersection (see later sections of this paper). Actually if the vehicle accelerates to proceed, it may be able to clear the intersection even if it is in the dilemma zone defined in (1) and (2). This simple form of dilemma zone makes it more general (covering more vehicles) and static [9, 10]. The yellow time dilemma zone has been named as the “Type I dilemma zone”, while a second type, ”Type II dilemma zone” or the option zone has also been introduced. It is defined as the zone in which 10% to 90% of the drivers at yellow onset would stop. There are also some other definitions of the option zone, which are relatively easier to deal with. Researches show that the option zone typically is between 2s to 5s in terms of time-to-intersection of the vehicles at yellow onset. Figure 1 illustrates two types of dilemma zones in terms of the speed and distance to intersection at yellow onset. It is clearly seen that

3

the two types of dilemma zones are not independent. Without loss of generality, the option zone is more commonly seen than the type I zone.

Distance to Intersection @ Yellow onset (feet)

600

500

400

300

← v⋅(Y)

Type I Dilemma zone →

200

← 2s to intersection 100

0

Fig. 1.

v2/2a + δ v→ 5s to intersection →

0

10

20 30 40 50 Speed @ Yellow onset (mph)

60

70

Dilemma Zones (Yellow Interval = 4sec, a = 0.3g)

For high speed intersections, RLRs are typically associated with the dilemma-zone (type I) problems, as reported by Sheffi and Mahmassani [11]. The isolated, high speed intersections usually feature smoother traffic and drivers tend to exceed the speed limit more frequently. From the definition of the (type I) dilemma zone as well as from Figure 1, it is clear that drivers are more likely to be trapped in the dilemma zone when the speed is high, especially when it is higher than posted speed limit (intersection yellow intervals are designed to eliminate the dilemma zone, at least to some extent, for vehicles at posted speed limit[8, 9]). The dilemma zone protection system, such as (dynamic) green extension systems are proven to be effective in reducing RLR at high speed isolated intersections. At typical arterial intersections, the situation is different. California, for example, has state arterial roads with speed limit mostly at 30 mph to 35 mph. For intersections at such roads, it is much less likely for the vehicles to encounter the dilemma zone. Earlier studies showed some relevant results of RLR behavior at arterial intersections. Gate, Noyce et al in a recent research observed one to two days of videos recorded using commercial cameras at two low speed intersection at Madison, Wisconsin area. Manual review of these video showed that necessary deceleration rate for over 85% of the RLR vehicles

4

to stop was less than 12.7ft2 /s, which is a safe deceleration rate [12]. Observations of those different driver behaviors are interesting. An important subject to investigate into is how these observations could help to predict RLR and its related hazards. And it motivated the current research, which via field data collection and analysis, tried to: •

collect high quality RLR data at typical arterial intersection; and



learn RLR related driver behavior at arterial intersection when posted speed limits are lower (such as 30 mph, 35 mph). The research performed here focused on the empirical observations from infrastructure based sensors,

which would provide speeds of each individual vehicle at different distances-to-intersection. The sensors we used were emulated speed loops of the Autoscope (R) cameras. The characteristics of each individual driver, such as sex, age, etc., however, were not available during the study. This research tried to get the following specific results related to RLRs at arterial intersections. •

Basic Statistics of RLR, as opposed to the first-to-stop vehicles and the go-through-yellow vehicles;



Distances and speeds at Yellow onset, to learn whether or not drivers were trapped in the dilemma zone; and



Headway distributions of RLR, as opposed to the first-to-stop vehicles and the go-through-yellow vehicles.

