Evolutionary background for stress-coping styles: Relationships ...

16 downloads 0 Views 543KB Size Report
dDepartment of Psychology, University of Sussex, Brighton, E Sussex BN1 9QG, UK. eNERC Centre for Ecology ..... in Table 1 with varying speed of progress. The resulting ..... response in aversive trials (e.g. Willig et al., 1991). Similarly, in an ...
ARTICLE IN PRESS

Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 31 (2007) 396–412 www.elsevier.com/locate/neubiorev

Review

Evolutionary background for stress-coping styles: Relationships between physiological, behavioral, and cognitive traits in non-mammalian vertebrates Øyvind Øverlia,b,, Christina Sørensenc, Kim G.T. Pulmand, Tom G. Pottingere, Wayne Korzanf, Cliff H. Summersb, Go¨ran E. Nilssonc Department of Animal and Aquacultural Sciences, Norwegian University of Life Sciences, P.O. Box 5003, N-1432 A˚s, Norway b Biology Department and Neuroscience Group, University of South Dakota, 414 E. Clark Street, Vermillion, SD 57069, USA c Department of Molecular Biosciences, University of Oslo, P.O. Box 1041 Blindern, N-0316 Oslo, Norway d Department of Psychology, University of Sussex, Brighton, E Sussex BN1 9QG, UK e NERC Centre for Ecology and Hydrology Lancaster, Lancaster Environment Centre, Bailrigg, Lancaster LA1 4AP, UK f Department of Biological Sciences, Stanford University, USA

a

Received 25 September 2006; accepted 30 October 2006

Abstract Reactions to stress vary between individuals, and physiological and behavioral responses tend to be associated in distinct suites of correlated traits, often termed stress-coping styles. In mammals, individuals exhibiting divergent stress-coping styles also appear to exhibit intrinsic differences in cognitive processing. A connection between physiology, behavior, and cognition was also recently demonstrated in strains of rainbow trout (Oncorhynchus mykiss) selected for consistently high or low cortisol responses to stress. The low-responsive (LR) strain display longer retention of a conditioned response, and tend to show proactive behaviors such as enhanced aggression, social dominance, and rapid resumption of feed intake after stress. Differences in brain monoamine neurochemistry have also been reported in these lines. In comparative studies, experiments with the lizard Anolis carolinensis reveal connections between monoaminergic activity in limbic structures, proactive behavior in novel environments, and the establishment of social status via agonistic behavior. Together these observations suggest that within-species diversity of physiological, behavioral and cognitive correlates of stress responsiveness is maintained by natural selection throughout the vertebrate sub-phylum. r 2006 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. Keywords: Behavioral syndromes; Brain monoamines; Coping styles; Evolution; Glucocorticoids; Stress

Contents 1. 2.

3.

Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1.1. Individuality of the stress response—a widespread phenomenon . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . The HR–LR trout model . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2.1. Selection program . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2.2. Behavior of HR and LR trout . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2.3. Conditioned ‘emotional’ learning in HR and LR trout . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2.4. Effects of selection for stress responsiveness on brain monoamine neurochemistry in rainbow trout . . . . . . . . . . . . . Observations on hatchery strains of rainbow trout . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .

397 397 398 398 398 399 399 399

Corresponding author. Department of Animal and Aquacultural Sciences, Norwegian University of Life Sciences, P.O. Box 5003, N-1432 A˚s, Norway. Tel.: +47 41506222; fax: +47 47 64965101. E-mail address: [email protected] (Ø. Øverli).

0149-7634/$ - see front matter r 2006 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.10.006

ARTICLE IN PRESS Ø. Øverli et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 31 (2007) 396–412

4.

5.

6.

3.1. Feeding behavior as an indicator of stress-coping style . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 3.2. Boldness, hunger, or learning? . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . The Anolis carolinensis model . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4.1. General description of Anolis behavior . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4.2. Predicting social dominance . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4.3. Opponent recognition . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Observations in other species, synthesis, and discussion. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 5.1. Stress and cognition . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 5.2. Mechanisms behind variation in stress responsiveness. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 5.2.1. Cortisol production and interrenal sensitivity to adrenocorticotrophic hormone . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 5.2.2. Brain neurotransmitter systems . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 5.2.3. Neurogenesis and neural plasticity . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Conclusions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Acknowledgements . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .

1. Introduction 1.1. Individuality of the stress response—a widespread phenomenon Few terms are as popular and frequently used in biology, medicine and psychology as ‘‘stress’’. The very definition of ‘‘stress’’ as a biological term, however, has a long history of controversies. Many of these controversies stem from the fact that what is stressful to one person, or animal, is not always stressful for other members of the same species. It is now recognized that stress coping is of fundamental importance to fitness and quality of life in a number of species (Cavigelli and McClintock, 2003; Janczak et al., 2003; Korte et al., 2005), including humans (e.g. Susman et al., 1999). In fact, understanding individual differences in coping ability has become a paramount task in biological psychiatry and stress research (see e.g. Bartolomucci et al., 2005; Korte et al., 2005). Behavioral responses to stress have been described with a variety of terminologies (see e.g. Koolhaas et al., 1999; Wingfield, 2003), which all discriminate between similar basic behavioral characteristics such as response initiation and behavioral flexibility (de Boer and Koolhaas, 2003). A distinction is often made between proactive (active coping, or ‘fight-flight’) and reactive (passive coping, or ‘conservation-withdrawal’) responses. During stress or exposure to elevated levels of glucocorticoid hormones a transition from behavioral activation to inhibition is typically seen with increasing duration or severity of the challenge (Haller et al., 1998; Øverli et al., 2002a; Wingfield, 2003). The threshold at which the shift from an active to a passive behavioral response occurs is subject to great individual variation, as is the period of recovery needed to restore normal behavior. It has been firmly established that individual differences in behavior are associated with differences in the physiological stress response. Koolhaas et al. (1999) promoted the term stress-coping style to describe this phenomenon. These authors defined coping style as ‘‘a coherent set of behavioral

397

399 400 401 401 402 402 403 403 404 404 404 405 406 407 407

and physiological stress responses, which is consistent over time and which is characteristic to a certain group of individuals’’. Koolhaas et al. (1999) reviewed the evidence that two opposing stress-coping styles (proactive and reactive) can be identified in mammals. A proactive stress-coping style is characterized by a high level of active avoidance, aggression, and other actions indicating active attempts to counteract the stressful stimulus. Reactive coping, on the other hand, involves immobility and low levels of aggression. Physiologically, the proactive strategy is associated with low hypothalamus–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis responsiveness, but high sympathetic reactivity, while the opposite is true for reactive coping. A genetic basis for the expression of behavioral and physiological components of individual coping styles has repeatedly been demonstrated (e.g. Driscoll et al., 1998; Lepage et al., 2000; Ellenbroek and Cools, 2002; de Boer et al., 2003; Veenema et al., 2003). Koolhaas et al. (1999), however, point out that genetic and epigenetic factors combine to generate a relatively stable trait characteristic that confers differential adaptation to environmental conditions such as population density, social stability and food availability. Persistence of a given set of characteristics could thus depend on fluctuations in environmental factors (Dingemanse et al., 2004; Ho¨jesjo¨ et al., 2004) as well as frequency-dependent selection. Compared to the literature in mammals, relatively little information is available on the occurrence of different individual stress-coping styles in other animal groups. There is a tendency to focus on behavioral aspects of personality in comparative models, rather than distinct sets of correlated behavioral and physiological traits. Among teleost fish, consistent behavioral patterns that may reflect alternative coping styles have been observed in cichlids, sticklebacks, salmonids, and a variety of tropical species (Huntingford, 1976; Francis, 1990; Wilson et al., 1993; Van Raaij et al., 1996; Budaev, 1997; Budaev et al., 1999; Iguchi et al., 2001; Bell and Stamps, 2004; Ward et al., 2004; Brelin et al., 2005; Brown et al., 2005; Huntingford and

ARTICLE IN PRESS 398

Ø. Øverli et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 31 (2007) 396–412

Adams, 2005; Martins et al., 2005; Schjolden et al., 2005a) Recent evidence suggests that divergent hypothalamus– pituitary–interrenal (HPI) axis reactivity is associated with consistent differences in behavior in rainbow trout (Pottinger and Carrick, 2001a; Øverli et al., 2002b, 2005). The HPI-axis is the teleost equivalent of the mammalian hypothalamus–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis (Mommsen et al., 1999), and in rainbow trout the magnitude of the cortisol response to stress is an individual trait which is stable over time, with a moderate to high degree of heritability (Pottinger and Carrick, 1999). Lines of rainbow trout selected for high (HR) and low (LR) post-stress cortisol levels have been established by T.G. Pottinger at the UK Natural Environment Research Council, Center for Ecology and Hydrology. Work with the HR–LR lines has demonstrated that a heritable association between increased cortisol production, anxiety-like behavior, and altered cognitive function is present in a non-mammalian vertebrate model system. In this paper we also review studies indicating the existence of similar trait associations both in non-selected aquaculture populations of fish (Øverli et al., 2004a, 2006), and in another poikilotherm species, the lizard Anolis carolinensis (Summers et al., 2005a, b; Korzan et al., 2006a, b). Some comparisons with studies on mammals are made, which together suggest that several trait associations are consistent throughout the vertebrate sub-phylum. Finally, we give a brief overview of some of the possible underlying physiological and neuronal mechanisms. 2. The HR–LR trout model 2.1. Selection program The HR and LR rainbow trout lines were initiated in 1996 by repeated stress testing (3 h confinement in 50 L water in groups of 6–7 individuals once monthly) of passive integrated transponder (PIT) tagged 2-year-old rainbow trout (for details see Pottinger and Carrick, 1999). Confinement stress testing of the 1st generation offspring (F1), consisting of 15 HR and 14 LR families each resulting from a unique male–female crossing, were carried out on five different occasions between September 1997 and September 1998, and on five occasions in 1999. A highly significant regression of mid-parent cortisol response on progeny response was seen (estimated r2 [h2] value ¼ 0.41), and the six LR families with the lowest mean cortisol response and the six HR families with the highest mean cortisol response were identified and used in further breeding (Pottinger and Carrick, 1999). At present, studies are being carried out on the F4 and F5 generations of fish. 2.2. Behavior of HR and LR trout The HR–LR trout lines were originally bred for studies aimed at improving performance in aquaculture, but it soon became apparent that they were also a valuable model

for studying the links between physiology and behavior. Adult female F1 generation HR and LR fish were used in experiments investigating the effect of selection for stress responsiveness on behavior and brain monoaminergic activity (Øverli et al., 2001, 2002b). In this experiment, HR and LR trout were transferred from communal holding tanks to isolation in observation tanks. After 6 days in isolation, locomotor activity was quantified while observing each fish first in an undisturbed condition and thereafter during a territorial intruder test. In this test, both HR and LR rainbow trout increased their activity level when the intruder was present (intruders are smaller, previously unfamiliar individuals of the same species that are suddenly introduced in the holding unit of a territorial fish, cf. Ho¨glund et al., 2001). HR trout displayed higher locomotor activity than LR trout in the presence of an intruder, but not while they were undisturbed. Approximately 40% of the LR fish resumed feeding during the 1-week experimental period, while no HR fish ingested any food during this time. Hence, low cortisol production during stress coincided with both a low locomotor response to stress and more rapid reversal of stress-induced anorexia (Øverli et al., 2002b). Behavioral differences between the HR–LR trout lines were also investigated by Pottinger and Carrick (2001a), who observed the outcome of fights for social dominance between juvenile fish from the F2 generation in a series of dyadic encounters. Juvenile rainbow trout, in common with other predominantly stream-resident salmonids (Keenleyside and Yamamoto, 1962; Taylor, 1990; Hutchison and Iwata, 1997), are intensely territorial animals. When introduced simultaneously into an arena they will engage in agonistic activity that results in the establishment of distinct dominance hierarchies (Jo¨nsson et al., 1998; Winberg and Lepage, 1998; Øverli et al., 1999, 2004b). Using position of the fish within the tank, locomotor activity, agonistic behavior, feeding, and plasma cortisol as criteria of social dominance, Pottinger and Carrick (2001a) reported that LR fish became dominant in the majority of HR–LR pairings. The mechanistic basis for the apparent co-selection of competitive ability and stress responsiveness is as yet undetermined. Clearly, both the direct effects of cortisol (DiBattista et al., 2005) and the involvement of central signaling substances such as corticotrophin releasing hormone (CRH) and brain monoamine neurotransmitters (Winberg and Nilsson, 1992, 1993) require further investigation. In summary, some of the features of the HR–LR trout lines suggest that they represent selection for a pro-active/ reactive stress-coping styles, as defined by Koolhaas et al. (1999). It should, however, also be pointed out that the behavior of the HR–LR lines of fish is highly context dependent, and these fish are influenced by factors such as novel environments and group size (Schjolden et al., 2005b, 2006a). It can therefore not yet be ascertained to what degree the behavioral and endocrine profiles of HR and LR rainbow trout correspond to the trait associations identified