Our primary method is to compare the observed empirical statistics of RLR vehicles with those first-tostop vehicles and go-through yellow vehicles to form reasonable conclusion on the RLR related driver behaviors. II. DATA C OLLECTION AND P ROCESSING A. Field Setup and Configuration of Point Detectors Field data were collected using the Autoscope cameras. Nine cameras were installed at three approaches of an arterial intersection (Page Mill Road and El Camino Real, Caltrans State Route 82) in the San Francisco Bay Are. We note that the signal phase and timing setting of this intersection fully conforms to the ITE standard. Three cameras were installed for each intersection leg, covering respectively the exit area, stop area and advance area. We summarize the configurations into Table I. Raw video data were also recorded to serve as ground truth of the engineering data. Part of the video clips were manually reviewed to verify the RLR events. Data were collected during May 14th, 2008 to June 21st, 2008.

5

Fig. 2.

Google Earth Snapshot of the Field Intersection

As can be seen in Table I, multiple emulated speed loops were placed at the advance area as well as the stop area of difference approaches. Results of the configurations of the emulated speed loops at the field intersection are summarized in Table I. We note that “distance” indicates the distance of sensor from the stop bar to upstream direction (in feet). When less than zero, it indicates a sensor inside the intersection.

B. Sensor Calibration A Nissan Infinity FX45 probe vehicle equipped with high accuracy GPS and data computer was used for calibration. For each run and for every leg, the engineering data obtained from the Autoscope speed loops were processed to generate the vehicle trajectories. The tracking and data association procedure helps to reduce the occasional missed-call / false-call problems of video camera based vehicle detectors [13]. The Autoscope measured speed was then compared to the wheel speed recorded by the computer on-board vehicle. Results showed that for most associated trajectories, the measurement from Autoscope speed loops presented speed error lower than 10% (c.f. Figure 3). It can also be seen that the error of the speed loop was random and can therefore be viewed as an independently-distributed error which is simply added to the variance of the observation vector (c.f. Figure 4).

6

TABLE I C ONFIGURATION OF THE FIELD INTERSECTION

Page Mill rd. at El Camino Real East bound

West bound

South bound

Lanes (recorded)

2

1

2

Speed Limit (mph)

30

35

35

Average Speed (mph)

30

35

35

90% speed less than (mph)

45

45

40

Yellow Duration (s)

4

4

4

All-red Interval (s)

0.5

# Speed Loops (Exit)/ Lane

1

1

1

# Speed Loops (advance) /

3

3

3

3

3

2

2.5s

2.5s

2.5s

0.15 Mile from up-

No upstream intersec-

0.25

stream intersection

tion for More than 0.7

upstream intersection,

mile upstream

main street

Lane # Speed Loops (stop / conflict entry) / Lane 95% RLR time-into-red less than (s) Description

of

the

ap-

proach

mile

from

TABLE II C ONFIGURATIONS OF THE AUTOSCOPE VIRTUAL LOOPS

Page Mill rd. at El Camino Real : Sensor Configurations East bound

West bound

South bound

Average Speed of RLR

30 mph

35 mph

38 mph

Locations of All Speed

-110, -15, 0, 15, 40, 63,

-110, -15, 0, 20, 40, 95,

-120, -30, 0, 30, 90,

Loops

97, 120, 190

139, 190

120, 195

For the Autoscope sensors, the missing rate and false report rate varies from different data sources. Our own observation of the missing rate of Autoscope speed sensors is about 1.5%, and false report rate 4.1%. This is obtained by comparing the Autoscope output data with 50 cycles of human readings. The

results are shown in Figure 5.

7

Autoscope Speed Loop measurements (mph)

50 45 40 35 30 +10% error →

20 15 10 5 5

Fig. 3.

← -10% error

25

10

15

20

25 30 35 Wheel Speed (mph)

40

45

50

Comparison of Autoscope Speed and Wheel speed

0.35

0.3

Frequency

0.25

0.2

0.15

0.1

0.05

0 -10

-8

-6

-4

-2

0

2

4

∆ v (mph)

Fig. 4.

The empirical distribution of the error of Autoscope speed measurements

6

8

10

8

Fig. 5.

The statistics of Autoscope missing rate and false report rate

C. Data Processing The stored engineering data from emulated speed loops of Autoscope included the speed, timestamp (accuracy up to 33ms) as well as the real-time status of the signal phase. The discrete sensors reports were associated using a Multiple-Hypothesis-Tracking (MHT) algorithm to build full trajectories of each vehicle (proceeding through the intersection or (first-to) stopping at the intersection) [4]. For each approaching vehicle i, we have a series of speeds {v1 (i) , v2 (i) , . . . , vK (i)}

(3)

{t1 (i) , t2 (i) , . . . , tK (i)} ,

(4)

and time-stamps

at the following distances to intersection, {d(1), d(2), . . . , d(K)}

(5)

where K is the total number of discrete emulated speed loops for each lane. To simplify the expressions, the time stamps were all referenced to the time of yellow onset of the recorded cycle.

9

Yellow onset

Distance to intersection (ft)

190

Red onset

dY(i+1)

d(1) RLR

dY(i) 120 Go through yellow 97

d(2) d(3) d(4)

63 40

tinred

15 0 t3(i) t4(i)

t3(i+1)

d(5) d(6) d(7) Stop bar

Time (sec)

Fig. 6.

Time space diagram

Interesting samples of all these trajectories were divided into three categories, to include the first-tostop vehicles, the going-through-yellow vehicles and the RLR vehicles. The observed trajectories were further processed and only the above three interesting types of data were extracted. We also excluded all the samples when the above mentioned vehicles were moving very slowly due to heavy traffic. In such scenarios, vehicles might experience stopping-then-going maneuver which is difficult to track. We put a lower speed limit of 10 mph, average speed above which was our interesting case. During our field observation period for over one month, we did not see any RLR case with speed lower than 10 mph. The numbers of total interesting samples collected in the field observations were tabulated in Table III. III. E MPIRICAL O BSERVATIONS This section summarizes an analysis of the empirical observations of the RLR. Specifically the analysis compared the RLR statistics to the other two closely related vehicle maneuvers, the first-tostop and the going-through-yellow. We examined the following issues, to include (1) the statistics of the speeds and acceleration; (2) the distances and speeds of interesting vehicles at yellow onset; and (3) headway distributions at advanced area. The first issue helps to understand how the RLR vehicles

10

TABLE III I NTERESTING S AMPLES

Page Mill rd. at El Camino Real East Bound

West Bound

South Bound

RLR Occurrences

61

35

22

First-to-Stop Occurrences

912

419

1627

Go through Yellow Occur-

4318

1718

3005

rences

generally proceed through the intersection. The second issue helps to identify if the RLR drivers were trapped in the dilemma zone at the time of yellow onset. A comparison of the RLR trajectories with other trajectories also helps to reveal the possibilities of RLR causes like the inattentiveness of the driver and deliberate running, etc. We studied the third issue to further identify the driver behaviors of RLR vehicles’ interaction with leading vehicles. Summary of the variables used in the samples are provided in Table I, Table II and Table III.

A. Statistics of Speeds and Accelerations During the field observation, 118 vehicles were found to run the red light. We first get the running speed of red light violator i, which is the speed at stop bar, or running speed = vK (i), when i is a RLRer.

(6)

K is the index of the stop-bar speed loop.

The observed statistics of running speed were compared to the speed of the yellow through and the results are shown in Table IV. It can be seen that the RLR vehicles do have a slightly higher average speed than the vehicles going through yellow, while the differences are not significant. And when compared the running speeds to the posted speed limit (c.f. Table I), it is also observed that running speeds are at or around the posted speed limits. For West Bound and South Bound where the posted speed limits are higher, we do find the running speed also higher than that from the East Bound where the posted speed limit is 5 mph lower. Average speed and the acceleration rate of the RLR vehicles are also compared to the other two kinds of maneuvers. The acceleration is calculated using the speed difference at two discrete locations

11

TABLE IV AVERAGE S PEED AND ACCELERATION

standard devia-

acceleration

standard devia-

v¯(i)

tion of v¯(i)

a ¯(i)

tion of a ¯(i)