ARTICLE IN PRESS Ø. Øverli et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 31 (2007) 396–412

in other animal groups (Steimer et al., 1998; Koolhaas et al., 1999; Bolhuis et al., 2004, 2005; Groothuis and Carere, 2005) or in humans (Bohnen et al., 1991; Sgoifo et al., 2003; Salvador, 2005). 2.3. Conditioned ‘emotional’ learning in HR and LR trout Moreira et al. (2004) performed a classical fear conditioning experiment where groups of HR and LR fish were exposed to a conditioned stimulus (CS, termination of water flow) followed by confinement stress (the unconditioned stimulus, US). After exposure to 18 CS–US pairings, at least 70% of the individuals of both lines acquired a conditioned response (CR, elevation of plasma cortisol levels) upon presentation of the CS. Post-conditioning, the fish were tested by presentation of the CS at weekly intervals with no further reinforcement, and the extinction of the CR in the two lines was compared. The frequency of individuals who retained the conditioned response was greater among LR fish at 14, 21 and 28 days after the last CS–US pairing took place. Importantly, during exposure to the CS in the absence of the US the subsequent levels of cortisol induced by a fear response in the HR and LR lines were not significantly different. It was concluded therefore that the divergence in length of retention was not simply due to divergence in cortisol levels during the extinction phase. To our knowledge, the study by Moreira et al. (2004) is the first to infer a genetic basis for an association between cognitive, neuroendocrine, and behavioral traits in teleost fish. There is, however, some indirect evidence from other studies that this link exists in teleosts. For example, higher fearfulness in guppies (Poecilia reticulata) facilitated both earlier appearance and acquisition of shuttlebox avoidance responses, especially in less exploratory and active fish (Budaev and Zhuikov, 1998). In the same species, Dugatkin and Alfieri (2003) observed a positive relationship between male boldness and a simple associative learning task. It is important to note that there is increasing evidence of functional similarities between the hippocampus and amygdala of mammals and areas of the fish forebrain thought to be their homologues (e.g. Portavella et al., 2002, 2004; Rodriguez et al., 2002; Portavella and Vargas, 2005; Wai et al., 2006). The demonstration that fish from genetically selected strains differentially retain fear responses suggests a much greater level of complexity in teleost forebrain function that is currently accepted.

399

(McGaugh and Roozendaal, 2002). The influence of HPA-axis activation on cognition in mammals has been extensively explored (for reviews see e.g. Roozendaal, 2002; Wolf, 2003). The brain monoamine neurotransmitters serotonin (5-hydroxytryptamine, 5-HT), dopamine (DA) and norepinephrine (NE) have also been shown to modulate learning and memory in a number of different training tasks and contexts (e.g. Meneses, 1998; Liang et al., 2001; Dreher et al., 2002). For instance, there appears to be a link between dopaminergic cortical innervation and the development of cognitive capacities (Nieoullon and Coquerel, 2003), and the magnitude of dopamine release predicts performance in a delayed response task (Phillips et al., 2004). Øverli et al. (2001) described differences in brain monoaminergic function between HR and LR rainbow trout. In this study only a limited number of fish could be sacrificed for analysis of brain neurochemistry (and only post-spawning females were available), hence the results may not give a complete picture of monoamine metabolism in these strains of fish. However, the pattern of stressinduced elevation of brain stem and optic tectum concentrations of 5-HT, NE, DA, and their metabolites, suggested that both synthesis and metabolism of these transmitters were elevated after stress to a larger degree in HR than in LR trout. A divergent pattern was seen in the hypothalamus, where LR fish displayed elevated levels of 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid (5-HIAA, a serotonin metabolite) and 3-methoxy-4-hydroxyphenylglycol (MHPG, an unsulfated norepinephrine metabolite in salmonids). In the telencephalon, LR fish had higher baseline 5-HIAA/5-HT ratios, while there were no significant differences between stressed HR and LR fish. Recently, Schjolden et al. (2006b) found that brain monoamine oxidase activity was similar in HR and LR fish, suggesting that the demonstrated differences in turnover reflects a true difference in functional transmitter release. As in mammals, brain monoamine neurotransmitters are centrally involved in the control of behavioral and endocrine stress responses in teleost fish (Winberg and Nilsson, 1993; Winberg et al., 1997, 2001; Øverli et al., 1998, 1999; Ho¨glund et al., 2001, 2002a, b, 2005a; Clements et al., 2003; Larson et al., 2003; Perreault et al., 2003; Lepage et al., 2005), but the possible role of these signal substances in memory function has not been extensively studied in fish. 3. Observations on hatchery strains of rainbow trout

2.4. Effects of selection for stress responsiveness on brain monoamine neurochemistry in rainbow trout

3.1. Feeding behavior as an indicator of stress-coping style

In mammals, it has repeatedly been shown that signal systems conveying the effects of stress on physiological processes are also involved in behavior and cognitive ability. For example, catecholamines released in the first phase of the stress response are known to promote consolidation and/or storage of novel information

In previous experiments with HR and LR trout (Pottinger and Carrick, 2001a; Øverli et al., 2002b), the most striking differences in behavior between the lines were seen in the locomotor response to territorial intrusion (higher in HR), the tendency to regain feed intake after stress (more rapid in LR), and the ability to gain social

ARTICLE IN PRESS Ø. Øverli et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 31 (2007) 396–412

400

Holding tank > 2 weeks

Feeding test ~ 1 week

Test for social dominance 0.5 -60min

And / Or: Confinement test 30 min Fig. 1. Generalized summary of the experimental approach used to describe stress coping styles in rainbow trout (cf. Øverli et al., 2004a, 2006). Feeding behavior is observed during one week after transfer from group rearing to social isolation, followed by analysis of other trait characteristics such as the physiological and behavioral response to acute confinement stress, or aggressive behavior in pairs.

Table 1 Grading of behavior of individual fish held in isolation, and corresponding point scores in the feeding test (reprinted from Physiology and Behavior) Points

Behavior

0 1 2 3

Fish Fish Fish Fish

does not respond to food eats only pellets that falls directly in front, and does not move to take food moves more than one body length to take food, but returns to original position in aquarium between each food item moves continuously between food items and consumes all food presented

dominance in pairs (higher in LR). Later experiments addressed whether the same trait associations are present also in non-selected hatchery populations of rainbow trout (Øverli et al., 2004a, 2006). It was found that the duration of appetite inhibition after stress predicted social dominance with near 100% certainty, with rapid reversal of stress-induced anorexia being characteristic of individuals likely to become dominant in later contests (Øverli et al., 2004a). The tendency to become dominant did not depend on a size advantage in animals that rapidly resumed feeding. In fact, even if they took food on more occasions, these individuals displayed significantly lower growth rates during the experimental period (Øverli et al., 2004a). This observation is in line with previous reports that high metabolic rates predict social dominance in salmonid fishes (Metcalfe et al., 1995; McCarthy, 2001). The general approach in our studies with hatchery populations of rainbow trout has been to remove individuals from a large homogenous population, and then study the behavior and physiology of these fish in isolation (Øverli et al., 1999, 2004a, b, 2006). The weeklong isolation period, and the fact that all fish come from the median size interval in a larger population, is intended to minimize the effect of previous social interactions. After the acclimation period, fish are subjected to various treatments such as tests of stress responsiveness, or social interaction in pairs. This experimental approach is summarized in Fig. 1. A similar approach is used in studies with the lizard Anolis carolinensis (described in Section 4), but these animals are caught in the wild before being transferred to the laboratory.

In some experiments (Øverli et al., 2006; Sørensen, 2005), feeding behavior was assessed in detail using clearly defined criteria and consequently assigning a score to each fish on each feeding occasion (once daily for 1 week). Feeding behavior was rated according to a 4-step scale following the criteria listed in Table 1, and all points obtained during 1 week were later summed for each individual. All fish would advance through the stages listed in Table 1 with varying speed of progress. The resulting sum of scores would depend on how rapidly fish resumed normal feeding behavior, with high points indicative of quick acclimation to the new environment. Total feeding score was found to correlate strongly to other behavioral and physiological measures. The relationship between feeding score and plasma cortisol levels in undisturbed socially isolated fish is illustrated in Fig. 2 (Ø. Øverli, C. Sørensen, and G.E. Nilsson, unpublished results). Other clear patterns demonstrated that fish that obtained a high total feeding score showed lower brain stem 5-HIAA concentrations, high aggression, and less locomotion during acute confinement stress (Table 2, summary of previously unpublished data and data from Øverli et al., 2006). 3.2. Boldness, hunger, or learning? Obviously, it is not straightforward to adjudge whether fish that start to eat quickly are bolder (i.e., the anxiogenic effect of novelty and transport wear off faster) or simply hungrier in the feeding test (i.e., the anorectic effect of stress wear off faster). Furthermore, boldness and

ARTICLE IN PRESS Ø. Øverli et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 31 (2007) 396–412

motivation to feed are tightly linked through common neuroendocrine signaling systems (see e.g. Bernier and Peter, 2001; Carr, 2002). It should, however, be kept in mind that the rainbow trout used in our studies are poikilotherm animals kept in relatively cold water (5–15 1C). These fish can live for long periods without food, if necessary. Hence, in most situations where active feeding behavior would imply both benefits and potential costs, salmonid fish are more likely to decline feeding than animals with higher body temperature and hence faster metabolism. In fact, anyone who has ever fished for salmonids using prey imitations as lures can subscribe to how easy it is to disturb these fish so that they refuse to open their mouths. In the study by Øverli et al. (1998) it took 1 week to restore normal feeding behavior in previously subordinate fish after they had been removed from their dominant partner. At this point the effect of social position on brain serotonin metabolism had also disappeared (Øverli et al., 1998). Hence, restoration of feeding after stress is likely to reflect downregulation of the physiological stress response, and feeding score would seem to be a very precise indicator of stress-coping style in salmonid fish.

25

Σ feeding points

20

15 y = 0.061x2-2.1341x + 19.533

401

Several other factors, such as differences in metabolic rate, are of course likely to influence feeding behavior. A fish with a relatively high metabolic rate will consume energy resources more rapidly, and physiological hunger signals will take effect. Consider, for instance, the graph in Fig. 2: Fishes with higher levels of the catabolic hormone cortisol generally have low feeding scores, but scores appear to rise again at the high end of the cortisol range. Notably, very few individuals contributed to the high-cortisol part of the curve. Nevertheless, the curvilinear approach (second-order polynomial with y ¼ 0.061x22.1341x+19.533) yielded a much better fit (r2 ¼ 0.78) than a linear trend line (r2 ¼ 0.41), and a runs test for deviation from linearity came out marginally significant (p ¼ 0.047). Hence, with respect to the control of feed intake in rainbow trout, a likely conclusion is that cognitive and emotional aspects of HPI-axis activation can outweigh metabolic effects for extended periods of time. This notion may, however, only be valid up to a certain level of cortisol exposure. As pointed out by Sneddon (2003), an apparent difference in boldness could also come from a difference in learning (i.e. some fish are faster to learn that feeding in the new environment was not associated with danger or other negative experience). Again, the association between stress responsiveness and cognitive abilities recently demonstrated by Moreira et al. (2004) suggests that learning and/or memory retention is important for the expression of alternative stress-coping styles.