30 mph

7.4 mph

0.1 m/s

1.9 m/s2

28 mph

7.5 mph

0.1 m/s2

2.1 m/s2

First-to Stop

20 mph

8.2 mph

-2.1 m/s2

2.4 m/s2

RLR

35 mph

7.2 mph

0.2 m/s2

1.6 m/s2

28 mph

6.3 mph

0.2 m/s2

1.7 m/s2

First-to Stop

22 mph

6.9 mph

-1.4 m/s2

1.7 m/s2

RLR

35 mph

8.2 mph

-0.1 m/s2

2.1 m/s2

34 mph

10 mph

-0.2 m/s2

2.0 m/s2

23 mph

9.2 mph

-1.4 m/s2

2.4 m/s2

RLR East Bound

average speed

Go

through

2

Yellow

West Bound

Go

through

Yellow

South Bound

Go

through

Yellow First-to Stop

close to intersection (such that most drivers would have already made their decisions to go or not) a ¯(i) =

vk2 (i) − vk1 (i) , tk2 (i) − tk1 (i)

(7)

where k1 and k2 are set to the indexes of the 60 ft and 90 ft sensors, and the average speed of a running vehicle is formulated as v¯(i) = (vk1 (i) + vk2 (i)) /2.

(8)

Results are summarized in Table IV. We further group the RLR and going-through-yellow maneuvers as going-through maneuvers as opposed to the first-to-stop maneuvers. The vehicles proceeded through the intersection were observed, in average, to move at constant speed. While there is a significant difference in the acceleration rate for the go-through and first-to-stop maneuvers, as expected. We note that the running speed distribution (average to standard deviation ratio) shows good agreement with the finding reported by Bonneson, et al in [14]. B. Distances and Speeds at the Time of Yellow Onset The distances and speeds of the observed interesting vehicle trajectories are of vital importance to understand the driver behavior. The dilemma zone (and the option zone) theory tells if the driver was able to pass the intersection safely or stop safely. The information of which zone the RLR drivers were in

12

or were not in at the time of yellow onset could distinguish the trapped-in-dilemma-zone with inattentive / deliberate running. The distances and speeds at the yellow onset were not directly reported by the discrete sensors, since they were configured to report vehicle speed at given locations only, not at given time. The distances and speeds at the yellow onset were obtained using the following interpolation method. We denote the speed of vehicle i at yellow onset as vY (i) and the corresponding distance as dY (i) (c.f. Figure 6). The time of yellow onset is denoted as tyon . The distances of the vehicle i at tyon is obtained using

dY (i) =

 d(k2 ) − d(k1 )    · (tyon − tk−1 (i)) ,

when tk1 (i) < tyon < tk2 (i)

  d(1) + (t (i) − t ) · v (i), 1 yon 1

when t1 (i) > tyon

tk2 (i) − tk1 (i)

,

(9)

.

(10)

and vY (i) in a similar way, vY (i) =

   vk2 (i) − vk1 (i) · (tyon − t  k−1 (i)) ,

when tk1 (i) < tyon < tk2 (i)

  v (i), 1

when t1 (i) > tyon

tk2 (i) − tk1 (i)

In (9) and (10), at the time of yellow onset tyon , if the interesting vehicle i has not yet arrived at the first advanced detector, or t1 (i) > tyon , we assume that the vehicle moved at constant speed from where it was at yellow onset to the first detector. If the interesting vehicle i has already passed the first detector at the time of yellow onset, then it must be between two of our detectors, say k1 and k2 . The distance and speed at yellow onset are then obtained from the interpolation of the speeds and distances at the detectors k1 and k2 . The statistics from the interesting samples with respect to (9 and (10) are summarized in Table V and illustrated in Figure 7-9. The parameters used in the dilemma zone definition (1) and (2) are δ = 1.0s,

(11)

a = 0.3g, and

(12)

and time-to-intersection is from 2s to 5s for the option zone. Based on the speeds and distances at the time of yellow onset, we categorized the RLR and goingthrough driver behaviors into different zones, according to the dilemma zone definition, to include - the dilemma zone, in which drivers could neither stop safely nor go through before the red onset in current speed;

13

- safe-to-stop zone, in which the distance to intersection was far enough for the driver to react and safely stop; and - good-to-go zone, in which the drivers could pass the intersection at current speed legally. Note that the safe-to-stop zone and the good-to-go zone could be overlapped. Or in another word, a vehicle could be in a zone that it is both safe to stop and not too late to proceed through the intersection legally. TABLE V S TATISTICS OF D ISTANCES AND S PEEDS AT Y ELLOW O NSET

Page Mill rd. at El Camino Real East Bound

West Bound

South Bound

Number of RLR samples

61

35

22

Number in dilemma zone

6 (10%)

1 (3%)

2 (9%)

Could Have Stopped Safely

52(85%)

31 (88%)

14 (64%)

Observed Decelerate-then-go

9 (14%)

4 (11%)

7 (32%)

Go through Yellow Samples

4318

1718

3005

Would have violated signal if not

431 (10%)

52 (3%)

241 (8%)

accelerated

The statistics of the RLR drivers percentage of being trapped in dilemma zone was less than 10%. We note that this percentage depends on the definition and parameters of the dilemma zone. When acceleration to pass is allowed, the dilemma zone becomes even smaller [9]. The bigger the drivers perceptive reaction time (PRT) is, the larger the dilemma zone is. Current setting of PRT or δ = 1s is based on the widely used agreement that PRT < 1s for 85% of the drivers [14]. If the drivers were not actually in the dilemma zone, they might be in (1) the safe-to-stop zone, but they did not stop; (2) good-to-go zone; or both. The statistics also showed that, from 64% to 88% of the RLR vehicles were actually in the safe-to-stop zone. Instead, they did not stop due to their attitude or inattentiveness. The percentage of the RLR vehicles in the safe-to-stop zone is in agreement with a previous study by Gates, et al. , where they showed that over 85% of the observed RLR drivers could have stopped safely [12]. It is also possible that the RLR drivers were in the good-to-go zone while still violated the signal. This was observed to happen when the drivers decelerated at first and then changed the decision to go. The deceleration in the beginning caused some delay and turned the passing to a violation. The percentage

14

of these observed decelerate-then-go behaviors at different approaches varied from 11% to 32% (for the South Bound, the sample size is relatively small, 7 out of 22 were observed with decelerate-then-go). We illustrated the distances and speeds of RLR and yellow through vehicles in Figure 7-9. The illustrations also showed that among the go-through yellow vehicles, from 3% to 10% were not in the good-to-go zone, or in another word, they would violate the signal if they did not accelerate. From the figure, this is the area above the straight line v · Y . These accelerate-to-go drivers were actually at the same situation with most of the RLR drivers, while the difference was that they accelerated and made it and the RLR drivers did not. In this next subsection, we get statistics of the headway to address the inattentiveness of RLR drivers. (a): East Bound

← v2/2a + vδ

Distance@ Yellow onset(ft)

500

400 ← v⋅ Y 300 Type I Dilemma zone → 200

100

0

Fig. 7.

5s to intersection →

← 2s to intersection

0

10

20

30 40 50 Velocity@ yellow onset (mph)

60

70

Distances and Speeds at Yellow Onset (East Bound, Circle: RLR, Triangles: Go through yellow)

C. Headway Distributions The headway at the 90 ft detector was used for all the three interesting categories. The headway distributions were summarized in Table VI. The headway statistics showed that in average, the headway of RLR vehicles was the smallest among the three interesting categories. The average headway of RLR vehicles ranged from 1.74s to 2.0s, with a standard deviation from 0.53s to 0.73s. While for the yellow through vehicles, the average headway ranged from 1.82s to 2.2s and the standard deviation from 0.72s to 0.74s. The headway of the RLR vehicles were observed to be 5% to 10% less than that of the yellow through vehicles. The difference in the headway showed that RLR vehicles were closer to the leading

15

(b): West Bound

← v2/2a + vδ

Distance@ Yellow onset(ft)

500

400 ← v⋅ Y 300 Type I Dilemma zone → 200

100

0

Fig. 8.