4. The Anolis carolinensis model 4.1. General description of Anolis behavior

10

5

R2=0.78

0 0

10 20 30 Plasma cortisol (ng/ml)

40

Fig. 2. Regression between feeding points and resting plasma cortisol in isolated and undisturbed fish.

Wild-caught male Anolis carolinensis lizards have been recruited for many studies investigating physiological responses associated with the establishment of social rank (Summers, 2002; Summers and Greenberg, 1994; Summers et al., 1998, 2005a, b; Korzan et al., 2000, 2002, 2004, 2006a, b; Yang et al., 2001; Plavicki et al., 2004; Ho¨glund et al., 2005b). The study of stress associated with agonistic behavior in A. carolinensis is facilitated by their eyespots.

Table 2 Correlations between feeding score and other behavioral and physiological parameters Correlation tested

R2

p

Direction of regression

Feeding points vs. brain stem [5-HIAA] (socially isolated fish)

0.77

0.002

Feeding points vs. locomotor response (socially isolated fish)

0.24

0.04

Feeding points vs. aggressive acts performed (dominant fish)

0.65

0.005

Negative. Fish with a high feeding score show less baseline activation of brain stem 5-HT neurons after 1 week in isolation. (Ø. Øverli, C. Sørensen, G.E. Nilsson, unpublished data) Negative. Fish with a high feeding score show a moderate locomotor response to acute confinement after 1 week in isolation (Published in Øverli et al., 2006) Positive. Fish with a high feeding score perform more aggressive acts when placed in a dominant social position after 1 week in isolation (Ø. Øverli, C. Sørensen, G.E. Nilsson, unpublished data)

ARTICLE IN PRESS 402

Ø. Øverli et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 31 (2007) 396–412

Table 3 The number of Anolis carolinensis pairs where the future dominant male showed the quickest (or highest) response prior to social interaction (reprinted from Acta Ethologica) Behavior

Winners response first/highest/ longest

Winners response last/lowest/ shortest

Chi-square probability (expected 1:1 ratio)

Feed intake Courtship Aggressive displays, latency Aggressive displays, frequency Eyespot, latency Eyespot, duration

11 11 14 15 12 12

3 3 1 4 0 0

0.03 0.03 0.008 0.01 40.001 40.001

Eyespots are specialized patches of skin located postorbitally to each eye (Hadley and Goldman, 1969; Vaughan and Greenberg, 1987), which darken when the animal is agitated or aggressive. This response has been reported to depend on both sympathetic activation and brain monoaminergic neurotransmission (Summers and Greenberg, 1994; Larson and Summers, 2001; Ho¨glund et al., 2005b). These visual signals are especially utilized when males defend territories against conspecifics (Korzan et al., 2000, 2002). Rival males perceive the presence or absence of this sign stimulus and as a result modify their behavioral output. For instance, Korzan et al. (2000) found that male A. carolinensis showed more aggressive displays towards their own mirror image if the eyespot was masked by green paint. Furthermore, males that had their eyespots permanently darkened by black paint became dominant in paired interactions (Korzan et al., 2002). Hence, manipulation of the eyespot signal can dictate social rank by influencing behavior of the individual viewing the signal. The effect of the sign stimulus is however, not as potent as memory of a recent interaction with a specific opponent (Korzan et al., 2004; Forster et al., 2005; Summers et al., 2005b; see Section 4.3). Not surprisingly, males that express the eyespot signal first become dominant in paired contests when visual cues have not been experimentally manipulated (Summers and Greenberg, 1994; Larson and Summers, 2001). Thus, it would appear that a rapid and large catecholamine response to social challenge is indicative of a pro-active stress-coping style even in lizards. In the study of Larson and Summers (2001) latency to eyespot darkening was delayed by treatment with the serotonin reuptake inhibitor sertraline, which in turn lead to lower social status. Hence, it could be hypothesized that differences in brain serotonergic activity mediating individual differences in behavior and physiology exist prior to and independent of consequent social interaction. Whether such differences predict social rank in Anolis was later tested by Summers et al. (2005a). 4.2. Predicting social dominance Like the rainbow trout, A. carolinensis posses behavioral and physiological characteristics that make it possible to

predict future social standing of an individual prior to agonistic interaction (Summers et al., 2005a; Korzan et al., 2006b). Korzan et al. (2006b) found that males responding to stimuli (such as the presentation of a receptive female or food) with a shorter latency than an opponent will become dominant in the majority of paired interactions (see Table 3 for summary). The fact that the latency to respond to stimuli allows accurate prediction of future dominant status was used by Summers et al. (2005a, c) to study serotonergic and corticosterone responses in males destined to achieve dominant or subordinate social status. These authors found that among future dominant individuals baseline serotonergic activity was reduced in the septum, nucleus accumbens, striatum, medial amygdala, anterior hypothalamus, raphe, and locus coeruleus, but not in the hippocampus, lateral amygdala, preoptic area, substantia nigra, or ventral tegmental area (Fig. 3). In the lizard model, a higher baseline level of circulating corticosterone was found to predict social dominance (Summers et al., 2005c). Corticosterone and serotonergic activity also rise faster in animals that become dominant (Summers et al., 2005c). These authors suggested that even if the permissive effect of glucocorticoids on aggression does not suggest an active role for the hormone, these steroids might be necessary for full expression of aggressive behavior (Summers et al., 2005c). In summary, similar to the rainbow trout, male A. carolinensis exhibit differences in stress-coping strategies that predict future social status. The shorter the latency to respond or adapt to a variety of stimuli reliably predicts behavioral output in terms of aggression and future social dominance. These behavioral predictors of dominance are subserved by more rapid changes in monoamines and plasma hormones during stressful social confrontation. 4.3. Opponent recognition Among male A. carolinensis behavioral output is potently influenced by direct aggression, stereotyped displays of aggressive intent, and visual sign stimuli (Summers and Greenberg, 1994; Korzan et al., 2000, 2002). None of these social signals are, however, as potent as memory of previous opponents (Forster et al., 2005; Summers et al., 2005b). This results in relatively stable dominant–subordinate

ARTICLE IN PRESS Ø. Øverli et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 31 (2007) 396–412 10.0 8.0 6.0

403

Future dominant Future subordinate

5-HIAA/5-HT

0.8

0.6

0.4

0.2

0.0 Hippo Septum NAcc Striatum IAmyg CA3

MeA

POA

AH LH PVN VMH

Raphe

LC

SN

VTA

Fig. 3. Brain serotonergic activity, indexed by the 5-HIAA/5-HT ratio, in various brain parts of Anolis carolinensis lizards grouped by likely future social status as predicted by time to resume feeding after transfer to social isolation (data from Summers et al., 2005a).

relationships, which may last up to a week (Larson and Summers, 2001; Forster et al., 2005), and individual lizards from socially interacting pairs also remember specific opponents when reintroduced after a period of separation. Learning opponent identity and, perhaps, appropriate behavior for specific social settings appears to be influenced by the hippocampus, where NMDA receptors (particularly those that include the NR2B subunit) increase during social interaction (Meyer et al., 2004). This effect is not associated with learned or conditioned defeat, because defeat alone will not inhibit aggressive behavior. Males reintroduced to the same male whose domination created their subordinate status 1, 3 or 7 days earlier, respond with a dramatically reduced aggressive response, and the previous social order is maintained. However, males that lose a first interaction significantly increase their aggressive output 3 days later during a second interaction with a different, but potentially dominant (that is, has recently won during a different paired interaction) opponent (Forster et al., 2005). Although social rank relationships remain relatively stable, this time period is limited. After 10 days, aggressiveness increases between previously paired lizards and approximately half of the social rank relationships are reversed. These data suggest that proactive and reactive strategies are plastic and limited temporally in the lizards; perhaps because their adaptive value may change with time and the neuroendocrine condition of the potential opponent. Most likely, altered behavioral strategies are dependent on changes in neural mechanisms. For instances, elevation in dopaminergic activity may increase aggression (Kramarcy et al., 1984) or shorten latency to attack (Ho¨glund et al., 2001) and increase the propensity for dominant status (Winberg and Nilsson, 1992). Chronically elevated serotonergic activity, on the other hand, inhibits aggression, and may reverse dominant status (Larson and Summers, 2001; Summers et al., 2005b). The plasticity of pro- and reactive

strategies, while rarely seen in the same population, may follow when social rank must occasionally be reassessed. 5. Observations in other species, synthesis, and discussion 5.1. Stress and cognition In both animals and man an increasing amount of evidence suggests that psychological processes associated to how individuals assess a given situation might be equally important factors as the actual physical challenge in determining the severity of the stress response (von Holst, 1998; Ursin and Eriksen, 2004; Eriksen et al., 2005). Expectancy of the outcome of stimuli is one example of a powerful cognitive modulator of the stress response. Furthermore, in several laboratory reared rodent lines it also appears that genetically determined differences in stress-coping characteristics are associated with divergence in cognitive ability (e.g. Castellano et al., 1999; Nguyen et al., 2000; Balogh and Wehner, 2003; Brennan, 2004) and, vice versa, if one selects for variation in cognitive ability, concomitant differences in stress-coping style arise (e.g. Giorgi et al., 2003; Steimer and Driscoll, 2005; Aguilar et al., 2004). In particular, rodents exhibiting behavioral and physiological traits typical of a reactive stress-coping style perform poorly in the acquisition of a conditioned response in aversive trials (e.g. Willig et al., 1991). Similarly, in an unselected population, individual differences in cognitive performance in rodents have been shown to predict stress-induced corticosterone release and mesolimbic levels of brain monoamine neurotransmitters (Tomie et al., 2000). It is well known that chronic stress and exposure to glucocorticoid hormones may impair memory and learning (see e.g. McEwen and Sapolsky, 1995; McEwen, 2000; Roozendaal, 2002; Wolf, 2003 for reviews). However, it has also become widely accepted that corticosteroids are necessary at lower levels to facilitate learning. For example

ARTICLE IN PRESS 404

Ø. Øverli et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 31 (2007) 396–412

excitability of hippocampal neurons, including the ability to generate long-term potentiation (LTP), is regulated biphasically by adrenal steroids (McEwen, 1994). It is far from clear, however, whether the apparent association between stress-coping style and cognition reflect acute effects of circulating glucocorticoids, organizational and structural effects of chronic exposure to different levels of hormones during pre and post-natal development, or inherent differences in brain function. It is, in fact, not even clear to what extent differences in neuroendocrine function is a cause or a consequence of differences in cognitive processing. There is a possible evolutionary implication of the relationship between glucocorticoids, cognition and behavior that is rarely discussed: It has been hypothesized that proactive behavioral responses are maladaptive under repeated, uncontrollable or unpredictable stress (Haller et al., 1998; Wingfield, 2003). Proactive responses should instead be attempted against known controllable stressors where the outcome of the behavior can be predicted. It follows that a reduced capacity to make use of memories of actual events may decrease controllability and predictability, which in turn should increase HPI/HPA-axis activation, and inhibit proactive behavior. The results of Moreira et al. (2004), suggesting a more rapid extinction of conditioned responses in the HR rainbow trout, are clearly in line with this argument. 5.2. Mechanisms behind variation in stress responsiveness In the following, we review some potential mechanisms that may be involved in heritable variation in the stress response. First, we have concentrated on those biochemical pathways where polymorphisms altering gene function already have been related to stress coping or other personality traits in mammalian species, as this provides evidence that mutations in these genes can survive in a population. Thereafter, we the review the possible role of brain structural plasticity and the process of neurogenesis in the association between physiological, behavioral and cognitive traits. 5.2.1. Cortisol production and interrenal sensitivity to adrenocorticotrophic hormone The behavioral differences between HR and LR trout are consistent with some reported effects of corticosteroid hormones in poikilotherms (decreased appetite: Gregory and Wood, 1999; increased locomotor activity: Cash and Holberton, 1999; Øverli et al., 2002a). In fish, as in mammals, these steroids have time-, context-, and dosedependent effects on behavior (Øverli et al., 2002a). Cognitive effects of cortisol have to our knowledge not been reported in teleost fish, but the conserved nature of the central signaling systems in the stress response suggest that such effects are present. Pottinger and Carrick (2001b) reported that stressinduced adrenocorticotrophic hormone (ACTH) levels

are similar in HR and LR fish, but exogenous ACTH elevated plasma cortisol levels to a significantly greater extent in HR than LR fish. It was concluded that interrenal sensitivity to ACTH is different in HR and LR fish. Corticosteroids are synthesized from cholesterol by side chain cleavage mediated by the enzymatic activity of the P450SCC complex located in the inner mitochondrial membrane. Pregnenolone, the product of this cleavage, then undergoes a series of isomerizations and hydroxylations to produce cortisol. In mammals, it has been shown that this synthetic activity depends on the availability of cholesterol in the inner mitochondrial membrane. One of the factors that regulates this availability is the steroidogenic acute regulatory protein (StAR), a sterol transport protein, which allows rapid transport of cholesterol from the outer to the inner mitochondrial membrane following an acute stimulus, thereby making the precursor available to P450SCC. A multitude of genetic polymorphisms are known in this pathway in humans (see e.g. Wedell and Luthman, 1993; Stratakis and Rennert, 1999), which are all connected with debilitations in untreated patients. Polymorphisms in corresponding genes could be present, but have less severe effects, in the tetraploid trout genome. Furthermore, an ACTH receptor promoter polymorphism that results in a lower promoter activity in vitro and lower cortisol secretion in response to ACTH stimulation in vivo was recently reported by Slawik et al. (2004). Hence, the possibility should be explored that differences in stressinduced cortisol concentrations might arise from differences in interrenal function and sensitivity, rather than in central HPI/HPA-axis control. On the other hand, variability in adrenal function does not have to be of genetic origin. For instance, seasonal variations in stressinduced plasma corticosteroid levels have been correlated to alterations in adrenocortical steroidogenic capacity in lizards (Sceloporus undulatus) (Carsia and John-Alder, 2003). 5.2.2. Brain neurotransmitter systems In humans and other mammals it has long been recognized that genetically determined differences in the brain serotonin system are associated with personality and temperament traits, as well as the propensity to develop mood disorders. The most well-known examples are polymorphisms in monoamine oxidase and the serotonin transporter (5-HTT) gene or promoter region (Lesch et al., 1996; Shih et al., 1999; Fernandez et al., 2003). Glucocorticoid hormones regulate the expression of 5-HTT, but the response to these hormones is attenuated in the 5-HTT ’short’ type (Glatz et al., 2003). Thus, it could be proposed that the functional link between 5-HTT polymorphisms and personality traits is, at least in part, dependent on interaction with glucocorticoid hormones. In the HR–LR trout model, it seems likely that other monoamine neurotransmitters are also involved in determining behavioral profile. Evidence for an ascending

ARTICLE IN PRESS Ø. Øverli et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 31 (2007) 396–412

dopaminergic system comparable to that found in mammals was put forward by Rink and Wullimann (2002). It is also highly likely that neuropeptides such as corticotrophin releasing hormone (CRH, or corticotrophin releasing factor, CRF) are involved. In juvenile chinook salmon (Oncorhynchus tshawytscha), intracerebroventricular injections of CRH-induced hyperactivity, an effect that was shown to depend on concurrent 5-HT activation (Clements et al., 2003). Interestingly, CRH administration increases dopamine (DA) concentrations in the dorsal medial hypothalamus of newts (Taricha granulosa) (Lowry et al., 2001). One of the main neurochemical differences between HR and LR rainbow trout is that HR fish respond to stress with increased DA concentrations and turnover in several brain areas, while LR fish do not (Øverli et al., 2001). Increased DA synthesis and release may, however, also be an effect of acute elevations in glucocorticoid concentrations (Barrot et al., 2001). Furthermore, genetically determined differences in DA systems have been reported in several mammalian models (Rots et al., 1996a,b,c; Lecca et al., 2004). Thus, at present it is not known whether differences in DA systems between HR and LR trout are a cause or a consequence of hormone dynamics, but there are interesting parallels to mammalian models, which suggest the presence of evolutionary conserved trait correlations. For instance, a negative relationship between DA reactivity and social competitive ability has been demonstrated in cynomolgus monkeys (Macaca fascicularis) (Morgan et al., 2000). The physiological and behavioral role of brain dopaminergic systems has also been studied in the Anolis model (e.g. Waters et al., 2005; Korzan et al., 2006a). Elevated motivation requires dopaminergic action (Phillips et al., 2003), and aggressive social interaction stimulates dopaminergic as well as serotonergic activity in limbic brain regions (Winberg and Nilsson, 1993; Miczek et al., 2002). Dopaminergic activity in different brain regions appear to influence aggression (Kramarcy et al., 1984; Ho¨glund et al., 2005b), social status (Winberg and Nilsson, 1992), motor activity (Waters et al., 2005), learning, and perhaps most important, motivation and reward (Korzan et al., 2006a). All of these factors influence social interaction, and are integrated as a part of the neural regulation of social stress. During social interactions between male A. carolinensis increased hypothalamic DA, and increased DA in substantia nigra and ventral tegmental areas were associated with increased aggressive behavior and status (Korzan et al., 2006a). Elevated dopaminergic activity is usually associated with greater aggressiveness and elevated social status, but very high physiological levels may even limit aggressive interaction (Ho¨glund et al., 2005b). As with most vertebrates, locomotor activity is associated with dopaminergic activity in striatum of A. carolinensis (Waters et al., 2005). However, during aggressive displays and attacks, changes observed in dopaminergic activity in nuclei associated with motor activity, such as the striatum, seem to be coupled with

405

expression of specific stereotyped movements associated with social communication more than general motor activity (Korzan et al., 2006a). The striatum and part of its ventral continuum, the nucleus accumbens, are believed to be key structures involved in the modulation of stereotypy (Canales and Graybiel, 2000) and motivation (Mogenson et al., 1980) respectively. Increases in DA and its metabolite 3,4-dihydroxyphenylacetic acid (DOPAC) in the nucleus accumbens transpire when Anolis males effectively attain dominant status and may be characteristic of impetus and reward (Korzan et al., 2006a). Effects of social interaction on dopaminergic activity in hippocampal and amygdalar nuclei in Anolis appear consistent with current literature referring to neurochemical changes associated with memory formation (Korzan et al., 2006a). Together these data suggest that the combination of social signal perception, social rank, and behavioral manifestation, but not any one of these factors alone, may subserve the changes in dopaminergic activity associated with formation of dominant and subordinate status. Herein, the DA systems seem to link behavioral responses to stimuli, and control of stimuli influences not only the behavioral response but also the neural machinery involved with these response strategies. Although the focus of our work has been predominantly on the monoamines, other transmitter systems that could well be involved in integrating physiological and behavioral aspects of stress-coping style include gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA) and endorphins. Modulation of the stress response in mammals includes glucocorticoid feedback at the hippocampal and hypothalamic levels (De Kloet et al., 1998) and regulation by neuronal pathways, including the GABAergic inhibitory system (Jessop, 1999). The neurotransmitter GABA, which is the main inhibitory neurotransmitter in the vertebrate central nervous system, inhibits the HPA-axis through its actions on GABA receptors expressed by CRH neurons within the paraventricular nucleus (PVN) of the hypothalamus. Uhart et al. (2004) recently established a link between gene polymorphism in the GABAAa6 receptor subunit and stress responsiveness in humans. Brain CRH neurons also express m opioid receptors and are modulated by b-endorphin neurons originating in the arcuate nucleus. In humans a polymorphism in the extracellular N-terminal domain of m opioid receptor has been identified (Bergen et al., 1997; Wendel and Hoehe, 1998), and a link to altered HPA-axis function has been demonstrated (Wand, 2002; Chong et al., 2006). 5.2.3. Neurogenesis and neural plasticity Plasticity in structural processes in the brain may be highly important for behavior, mood, and cognitive function in both animals and human beings, and is affected by a range of environmental, endocrine, and pharmacological factors (Nilsson et al., 1999; Gould et al., 2000; Jacobs et al., 2000; Fuchs and Flu¨gge, 2001; Garcı´ a-Verdugo et al., 2002; Nottebohm, 2002; Song et al., 2002; Shors, 2004).

ARTICLE IN PRESS 406

Ø. Øverli et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 31 (2007) 396–412

The concept of LTP in the hippocampus is often described as the underpinning of learning, but it is not entirely clear that this form of synaptic plasticity adequately explains the process of ‘natural’ learning (LeDoux, 2000). Recent evidence suggests that other forms of plasticity such as neurogenesis are actually essential for long-term memory formation (e.g. Shors et al., 2001, 2002; Bruel-Jungerman et al., 2005, 2006). Several reports indicate that new neurons are added to continually growing structures and replace old and dying ones in adults of all major vertebrate taxa, a process known as neurogenesis (although see Rakic, 2004 for a counter argument). In mammals, chronic stress, glucocorticoids, aging, and repeated exposure to opiate drugs decrease adult hippocampal neurogenesis (Kuhn et al., 1996; Gould et al., 1997; Kempermann et al., 1998; Eisch et al., 2000), while voluntary exercise, enriched environments, and hippocampal-dependent learning increase neurogenesis (Gould et al., 1999; Nilsson et al., 1999; van Praag et al., 1999; Ambrogini et al., 2000; Olson et al., 2006). Increased adult neurogenesis also correlates with improved performance in hippocampal-dependent learning tasks (Nilsson et al., 1999; van Praag et al., 1999). These results have received a great deal of attention due to their implicit conclusion that more hippocampal neurons equal better memory function. Whether adult neurogenesis truly generates new nerve cells remains debated, although, in their review, Kempermann et al. (2004) submit ample evidence for function and contribution within cognitive processes. Reduced brain cell proliferation has also repeatedly been linked to depression and depression-like behavioral patterns (see e.g. Duman et al., 1999; Jacobs et al., 2000; Kempermann and Kronenberg, 2003). Perhaps even more importantly, an oftenoverlooked phenomenon is the discovery of adult neurogenesis in other brain structures such as the amygdala (e.g. Bernier et al., 2002; Fowler et al., 2002), which is the seat of ‘emotional’ function and fear underpinning much of the stress-coping behavior described in this review. Compared to mammals, teleost fish and other nonmammalian vertebrates have a much higher degree of adult neurogenesis (Zupanc, 2001). The size of a teleost brain increases with age, body weight and body length throughout life, and adult proliferation has been observed within all major teleost brain structures (Birse et al., 1980; Leyhausen et al., 1987; Brandstatter and Kotrschal, 1990; Zupanc and Horschke, 1995). Adult proliferation zones have been mapped in detail in the brains of the adult stickleback (Gasterosteus aculeatus) (Ekstro¨m et al., 2001), zebrafish (Zupanc et al., 2005), and gilthead sea bream (Sparus aurata) (Zikopoulos et al., 2000). Compared to the number of anatomical and developmental studies in teleost fish, surprisingly little is known about the functional significance of adult neurogenesis. Ekstro¨m et al. (2001) noted a certain degree of interindividual variation in the labeling of proliferating cells in sticklebacks. They did not, however, investigate this further in their study, and called for a systematic exploration of