5s to intersection →

0

← 2s to intersection

10

20

30 40 50 Velocity@ yellow onset (mph)

60

Distances and Speeds at Yellow Onset (West Bound, Circle: RLR, Triangles: Go through yellow)

(c): South Bound

← v2/2a + vδ

Distance@ Yellow onset(ft)

500

400 ← v⋅ Y 300 Type I Dilemma zone → 5s to intersection →

200

100

0

Fig. 9.

70

← 2s to intersection

0

10

20

30 40 50 Velocity@ yellow onset (mph)

60

70

Distances and Speeds at Yellow Onset (South Bound, Circle: RLR, Triangles: Go through yellow)

vehicle than those went through yellow. The statistics of the RLR headway showed that from 60% to 75% of the vehicles had a headway less than 2s. These vehicles were likely to be in a platoon tail. Distributions of the headway distributions are illustrated in Figure 10-12. The percentile of the vehicles which had a headway less than 2s were from 60% to 75% for different approaches. Further studies of the platoon ratio [14] of these observed flow would be helpful to understand

16

TABLE VI H EADWAY D ISTRIBUTIONS

East Bound

West Bound

South Bound

average

standard devia-

percentile

headway

tion of head-

headway

way

than 2s

RLR

1.98 s

0.73 s

60%

Go through Yellow

2.2 s

0.74 s

55%

First-to Stop

2.3 s

0.80 s

53%

RLR

2.0 s

0.72 s

70%

Go through Yellow

2.13 s

0.77 s

55%

First-to Stop

2.4 s

0.75 s

45%

RLR

1.74 s

0.53 s

75%

Go through Yellow

1.82 s

0.72 s

70%

First-to Stop

2.0 s

0.78 s

60%

the behaviors in a context of platoon.

(a) East Bound 30 Headway of RLR Headway of Yellow through Headway of First−to−Stop

Percentage (%)

25

20

15

10

5

0

Fig. 10.

0

1

2

3

4 5 Headway(s)

Headway Distribution at Advanced Detector (East Bound)

6

7

8

9

of less

17

(b) West Bound 30 Headway of RLR Headway of Yellow through Headway of First−to−Stop

Percentage (%)

25

20

15

10

5

0

Fig. 11.

0

1

2

3

4 5 Headway(s)

6

7

8

9

Headway Distribution at Advanced Detector (West Bound)

(c) South Bound 30 Headway of RLR Headway of Yellow through Headway of First−to−Stop

Percentage (%)

25

20

15

10

5

0

Fig. 12.

0

1

2

3

4 5 Headway(s)

6

7

8

9

Headway Distribution at Advanced Detector (South Bound)

IV. C ONCLUSION The empirical observations of the Red Light Running vehicles were obtained using multiple emulated speed loops from the Autoscope cameras. We installed nine cameras at one typical arterial intersection in the San Francisco Bay area to cover three approaches of that intersection. Over one month of data were collected and among which we selected the RLR , first-to-stop and go-through yellow samples. Statistics