this issue (Ekstro¨m et al., 2001). Sørensen (2005) recently showed that brain cell proliferation was reduced in socially subordinate rainbow trout, as compared to socially isolated and undisturbed controls. Similar observations have been made in mammals (Gould et al., 1997). It is not known, however, whether reduced brain cell proliferation is an effect of a general inhibition in growth rate, an effect of stress, or a persistent feature predisposing certain individuals for a subordinate social position and reactive behavior. The above results warrant investigating whether rates of brain cell proliferation and neurogenesis differ between HR and LR trout lines, or predict social status in non-selected populations. Divergence in the rate or total amount of structural changes in key brain areas may well explain apparent differences in cognitive ability irrespective of the mechanisms that lead to this divergent neuronal plasticity. The proteins needed for cell proliferation and cell fate determination are often highly conserved through evolution, and one would not expect to find functional polymorphisms in their genes that can be linked to altered stress responsiveness. Several systems with a regulatory role in cell proliferation, including the brain serotonergic system, are, on the other hand, likely to show such polymorphism. Notably, Veenema et al. (2004) recently demonstrated that there are several differences in HPA-axis responsiveness, 5-HT responsiveness, and hippocampal cell proliferation in mouse lines selected for divergence in aggressive behavior. 6. Conclusions Both genetic and environmental factors (e.g. social interactions and previous exposure to stress) contribute to extensive inter-individual variation in how stress affects behavior and physiology (Winberg and Nilsson, 1993; Koolhaas et al., 1999; Blanchard et al., 2001; Summers, 2002; Bartels et al., 2003; Sapolsky, 2003; Sgoifo et al., 2003; Entringer et al., 2004; Wust et al., 2004a, b; Korte et al., 2005). However, the existence of similar trait associations in the HR–LR strains and non-selected aquaculture populations of rainbow trout (Øverli et al., 2004b, 2006), and the apparent parallel between stresscoping styles in fish, the Anolis lizard model and mammals, suggests that suites of correlated physiological and behavioral traits have been conserved during evolution. In other species variation in the physiological stress response is associated with a number of major axes of variation in behavior, including activity, shyness–boldness, and aggression (Korte et al., 1992, 2005; Koolhaas et al., 1999; van der Vegt et al., 2001; Cavigelli and McClintock, 2003; Popova et al., 2005). Such behavioral measures have all been identified as major personality traits in animals across a wide range of taxa (see Gosling and John, 1999; Gosling, 2001). It certainly cannot be concluded at this stage whether this reflects basic homology of integrative systems or parallel but independent evolution but the

ARTICLE IN PRESS Ø. Øverli et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 31 (2007) 396–412

weight of evidence from structural similarities, histochemical, biochemical, immunohistochemical and functional studies supports the latter. Considering the tight links between neuroendocrine signaling systems controlling physiological, emotional, and behavioral responses, and taking into account that behaviors can not be performed or emotions experienced without simultaneous physiological activation, one must conclude that behavioral syndromes observed in behavioral ecology (Sih et al., 2004), stress-coping style (Koolhaas et al., 1999), and psychological and behavioral components of personality (Gosling, 2001) may all be reflections of the same phenomenon—individuality in the stress response. The fact that stress-coping characteristics depend on genes as well as experience (and can even be transferred between parents and offspring by a multitude of non-genetic mechanisms) is therefore relevant to the interpretation of results from all biological research on live animals, and may provide a common conceptual framework uniting subjects as seemingly different as speciation and personality psychology. Acknowledgements Financial support came from the Norwegian Research Council, National Institutes of Health (USA), and the Natural Environment Research Council (NERC) of the United Kingdom. The authors wish to thank Dr. Kendra G. Bowman for her considerate revisal of assorted versions of this manuscript. References Aguilar, R., Gil, L., Ferna´ndez-Teruel, A., Toben˜a, A., 2004. Geneticallybased behavioral traits influence the effects of Shuttle Box avoidance overtraining and extinction upon intertrial responding: a study with the Roman rat strains. Behavioural Processes 66, 63–72. Ambrogini, P., Cuppini, R., Cuppini, C., Ciaroni, S., Cecchini, T., Ferri, P., Sartini, S., Del Grande, P., 2000. Spatial learning affects immature granule cell survival in adult rat dentate gyrus. Neuroscience Letters 286, 21–24. Balogh, S.A., Wehner, J.M., 2003. Inbred mouse strain differences in the establishment of long-term fear memory. Behavioural Brain Research 140, 97–106. Barrot, M., Abrous, D.N., Marinelli, M., Rouge´-Pont, F., Le Moal, M., Piazza, P.V., 2001. Influence of glucocorticoids on dopaminergic transmission in the rat dorsolateral striatum. European Journal of Neuroscience 13, 812–818. Bartels, M., Van den Berg, M., Sluyter, F., Boomsma, D.I., de Geus, E.J.C., 2003. Heritability of cortisol levels: review and simultaneous analysis of twin studies. Psychoneuroendocrinology 28, 121–137. Bartolomucci, A., Palanza, P., Sacerdote, P., Panerai, A.E., Sgoifo, A., Dantzer, R., Parmigiani, S., 2005. Social factors and individual vulnerability to chronic stress exposure. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 29, 67–81. Bell, A.M., Stamps, J.A., 2004. Development of behavioural differences between individuals and populations of sticklebacks, Gasterosteus aculeatus. Animal Behaviour 68, 1339–1348. Bergen, A.W., Peterson, R., Kokoszka, J., Long, J.C., Virkkunen, M., Linnoila, M., Goldman, D., 1997. m opioid receptor gene variants: lack

407

of association with alcohol dependence. Molecular Psychiatry 2, 490–494. Bernier, N.J., Peter, R.E., 2001. The hypothalamic–pituitary–interrenal axis and the control of food intake in teleost fish. Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology B 129, 639–644. Birse, S.C., Leonard, R.B., Coggeshall, R.E., 1980. Neuronal increase in various areas of the nervous system of the guppy Lebistes. Journal of Comparative Neurology 194, 291–301. Blanchard, R.J., McKittrick, C.R., Blanchard, D.C., 2001. Animal models of social stress: effects on behavior and brain neurochemical systems. Physiology and Behavior 73, 261–271. de Boer, S.F., Koolhaas, J.M., 2003. Defensive burying in rodents: ethology, neurobiology and psychopharmacology. European Journal of Pharmacology 463, 145–161. de Boer, S.F., van der Vegt, B.J., Koolhaas, J.M., 2003. Individual variation in aggression of feral rodent strains: a standard for the genetics of aggression and violence? Behavioural Genetics 33, 485–501. Bohnen, N., Nicolson, N., Sulon, J., Jolles, J., 1991. Coping style, trait anxiety and cortisol reactivity during mental stress. Journal of Psychosomatic Research 35, 141–147. Bolhuis, J.E., Schouten, W.G.P., de Leeuw, J.A., Schrama, J.W., Wiegant, V.A., 2004. Individual coping characteristics, rearing conditions and behavioural flexibility in pigs. Behavioural Brain Research 152, 351–360. Bolhuis, J.E., Schouten, W.G.P., Schrama, J.W., Wiegant, V.M., 2005. Individual coping characteristics, aggressiveness and fighting strategies in pigs. Animal Behaviour 69, 1085–1091. Brandstatter, R., Kotrschal, K., 1990. Brain growth patterns in four European cyprinid fish species (Cyprinidae, Teleostei): roach (Rutilus rutilus), bream (Abramis brama), common carp (Cyprinus carpio) and saber carp (Pelecus cultratus). Brain Behavior and Evolution 35, 195–211. Brelin, D., Petersson, E., Winberg, S., 2005. Divergent stress coping styles in juvenile brown trout (Salmo trutta). Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1040, 239–245. Brennan, F.X., 2004. Genetic differences in leverpress escape/avoidance conditioning in seven mouse strains. Genes Brain and Behavior 3, 110–114. Brown, C., Jones, F., Braithwaite, V., 2005. In situ examination of boldness-shyness traits in the tropical poeciliid, Brachyraphis episcopi. Animal Behaviour 70, 1003–1009. Bruel-Jungerman, E., Laroche, S., Rampon, C., 2005. New neurons in the dentate gyrus are involved in the expression of enhanced long-term memory following environmental enrichment. European Journal of Neuroscience 21, 513–521. Bruel-Jungerman, E., Davis, S., Rampon, C., Laroche, S., 2006. Longterm potentiation enhances neurogenesis in the adult dentate gyrus. Journal of Neuroscience 26, 5888–5893. Budaev, S.V., 1997. ‘‘Personality’’ in the guppy (Poecilia reticulata): a correlational study of exploratory behavior and social tendency. Journal of Comparative Psychology 111, 399–411. Budaev, S.V., Zhuikov, A.Y., 1998. Avoidance learning and ‘‘personality’’ in the guppy (Poecilia reticulata). Journal of Comparative Psychology 112, 92–94. Budaev, S.V., Zworykin, D.D., Mochek, A.D., 1999. Consistency of individual differences in behaviour of the lion-headed cichlid, Steatocranus casuarius. Behavioural Processes 48, 49–55. Canales, J.J., Graybiel, A.M., 2000. A measure of striatal function predicts motor stereotypy. Nature Neuroscience 3, 377–383. Carr, J.A., 2002. Stress, neuropeptides, and feeding behavior: a comparative perspective. Integrative and Comparative Biology 42, 582–590. Carsia, R.V., John-Alder, H., 2003. Seasonal alterations in adrenocortical cell function associated with stress-responsiveness and sex in the eastern fence lizard (Sceloporus undulatus). Hormones and Behavior 43, 408–420. Cash, W.B., Holberton, R.L., 1999. Effects of exogenous corticosterone on locomotor activity in the red-eared slider turtle, Trachemys scripta elegans. Journal of Experimental Zoology 284, 637–644.

ARTICLE IN PRESS 408

Ø. Øverli et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 31 (2007) 396–412

Castellano, C., Cabib, S., Puglisi-Allegra, S., Gasbarri, A., Sulli, A., Pacitti, C., Introini-Collison, I.B., McGaugh, J.L., 1999. Straindependent involvement of D1 and D2 dopamine receptors in muscarinic cholinergic influences on memory storage. Behavioural Brain Research 98, 17–26. Cavigelli, S.A., McClintock, M.K., 2003. Fear of novelty in infant rats predicts adult corticosterone dynamics and an early death. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 100, 16131–16136. Chong, R.Y., Oswald, L., Yang, X.J., Uhart, M., Lin, P.I., Wand, G.S., 2006. The m opioid receptor polymorphism A118G predicts cortisol responses to naloxone and stress. Neuropsychopharmacology 31, 204–211. Clements, S., Moore, F.L., Schreck, C.B., 2003. Evidence that acute serotonergic activation potentiates the locomotor-stimulating effects of corticotropin-releasing hormone in juvenile chinook salmon (Oncorhynchus tshawytscha). Hormones and Behavior 43, 214–221. DiBattista, J.D., Anisman, H., Whitehead, M., Gilmour, K.M., 2005. The effects of cortisol administration on social status and brain monoaminergic activity in rainbow trout Oncorhynchus mykiss. Journal of Experimental Biology 208, 2707–2718. Dingemanse, N.J., Both, C., Drent, P.J., Tinbergen, J.M., 2004. Fitness consequences of avian personalities in a fluctuating environment. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London 271B, 847–852. Dreher, J.C., Guigon, E., Burnod, Y., 2002. A model of prefrontal cortex dopaminergic modulation during the delayed alternation task. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 14, 853–865. Driscoll, P., Escorihuela, R.M., Fernande´z-Teruel, A., Giorgi, O., Schwegler, H., Steimer, T., Wiersma, A., Corda, M.G., Flint, J., Koolhaas, J.M., Langhans, W., Schulz, P.E., Siegel, J., Tobena, A., 1998. Genetic selection and differential stress responses—the Roman lines/strains of rats. In: Stress of Life, vol. 851. pp. 501–510. Dugatkin, L.A., Alfieri, M.S., 2003. Boldness, behavioral inhibition and learning. Ethology Ecology & Evolution 15, 43–49. Duman, R.S., Malberg, J., Thome, J., 1999. Neural plasticity to stress and antidepressant treatment. Biological Psychiatry 46, 1181–1191. Eisch, A.J., Barrot, M., Schad, C.A., Self, D.W., Nestler, E.J., 2000. Opiates inhibit neurogenesis in the adult rat hippocampus. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 97, 7579–7584. Ekstro¨m, P., Johnsson, C.M., Ohlin, L.M., 2001. Ventricular proliferation zones in the brain of an adult teleost fish and their relation to neuromeres and migration (secondary matrix) zones. Journal of Comparative Neurology 436, 92–110. Ellenbroek, B.A., Cools, A.R., 2002. Apomorphine susceptibility and animal models for psychopathology: genes and environment. Behavior Genetics 32, 349–361. Entringer, S., Federenko, I., Hellhammer, D.H., Kirschbaum, C., Bartels, M., Schlotz, W., Wust, S., 2004. Impact of genetic factors on the variability of perceived stress. Journal of Psychophysiology 18, 229. Eriksen, H.R., Murison, R., Pensgaard, A.M., Ursin, H., 2005. Cognitive activation theory of stress (CATS): from fish brains to the Olympics. Psychoneuroendocrinology 30, 933–938. Fernandez, F., Sarre, S., Launay, J.M., Aguerre, S., Guyonnet-Dupe´rat, V., Moisan, M.P., Ebinger, G., Michotte, Y., Morme`de, P., Chaouloff, F., 2003. Rat strain differences in peripheral and central serotonin transporter protein expression and function. European Journal of Neuroscience 17, 494–506. Forster, G.L., Watt, M.J., Korzan, W.J., Renner, K.J., Summers, C.H., 2005. Opponent recognition in male green anoles, Anolis carolinensis. Animal Behaviour 69, 733–740. Francis, R.C., 1990. Temperament in a fish: a longitudinal-study of the development of individual-differences in aggression and social rank in the midas cichlid. Ethology 86, 311–325. Fuchs, E., Flu¨gge, G., 2001. Psychosocial stress induces molecular and structural alterations in the brain—how animal experiments help to understand pathomechanisms of depressive illnesses. Zeitschrift Fur Psychosomatische Medizin Und Psychotherapie 47, 80–97. Garcı´ a-Verdugo, J.M., Ferro´n, S., Flames, N., Collado, L., Desfilis, E., Font, E., 2002. The proliferative ventricular zone in adult vertebrates:

a comparative study using reptiles, birds, and mammals. Brain Research Bulletin 57, 765–775. Giorgi, O., Lecca, D., Piras, G., Driscoll, P., Corda, M.G., 2003. Dissociation between mesocortical dopamine release and fear-related behaviours in two psychogenetically selected lines of rats that differ in coping strategies to aversive conditions. European Journal of Neuroscience 17, 2716–2726. Glatz, K., Mossner, R., Heils, A., Lesch, K.P., 2003. Glucocorticoidregulated human serotonin transporter (5-HTT) expression is modulated by the 5-HTT gene-promotor-linked polymorphic region. Journal of Neurochemistry 86, 1072–1078. Gosling, S.D., 2001. From mice to men: What can we learn about personality from animal research? Psychology Bulletin 127, 45–86. Gosling, S.D., John, O.P., 1999. Personality dimensions in nonhuman animals: a cross-species review. Current Directions in Psychological Science 8, 69–75. Gould, E., McEwen, B.S., Tanapat, P., Galea, L.A.M., Fuchs, E., 1997. Neurogenesis in the dentate gyrus of the adult tree shrew is regulated by psychosocial stress and NMDA receptor activation. Journal of Neuroscience 17, 2492–2498. Gould, E., Beylin, A., Tanapat, P., Reeves, A., Shors, T.J., 1999. Learning enhances adult neurogenesis in the hippocampal formation. Nature Neuroscience 2, 260–265. Gould, E., Tanapat, P., Rydel, T., Hastings, N., 2000. Regulation of hippocampal neurogenesis in adulthood. Biological Psychiatry 48, 715–720. Gregory, T.R., Wood, C.M., 1999. The effects of chronic plasma cortisol elevation on the feeding behaviour, growth, competitive ability, and swimming performance of juvenile rainbow trout. Physiological and Biochemical Zoology 72, 286–295. Groothuis, T.G.G., Carere, C., 2005. Avian personalities: characterization and epigenesis. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 29, 137–150. Hadley, M., Goldman, J., 1969. Physiological color changes in reptiles. American Zoology 9, 489–504. Haller, J., Halasz, J., Makara, G.B., Kruk, M.R., 1998. Acute effects of glucocorticoids: behavioral and pharmacological perspectives. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 23, 337–344. Ho¨glund, E., Kolm, N., Winberg, S., 2001. Stress-induced changes in brain serotonergic activity, plasma cortisol and aggressive behavior in Arctic charr (Salvelinus alpinus) is counteracted by L-DOPA. Physiology and Behavior 74, 381–389. Ho¨glund, E., Balm, P.H.M., Winberg, S., 2002a. Behavioural and neuroendocrine effects of environmental background colour and social interaction in Arctic charr (Salvelinus alpinus). Journal of Experimental Biology 205, 2535–2543. Ho¨glund, E., Balm, P.H.M., Winberg, S., 2002b. Stimulatory and inhibitory effects of 5-HT1A receptors on adrenocorticotropic hormone and cortisol secretion in a teleost fish, the Arctic charr (Salvelinus alpinus). Neuroscience Letters 324, 193–196. Ho¨glund, E., Bakke, M.J., Øverli, Ø., Winberg, S., Nilsson, G.E., 2005a. Suppression of aggressive behaviour in juvenile Atlantic cod (Gadus morhua) by L-tryptophan supplementation. Aquaculture 249, 525–531. Ho¨glund, E., Korzan, W.J., Watt, M.J., Forster, G.L., Summers, T.R., Johannessen, H.F., Renner, K.J., Summers, C.H., 2005b. Effects Of LDOPA on aggressive behavior and central monoaminergic activity in the lizard Anolis carolinensis, using a new method for drug delivery. Behavioural Brain Research 156, 53–64. Ho¨jesjo¨, J., Johnsson, J., Bohlin, T., 2004. Habitat complexity reduces the growth of aggressive and dominant brown trout (Salmo trutta) relative to subordinates. Behavioural Ecology and Sociobiology 56, 286–289. von Holst, D., 1998. The concept of stress and its relevance for animal behavior. Advances in the Study of Behaviour 27, 1–131. Huntingford, F.A., 1976. Relationship between anti-predator behavior and aggression among conspecifics in three-spined stickleback, Gasterosteus aculeatus. Animal Behaviour 24, 245–260. Huntingford, F., Adams, C., 2005. Behavioural syndromes in farmed fish: Implications for production and welfare. Behaviour 142, 1207–1221.

ARTICLE IN PRESS Ø. Øverli et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 31 (2007) 396–412 Hutchison, M.J., Iwata, M., 1997. A comparative analysis of aggression in migratory and non-migratory salmonids. Environmental Biology of Fishes 50, 209–215. Iguchi, K., Matsubara, N., Hakoyama, H., 2001. Behavioural individuality assessed from two strains of cloned fish. Animal Behaviour 61, 351–356. Jacobs, B.L., van Praag, H., Gage, F.H., 2000. Adult brain neurogenesis and psychiatry: a novel theory of depression. Molecular Psychiatry 5, 262–269. Janczak, A.M., Pedersen, L.J., Bakken, M., 2003. Aggression, fearfulness and coping styles in female pigs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 81, 13–28. Jessop, D.S., 1999. Central non-glucocorticoid inhibitors of the hypothalamo–pituitary–adrenal axis. Journal of Endocrinology 160, 169–180. Jo¨nsson, E., Johnsson, J.I., Bjo¨rnsson, B.T., 1998. Growth hormone increases aggressive behavior in juvenile rainbow trout. Hormones and Behavior 33, 9–15. Keenleyside, M.H.A., Yamamoto, F.T., 1962. Territorial behaviour of juvenile Atlantic salmon (Salmo salar L.). Behaviour 19, 139–169. Kempermann, G., Kronenberg, G., 2003. Depressed new neurons—adult hippocampal neurogenesis and a cellular plasticity hypothesis of major depression. Biological Psychiatry 54, 499–503. Kempermann, G., Kuhn, H.G., Gage, F.H., 1998. Experience-induced neurogenesis in the senescent dentate gyrus. Journal of Neuroscience 18, 3206–3212. Kempermann, G., Wiskott, L., Gage, F.H., 2004. Functional significance of adult neurogenesis. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 14, 186–191. De Kloet, E.R., Vreugdenhil, E., Oitzl, M.S., Joels, M., 1998. Brain corticosteroid receptor balance in health and disease. Endocrine Reviews 19, 269–301. Koolhaas, J.M., Korte, S.M., De Boer, S.F., Van Der Vegt, B.J., Van Reenen, C.G., Hopster, H., De Jong, I.C., Ruis, M.A.W., Blokhuis, H.J., 1999. Coping styles in animals: current status in behavior and stress-physiology. Neuroscience and Biobehavioural Reviews. 23, 925–935. Korte, S.M., Buwalda, B., Bouws, G.A.H., Koolhaas, J.M., Maes, F.W., Bohus, B., 1992. Conditioned neuroendocrine and cardiovascular stress responsiveness accompanying behavioral passivity and activity in aged and in young rats. Physiology and Behavior 51, 815–822. Korte, S.M., Koolhaas, J.M., Wingfield, J.C., McEwen, B.S., 2005. The Darwinian concept of stress: benefits of allostasis and costs of allostatic load and the trade-offs in health and disease. Neuroscience and Biobehavioural Reviews 29, 3–38. Korzan, W.J., Summers, T.R., Ronan, P.J., Summers, C.H., 2000. Visible sympathetic activity as a social signal in Anolis carolinensis: changes in aggression and plasma catecholamines. Hormones and Behavior 38, 193–199. Korzan, W.J., Summers, T.R., Summers, C.H., 2002. Manipulation of visual sympathetic sign stimulus modifies social status and plasma catecholamines. General and Comparative Endocrinology 128, 153–161. Korzan, W.J., Forster, G. L., Watt, M. J., Øverli, Ø., Ho¨glund, E., Renner, K.J. and Summers, C.H., 2004. Influence of a second interaction after establishment of social rank, on hippocampal and raphe monoaminergic activity. Society of Neuroscience Abstracts 30, 760.20. Korzan, W.J., Forster, G.L., Watt, M.J., Summers, C.H., 2006a. Dopaminergic activity modulation via aggression, status, and a visual social signal. Behavioural Neuroscience 120, 93–102. Korzan, W.J., Øverli, Ø., Summers, C.H., 2006b. Future social rank: Forecasting status in the green anole (Anolis carolinensis). Acta Ethology, in press. Kramarcy, N.R., Brown, J.W., Thurmond, J.B., 1984. Effects of druginduced changes in brain monoamines on aggression and motor behavior in mice. European Journal of Pharmacology 99, 141–151. Kuhn, H.G., Dickinson-Anson, H., Gage, F.H., 1996. Neurogenesis in the dentate gyrus of the adult rat: Age-related decrease of neuronal progenitor proliferation. Journal of Neuroscience 16, 2027–2033.