18

of different interesting samples were obtained, to include the speed and acceleration distributions, the distances and speeds at the time of yellow onset and the headway distributions. We addressed the RLR related behaviors by a comparison study of the above-mentioned statistics to the other two interesting maneuvers. The statistics showed that the going-through yellow and RLR vehicles were in average moving at constant speed. Over 90% of the RLR drivers were not trapped in the dilemma zone at the time of yellow onset. For all the RLR cases, from 64% to 88% cases were observed to be in the safe-to-stop zone. It was also observed that a certain amount of RLR trajectories featured decelerate-then-accelerate-to-go behaviors (from 11% to 32%). The headway analysis showed that in average RLR vehicles had an headway about 5% − 10% lower than that of going through yellow group, and over 60% of RLR vehicles had a headway less than 2s. These statistics could help in understanding the behaviors of inattentive violators. Further study at more arterial intersections is necessary to justify the validity of the findings as a general conclusion for arterial intersections. ACKNOWLEDGMENT This work was sponsored by the State of California Business, Transportation and Housing Agency, Department of Transportation (Caltrans) and U.S. Department of Transportation (DOT) as part of the CICAS project. The contents of this paper reflect the views of the authors, who are responsible for the facts and accuracy of the data presented herein. The contents do not necessarily reflect the official views or policies of the State of California. R EFERENCES [1] D. R. Ragland and A. A. Zabyshny, “Intersection decision support project: Taxonomy of crossingpath crashes at intersections using GES 2000 data,” University of California, Berkeley, No. UCBTSC-RR-2003-08, 2003. [2] J. Bonneson, D. Middleton, K. Zimmerman, H. Charara, and M. Abbas, “Development and evaluation of a detection-control system for rural intersections,” U.S. Department Transportation, Washington D.C., Federal Highway Administation FHWA/TX-02-4022-1, 2001. [3] N. Brittany, B. N. Campbell, J. D. Smith, and W. G. Najm, “Analysis of fatal crashes due to signal and stop sign violations,” National Highway Traffic Safety Administration, Washington D.C., Report No DOT HS-809-779, 2004. [4] L. P. Zhang, K. Zhou, W. B. Zhang, and J. Misener, “Prediction of red light running based on

19

statistics of discrete point sensors,” in Proceedings of 88th TRB Annual Meeting, Washington D.C., 2009. [5] J. Chang, D. Cohen, L. Blincoe, R. Subramanian, and L. Lombardo, “CICAS-V research on comprehensive costs of intersection crashes,” National Highway Traffic Safety Administration, Washington D.C., Report No 07-0016, 2007. [Online]. Available: http://www-nrd.nhtsa.dot.gov/ pdf/nrd-01/esv/esv20/07-0016-O.pdf [6] C.-Y. Chan, “Observations of driver time gap acceptance at intersections in left-turn acrosspath opposite-direction scenarios,” Transportation Research Record: Journal of the Transportation Research Board, no. 1910, pp. 10–19, 2005. [7] Institute of Transportation Engineers, “Transportation and traffic engineering handbook,” Prentice Hall, Tech. Rep., 1985. [8] D. Gazis, R. Herman, and A. Maradudin, “The problem with the amber signal light in traffic flow,” Operations Research, vol. 8, no. 1, pp. 112–132, 1960. [9] C. Liu, R. Herman, and D. C. Gazis, “A review of the yellow interval dilemma,” Transportation Research Part A: Policy and Practice, vol. 30, no. 5, pp. 333 – 348, 1996. [10] Y. Liu, G. len Chang, R. Tao, T. Hicks, and E. Tabacek, “Empirical observations of dynamic dilemma zones at signalized intersections,” in Proceedings of 86th TRB Annual Meeting, Washington D.C., 2007. [11] Y. Sheffi and H. Mahmassani, “A model of driver behavior at high speed signalized intersections,” Transportational Science, vol. 15, pp. 50–61, 1981. [12] T. J. Gates, D. A. Noyce, L. Laracuente, and E. V. Nordheim, “Analysis of driver behavior in dilemma zones at signalized intersections,” Transportation Research Record: Journal of the Transportation Research Board, no. 2030, pp. 29–39, 2007. [13] J. C. Medina, M. Chitturi, and R. Benehohal, “Illumination and wind effects on video detection performance at signalized intersections,” in Proceedings of 87th TRB Annual Meeting, Washington D.C., 2008. [14] J. Bonneson, K. Zimmerman, and M. Brewer, “Engineering countermeasures to reduce red-light running,” Texas Transportation Institute, Texas, Federal Highway Administration FHWA/TX03/4027-2, August 2007. [Online]. Available: www.ite.org/library/IntersectionSafety/redlight.pdf

Suggest Documents