409

Larson, E.T., Summers, C.H., 2001. Serotonin reverses dominant social status. Behavioural Brain Research 121, 95–102. Larson, E.T., Norris, D.O., Summers, C.H., 2003. Monoaminergic changes associated with socially induced sex reversal in the saddleback wrasse. Neuroscience 119, 251–263. Lecca, D., Piras, G., Driscoll, P., Giorgi, O., Corda, M.G., 2004. A differential activation of dopamine output in the shell and core of the nucleus accumbens is associated with the motor responses to addictive drugs: a brain dialysis study in Roman high- and low-avoidance rats. Neuropharmacology 46, 688–699. LeDoux, J.E., 2000. Emotion circuits in the brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience 23, 155–184. Lepage, O., Øverli, Ø., Petersson, E., Ja¨rvi, T., Winberg, S., 2000. Differential stress coping in wild and domesticated sea trout. Brain Behavior and Evolution 56, 259–268. Lepage, O., Larson, E.T., Mayer, I., Winberg, S., 2005. Serotonin, but not melatonin, plays a role in shaping dominant–subordinate relationships and aggression in rainbow trout. Hormones and Behavior 48, 233–242. Lesch, K.P., Bengel, D., Heils, A., Sabol, S.Z., Greenberg, B.D., Petri, S., Benjamin, J., Muller, C.R., Hamer, D.H., Murphy, D.L., 1996. Association of anxiety-related traits with a polymorphism in the serotonin transporter gene regulatory region. Science 274, 1527–1531. Leyhausen, C., Kirschbaum, F., Szabo, T., Erdelen, M., 1987. Differential growth in the brain of the weakly electric fish, Apteronotus oeptorhynchus (Gymnotiformes), during ontogeny. Brain Behavior and Evolution 30, 230–248. Liang, K.C., Chen, H.C., Chen, D.Y., 2001. Posttraining infusion of norepinephrine and corticotropin releasing factor into the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis enhanced retention in an inhibitory avoidance task. Chinese Journal of Physiology 44, 33–43. Lowry, C.A., Burke, K.A., Renner, K.J., Moore, F.L., Orchinik, M., 2001. Rapid changes in monoamine levels following administration of corticotropin-releasing factor or corticosterone are localized in the dorsomedial hypothalamus. Hormones and Behavior 39, 195–205. Martins, C.I.M., Schrama, J.W., Verreth, J.A.J., 2005. The consistency of individual differences in growth, feed efficiency and feeding behaviour in African catfish Clarias gariepinus (Burchell 1822) housed individually. Aquaculture Research 36, 1509–1516. McCarthy, I.D., 2001. Competitive ability is related to metabolic asymmetry in juvenile rainbow trout. Journal of Fish Biology 59, 1002–1014. McEwen, B.S., 1994. Corticosteroids and hippocampal plasticity. Annals of New York Academy of Sciences 746, 134–142. McEwen, B.S., 2000. The neurobiology of stress: from serendipity to clinical relevance. Brain Research 886, 172–189. McEwen, B.S., Sapolsky, R.M., 1995. Stress and cognitive function. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 5, 205–216. McGaugh, J.L., Roozendaal, B., 2002. Role of adrenal stress hormones in forming lasting memories in the brain. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 12, 205–210. Meneses, A., 1998. Physiological, pathophysiological and therapeutic roles of 5-HT systems in learning and memory. Reviews in the Neurosciences 9, 275–289. Metcalfe, N.B., Taylor, A.C., Thorpe, J.E., 1995. Metabolic rate, socialstatus and life-history strategies in Atlantic salmon. Animal Behaviour 49, 431–436. Meyer, W.N., Keifer, J., Korzan, W.J., Summers, C.H., 2004. Social stress and corticosterone regionally upregulate limbic N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor (NR) subunit type NR2A and NR2B in the lizard Anolis carolinensis. Neuroscience 128, 675–684. Miczek, K.A., Fish, E.W., de Bold, J.F., de Almeida, R.M.M., 2002. Social and neural determinants of aggressive behavior: pharmacotherapeutic targets at serotonin, dopamine and gamma-aminobutyric acid systems. Psychopharmacology 163, 434–458. Mogenson, G.J., Jones, D.L., Yim, C.Y., 1980. From motivation to action: functional interface between the limbic system and the motor system. Progress in Neurobiology 14, 69–97.

ARTICLE IN PRESS 410

Ø. Øverli et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 31 (2007) 396–412

Mommsen, T.P., Vijayan, M.M., Moon, T.W., 1999. Cortisol in teleosts: dynamics, mechanisms of action, and metabolic regulation. Reviews in Fish Biology and Fisheries 9, 211–268. Moreira, P.S.A., Pulman, K.G.T., Pottinger, T.G., 2004. Extinction of a conditioned response in rainbow trout selected for high or low responsiveness to stress. Hormones and Behavior 46, 450–457. Morgan, D., Grant, K.A., Prioleau, O.A., Nader, S.H., Kaplan, J.R., Nader, M.A., 2000. Predictors of social status in cynomolgus monkeys (Macaca fascicularis) after group formation. American Journal of Primatology 52, 115–131. Nguyen, P.V., Abel, T., Kandel, E.R., Bourtchouladze, R., 2000. Straindependent differences in LTP and hippocampus-dependent memory in inbred mice. Learning and Memory 7, 170–179. Nieoullon, A., Coquerel, A., 2003. Dopamine: a key regulator to adapt action, emotion, motivation and cognition. Current Opinion in Neurology 16, S3–S9. Nilsson, M., Perfilieva, E., Johansson, U., Orwar, O., Eriksson, P.S., 1999. Enriched environment increases neurogenesis in the adult rat dentate gyrus and improves spatial memory. Journal of Neurobiology 39, 569–578. Nottebohm, F., 2002. Neuronal replacement in adult brain. Brain Research Bulletin 57, 737–749. Olson, A.K., Eadie, B.D., Ernst, C., Christie, B.R., 2006. Environmental enrichment and voluntary exercise massively increase neurogenesis in the adult hippocampus via dissociable pathways. Hippocampus 16, 250–260. Øverli, Ø., Winberg, S., Damsga˚rd, B., Jobling, M., 1998. Food intake and spontaneous swimming activity in Arctic char (Salvelinus alpinus): role of brain serotonergic activity and social interactions. Canadian Journal of Zoology 76, 1366–1370. Øverli, Ø., Harris, C.A., Winberg, S., 1999. Short-term effects of fights for social dominance and the establishment of dominant-subordinate relationships on brain monoamines and cortisol in rainbow trout. Brain Behavior and Evolution 54, 263–275. Øverli, Ø., Pottinger, T.G., Carrick, T.R., Øverli, E., Winberg, S., 2001. Brain monoaminergic activity in rainbow trout selected for high and low stress responsiveness. Brain Behavior and Evolution. 57, 214–224. Øverli, Ø., Kotzian, S., Winberg, S., 2002a. Effects of cortisol on aggression and locomotor activity in rainbow trout. Hormones and Behavior 42, 53–61. Øverli, Ø., Pottinger, T.G., Carrick, T.R., Øverli, E., Winberg, S., 2002b. Differences in behaviour between rainbow trout selected for high- and low-stress responsiveness. Journal of Experimental Biology 205, 391–395. Øverli, Ø., Korzan, W.J., Ho¨glund, E., Winberg, S., Bollig, H., Watt, M., Forster, G.L., Barton, B.A., Øverli, E., Renner, K.J., Summers, C.H., 2004a. Stress coping style predicts aggression and social dominance in rainbow trout. Hormones and Behavior 45, 235–241. Øverli, Ø., Korzan, W.J., Larson, E.T., Winberg, S., Lepage, O., Pottinger, T.G., Renner, K.J., Summers, C.H., 2004b. Behavioral and neuroendocrine correlates of displaced aggression in trout. Hormones and Behavior 45, 324–329. Øverli, Ø., Winberg, S., Pottinger, T.G., 2005. Behavioral and neuroendocrine correlates of selection for stress responsiveness in rainbow trout—a review. Integrative and Comparative Biology 45, 463–474. Øverli, Ø., Sørensen, C., Nilsson, G.E., 2006. Behavioral indicators of stress-coping style in rainbow trout: do males and females react differently to novelty? Physiology & Behavior 87, 506–512. Perreault, H.A.N., Semsar, K., Godwin, J., 2003. Fluoxetine treatment decreases territorial aggression in a coral reef fish. Physiology & Behavior 79, 719–724. Phillips, A.G., Ahn, S., Howland, J.G., 2003. Amygdalar control of the mesocorticolimbic dopamine system: parallel pathways to motivated behavior. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 27, 543–554. Phillips, A.G., Ahn, S., Floresco, S.B., 2004. Magnitude of dopamine release in medial prefrontal cortex predicts accuracy of memory on a delayed response task. Journal of Neuroscience 24, 547–553.

Plavicki, Yang, E.J., Wilczynski, W., 2004. Dominance status predicts response to nonsocial forced movement stress in the green anole lizard (Anolis carolinensis). Physiology & Behavior 80, 547–555. Popova, N.K., Naumenko, V.S., Plyusnina, I.Z., Kulikov, A.V., 2005. Reduction in 5-HT1A receptor density, 5-HT1A mRNA expression, and functional correlates for 5-HT1A receptors in genetically defined aggressive rats. Journal of Neuroscience Research 80, 286–292. Portavella, M., Vargas, J.P., 2005. Emotional and spatial learning in goldfish is dependent on different telencephalic pallial systems. European Journal of Neuroscience 21, 2800–2806. Portavella, M., Vargas, J.P., Torres, B., Salas, C., 2002. The effects of telencephalic pallial lesions on spatial, temporal, and emotional learning in goldfish. Brain Research Bulletin 57, 397–399. Portavella, M., Torres, B., Salas, C., 2004. Avoidance response in goldfish: emotional and temporal involvement of medial and lateral telencephalic pallium. Journal of Neuroscience 24, 2335–2342. Pottinger, T.G., Carrick, T.R., 1999. Modification of the plasma cortisol response to stress in rainbow trout by selective breeding. General and Comparative Endocrinology 116, 122–132. Pottinger, T.G., Carrick, T.R., 2001a. Stress responsiveness affects dominant–subordinate relationships in rainbow trout. Hormones and Behavior 40, 419–427. Pottinger, T.G., Carrick, T.R., 2001b. ACTH does not mediate divergent stress responsiveness in rainbow trout. Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology 129A, 399–404. van Praag, H., Christie, B.R., Sejnowski, T.J., Gage, F.H., 1999. Running enhances neurogenesis, learning, and long-term potentiation in mice. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 13427–13431. van Raaij, M.T.M., Pit, D.S.S., Balm, P.H.M., Steffens, A.B., van den Thillart, G.E.E.J.M., 1996. Behavioral strategy and the physiological stress response in rainbow trout exposed to severe hypoxia. Hormones and Behavior 30, 85–92. Rakic, P., 2004. Neuroscience—immigration denied. Nature 427, 685–686. Rink, E., Wullimann, M.F., 2002. Connections of the ventral telencephalon and tyrosine hydroxylase distribution in the zebrafish brain (Danio rerio) lead to identification of an ascending dopaminergic system in a teleost. Brain Research Bulletin 57, 385–387. Roozendaal, B., 2002. Stress and memory: opposing effects of glucocorticoids on memory consolidation and memory retrieval. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 78, 578–595. Rots, N.Y., Cools, A.R., Be´rod, A., Voorn, P., Roste`ne, W., De Kloet, E.R., 1996a. Rats bred for enhanced apomorphine susceptibility have elevated tyrosine hydroxylase mRNA and dopamine D2-receptor binding sites in nigrostriatal and tuberoinfundibular dopamine systems. Brain Research 710, 189–196. Rots, N.Y., Cools, A.R., Oitzl, M.S., de Jong, J., Sutanto, W., De Kloet, E.R., 1996b. Divergent prolactin and pituitary-adrenal activity in rats selectively bred for different dopamine responsiveness. Endocrinology 137, 1678–1686. Rots, N.Y., Workel, J., Oitzl, M.S., Be´rod, A., Roste`ne, W., Cools, A.R., De Kloet, E.R., 1996c. Development of divergence in dopamine responsiveness in genetically selected rat lines is preceded by changes in pituitary-adrenal activity. Developmental Brain Research 92, 164–171. Salvador, A., 2005. Coping with competitive situations in humans. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 29, 195–205. Sapolsky, R.M., 2003. Stress and plasticity in the limbic system. Neurochemical Research 28, 1735–1742. Schjolden, J., Stoskhus, A., Winberg, S., 2005a. Does individual variation in stress responses and agonistic behavior reflect divergent stress coping strategies in juvenile rainbow trout? Physiological and Biochemical Zoology 78, 715–723. Schjolden, J., Ba¨ckstrom, T., Pulman, K.G.T., Pottinger, T.G., Winberg, S., 2005b. Divergence in behavioural responses to stress in two strains of rainbow trout (Oncorhynchus mykiss) with contrasting stress responsiveness. Hormones and Behavior 48, 537–544. Schjolden, J., Pulman, K.G.T., Pottinger, T.G., Metcalfe, N.B., Winberg, S., 2006a. Divergence in locomotor activity between two strains of

ARTICLE IN PRESS Ø. Øverli et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 31 (2007) 396–412 rainbow trout Oncorhynchus mykiss with contrasting stress responsiveness. Journal of Fish Biology 68, 920–924. Schjolden, J., Pulman, K.G.T., Pottinger, T.G., Tottmar, O., Winberg, S., 2006b. Serotonergic characteristics of rainbow trout divergent in stress responsiveness. Physiology and Behavior 87, 938–947. Sgoifo, A., Braglia, F., Costoli, T., Musso, E., Meerlo, P., Ceresini, G., Troisi, A., 2003. Cardiac autonomic reactivity and salivary cortisol in men and women exposed to social stressors: relationship with individual ethological profile. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 27, 179–188. Shih, J.C., Chen, K., Ridd, M.J., 1999. Monoamine oxidase: from genes to behavior. Annual Review of Neuroscience 22, 197–217. Shors, T.J., 2004. Memory traces of trace memories: neurogenesis, synaptogenesis and awareness. Techniques in Neurosurgery 27, 250–256. Shors, T.J., Miesegaes, G., Beylin, A., Zhao, M., Rydel, T., Gould, E., 2001. Neurogenesis in the adult is involved in the formation of trace memories. Nature 410, 372–376. Shors, T.J., Townsend, D.A., Zhao, M., Kozorovitskiy, Y., Gould, E., 2002. Neurogenesis may relate to some but not all types of hippocampal-dependent learning. Hippocampus 12, 578–584. Sih, A., Bell, A.M., Johnson, J.C., Ziemba, R.E., 2004. Behavioral syndromes: an integrative overview. Quarterly Review of Biology 79, 241–277. Slawik, M., Reisch, N., Zwermann, O., Maser-Gluth, C., Stahl, M., Klink, A., Reincke, M., Beuschlein, F., 2004. Characterization of an adrenocorticotropin (ACTH) receptor promoter polymorphism leading to decreased adrenal responsiveness to ACTH. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism 89, 3131–3137. Sneddon, L.U., 2003. The bold and the shy: individual differences in rainbow trout. Journal of Fish Biology 62, 971–975. Song, H.J., Stevens, C.F., Gage, F.H., 2002. Neural stem cells from adult hippocampus develop essential properties of functional CNS neurons. Nature Neuroscience 5, 438–445. Sørensen, C., 2005. Stressful social interaction reduces telencephalic cell proliferation in rainbow trout, MSc Thesis, University of Oslo. Steimer, T., Driscoll, P., 2005. Inter-individual vs line/strain differences in psychogenetically selected Roman High-(RHA) and Low-(RLA) Avoidance rats: neuroendocrine and behavioural aspects. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 29, 99–112. Steimer, T., Escorihuela, R.M., Fernandez-Teruel, A., Driscoll, P., 1998. Long-term behavioural and neuroendocrine changes in Roman high(RHA/Verh) and low-(RLA-Verh) avoidance rats following neonatal handling. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience 16, 165–174. Stratakis, C.A., Rennert, O.M., 1999. Congenital adrenal hyperplasia: molecular genetics and alternative approaches to treatment. Critical Reviews in Clinical Laboratory Sciences 36, 329–363. Summers, C.H., 2002. Social interaction over time, implications for stress responsiveness. Integrative and Comparative Biology 42, 591–599. Summers, C.H., Greenberg, N., 1994. Somatic correlates of adrenergic activity during aggression in the lizard Anolis carolinensis. Hormones and Behavior 28, 29–40. Summers, C.H., Larson, E.T., Summers, T.R., Renner, K.J., Greenberg, N., 1998. Regional and temporal separation of serotonergic activity mediating social stress. Neuroscience 87, 489–496. Summers, C.H., Korzan, W.J., Lukkes, J.L., Watt, M.J., Forster, G.L., Øverli, Ø., Ho¨glund, E., Larson, E.T., Ronan, P.J., Matter, J.M., Summers, T.R., Renner, K.J., Greenberg, N., 2005a. Does serotonin influence aggression? Comparing regional activity before and during social interaction. Physiological and Biochemical Zoology 78, 679–694. Summers, C.H., Forster, G.L., Korzan, W.J., Watt, M.J., Larson, E., Øverli, Ø., Ho¨glund, E., Ronan, P.J., Summers, T.R., Renner, K.J., Greenberg, N., 2005b. Dynamics and mechanics of social rank reversal. Journal of Comparative Physiology 191A, 241–252. Summers, C.H., Watt, M.J., Ling, T.L., Forster, G.L., Carpenter, R.E., Korzan, W.J., Lukkes, J.L., Øverli, Ø., 2005c. Glucocorticoid

411

interaction with aggression in non-mammalian vertebrates: reciprocal action. European Journal of Pharmacology 526, 21–35. Susman, E.J., Schmeelk, K.H., Worrall, B.K., Granger, D.A., Ponirakis, A., Chrousos, G.P., 1999. Corticotropin-releasing hormone and cortisol: Longitudinal associations with depression and antisocial behavior in pregnant adolescents. Journal of the Amercian Academy of Child Adolescence Psychiatry 38, 460–467. Taylor, E.B., 1990. Phenotypic correlates of life-history variation in juvenile chinook salmon, Oncorhynchus tshawytscha. Journal of Animal Ecology 59, 455–468. Tomie, A., Aguado, A.S., Pohorecky, L.A., Benjamin, D., 2000. Individual differences in Pavlovian autoshaping of lever pressing in rats predict stress-induced corticosterone release and mesolimbic levels of monoamines. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior 65, 509–517. Uhart, M., McCaul, M.E., Oswald, L.M., Choi, L., Wand, G.S., 2004. GABRA6 gene polymorphism and an attenuated stress response. Molecular Psychiatry 9, 998–1006. Ursin, H., Eriksen, H.R., 2004. The cognitive activation theory of stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology 29, 567–592. Vaughan, G.L., Greenberg, N., 1987. Propranolol, a b-adrenergic antagonist, retards response to MSH in skin of Anolis carolinensis. Physiology and Behavior 40, 555–558. Veenema, A.H., Meijer, O.C., de Kloet, E.R., Koolhaas, J.M., 2003. Genetic selection for coping style predicts stressor susceptibility. Journal of Neuroendocrinology 15, 256–267. Veenema, A.H., Koolhaas, J.M., De Kloet, E.R., 2004. Basal and stressinduced differences in HPA axis, 5-HT responsiveness, and hippocampal cell proliferation in two mouse lines. Annals of New York Academy of Science 1018, 255–265. van der Vegt, B.J., de Boer, S.F., Buwalda, B., de Ruiter, A.J.H., de Jong, J.G., Koolhaas, J.M., 2001. Enhanced sensitivity of postsynaptic serotonin-1A receptors in rats and mice with high trait aggression. Physiology and Behavior 74, 205–211. Wai, M.S., Lorke, D.E., Webb, S.E., Yew, D.T., 2006. The pattern of c-fos activation in the cns is related to behavior in the mudskipper, Periophthalmus cantonensis. Behavioural Brain Research 167, 318–327. Wand, G.S., McCaul, M., Yang, X.J., Reynolds, J., Gotjen, D., Lee, S., Ali, A., 2002. The Mu-opioid receptor gene polymorphism (A118G) alters HPA axis activation induced by opioid receptor blockade. Neuropsychopharmacology 26, 106–114. Ward, A.J.W., Thomas, P., Hart, P.J.B., Krause, J., 2004. Correlates of boldness in three-spined sticklebacks (Gasterosteus aculeatus). Behavioural Ecology and Sociobiology 55, 561–568. Waters, R.P., Emerson, A.J., Watt, M.J., Forster, G.L., Swallow, J.G., Summers, C.H., 2005. Stress induces rapid changes in central catecholaminergic activity in Anolis carolinensis: restraint and forced physical activity. Brain Research Bulletin 67, 210–218. Wedell, A., Luthman, H., 1993. Steroid 21-hydroxylase deficiency: two additional mutations in salt-wasting disease and rapid screening of disease-causing mutations. Human Molecular Genetics 2, 499–504. Wendel, B., Hoehe, M.R., 1998. The human mu opioid receptor gene: 5 ’ regulatory and intronic sequences. Journal of Molecular Medicine 76, 525–532. Willig, F., Mharzi, M., Delacour, J., 1991. Contribution of the Roman strains of rats to the elaboration of animal models of memory. Physiology and Behavior 50, 913–919. Wilson, D.S., Coleman, K., Clark, A.B., Biederman, L., 1993. Shy bold continuum in pumpkinseed sunfish (Lepomis gibbosus): an ecological study of a psychological trait. Journal of Comparative Psychology 107, 250–260. Winberg, S., Lepage, O., 1998. Elevation of brain 5-HT activity, POMC expression, and plasma cortisol in socially subordinate rainbow trout. American Journal of Physiology 43R, 645–654. Winberg, S., Nilsson, G.E., 1992. Induction of social dominance by L-dopa treatment in Arctic charr. Neuroreport 3, 243–246. Winberg, S., Nilsson, G.E., 1993. Roles of brain monoamine neurotransmitters in agonistic behavior and stress reactions, with particular

ARTICLE IN PRESS 412

Ø. Øverli et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 31 (2007) 396–412

reference to fish. Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology 106C, 597–614. Winberg, S., Nilsson, A., Hylland, P., So¨derstro¨m, V., Nilsson, G.E., 1997. Serotonin as a regulator of hypothalamic–pituitary–interrenal activity in teleost fish. Neuroscience Letters 230, 113–116. Winberg, S., Øverli, Ø., Lepage, O., 2001. Suppression of aggression in rainbow trout (Oncorhynchus mykiss) by dietary L-tryptophan. Journal of Experimental Biology 204, 3867–3876. Wingfield, J.C., 2003. Control of behavioural strategies for capricious environments. Animal Behaviour 66, 807–815. Wolf, O.T., 2003. HPA axis and memory. Best Practice and Research in Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism 17, 287–299. Wust, S., Federenko, I.S., van Rossum, E.F.C., Koper, J.W., Hellhammer, D.H., 2004a. Adrenocortical responses to repeated psychosocial stress: further characterization and impact of genetic factors. Journal of Psychophysiology 18, 230. Wust, S., van Rossum, E.F.C., Federenko, I.S., Koper, J.W., Kumsta, R., Hellhammer, D.H., 2004b. Common polymorphisms in the glucocorticoid receptor gene are associated with adrenocortical responses to

psychosocial stress. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism 89, 565–573. Yang, E.J., Phelps, S.M., Crews, D., Wilczynski, W., 2001. The effects of social experience on aggressive behavior in the green anole lizard (Anolis carolinensis). Ethology 107, 777–793. Zikopoulos, B., Kentouri, M., Dermon, C.R., 2000. Proliferation zones in the adult brain of a sequential hermaphrodite teleost species (Sparus aurata). Brain Behavior and Evolution 56, 310–322. Zupanc, G.K.H., 2001. Adult neurogenesis and neuronal regeneration in the central nervous system of teleost fish. Brain Behavior and Evolution 58, 250–275. Zupanc, G.K.H., Horschke, I., 1995. Proliferation zones in the brain of adult gymnotiform fish—a quantitative mapping study. Journal of Comparative Neurology 353, 213–233. Zupanc, G.K.H., Hinsch, K., Gage, F.H., 2005. Proliferation, migration, neuronal differentiation, and long-term survival of new cells in the adult zebrafish brain. Journal of Comparative Neurology 488, 290–319.