CYBERPSYCHOLOGY, BEHAVIOR, AND SOCIAL NETWORKING Volume 15, Number 7, 2012 ª Mary Ann Liebert, Inc. DOI: 10.1089/cyber.2012.0121
Explaining the Use of Text-Based Communication Media: An Examination of Three Theories of Media Use Namkee Park, Ph.D.,1 Jae Eun Chung, Ph.D.,2 and Seungyoon Lee, Ph.D.3
Abstract
The present study examined the factors associated with individuals’ use of three different text-based communication media: e-mail, cell-phone texting, and Facebook Wall postings. Three theoretical perspectives, including media richness theory, uses and gratifications, and perceived network effects, were examined. Using data from a survey of college students (N = 280), the study found that the theoretical constructs from these theories play different roles when applied to different technologies. The results suggest that a simultaneous consideration of technological attributes, users’ motivations, and social circumstances in which users select and use the technology is useful for fully understanding the dynamics of the selection and the use of a given technology.
Introduction
R
ecent years have witnessed rapid innovations in communication technologies, encouraging communication researchers to revisit the fundamental question about why people select and use specific communication technologies in a given manner. The technology options currently available for communication are quite different from those of the early Internet era. Furthermore, current technologies offer much more sophisticated functions and diverse features. Early efforts that explain people’s technology selection and use employed the social presence model and media richness theory and focused on the attributes of the technology.1,2 Later research, however, shifted attention to individuals’ social surrounding and motivations as factors explaining the use of new communication technologies.3–5 For example, the social influence model6–8 proposed that technology use is affected by social settings and decisions made by close relationships. The uses and gratifications approach9,10 focused on the active role of individuals in seeking the satisfaction of their needs by the use of the technology in question. Studies conducted on technology selection and use have largely focused on a single theory. We propose that a fuller understanding of individuals’ technology selection and use requires a simultaneous examination of multiple theories, considering that today’s technologies offer more diverse functions and serve multiple roles. In particular, we consider both media attributes and individual factors, along with social circumstances surrounding technology selection and use. To this end, three different sets of theories are examined: (a) the
social presence model and media richness theory; (b) the uses and gratification approach; and (c) theory of network effects. In the current study, we focus on three popular text-based communication technologies: e-mail, cell-phone texting, and Facebook Wall postings. Although all three are based on text communication, each of these technologies presents unique characteristics as a communication platform. We aim at investigating the differences among the three technologies in their use, resulting from their respective characteristics in media richness, motivation, and social influence. E-mail has been one of the earliest and most popular applications on the Internet. It is a prototype of computer-mediated communication media that lacks nonverbal cues and synchronicity. Cell phones are characterized as a ubiquitous, faster, and more convenient medium.11 In particular, text messaging (or texting) has become increasingly popular, especially among young users, surpassing voice calling, face-to-face talk, and e-mail in terms of frequency of use.12–14 Cell phone users both send and receive a greater number of text messages than voice calls, and also about 30 percent of text users reported that they prefer texting over voice calling.15 Lastly, Facebook Wall postings offer a unique platform of communication in which the comments posted by one’s friends can be viewed by other users as well as the profile owner, facilitating the sharing of messages among one’s circle of social connections.16 While abundant literature has examined various uses of these communication channels, a few have simultaneously explained the determinants of uses based on multiple theoretical frameworks. We take this as an opportunity to empirically test the relative importance of the aforementioned theories.
1
Gaylord College of Journalism and Mass Communication, University of Oklahoma, Norman, Oklahoma. School of Communication Studies, Kent State University, Kent, Ohio. Brian Lamb School of Communication, Purdue University, West Lafayette, Indiana.
2 3
357
358 The social presence model and media richness theory The social presence model1 posits that communication technologies vary in their attribute of social presence, ‘‘the feeling that other actors are jointly involved in communicative interactions.’’1(p65) According to the model, communication technologies can be arrayed along a continuum from low to high social presence, and users select a specific medium that they perceive to have the highest social presence.17–19 Similarly, the media richness theory2 proposes that individuals distinguish communication technologies from ‘‘lean’’ to ‘‘rich’’ based on the technologies’ intrinsic properties.20,21 Specifically, four factors affect media richness: (a) transmission of multiple cues (e.g., vocal inflection, gestures); (b) immediacy of feedback; (c) language variety; and (d) the personal focus. The media richness theory claims that individuals seek to match the richness of a medium with the complexity of the task for which it is used.3 In empirical studies, face-to-face interaction was preferred for ambiguous tasks or messages, whereas written or electronic media were preferred for unambiguous communications.20 The ability to match the richness of a medium to the communicated messages was also found to be a valued communication skill in organizations.20,22 Several studies reported results that are inconsistent with the media richness theory’s predictions.5,23 For example, Markus5 uncovered that electronic mail, a lean medium, was used for communication tasks that involved high degrees of ambiguity. Faced with these contrasting findings, Fulk and Boyd4 emphasized the need to clarify the dimensions of media richness and to explain the core processes that drive the perception of media richness. They also noted that the media richness theory is more supportive of traditional media rather than new media.4,5 Nevertheless, the following reasons necessitate the reexamination of the social presence model and the media richness theory as frameworks for understanding media use behaviors. First, today’s communication technologies provide unique features and include new attributes that were not tested in previous studies. In particular, in order to fully understand the attributes of new technologies and their impacts on individuals’ media use, three technologies that operate on three unique platforms and applications are selected for investigation. By limiting the investigation to text-based communication, the current study seeks to explore the variations in media use that result from the attributes of media platforms. For example, individuals’ use of Facebook Wall postings can be uniquely affected by media attributes compared with that of e-mail or texting, because its medium, Facebook, may limit the immediacy of feedback and the privacy, or personal focus, of exchanged messages. Second, the social presence model and media richness theory have been primarily tested in organizational contexts. Thus, the current study retests them in the context of interpersonal communication: RQ1: How does the media richness of communication technologies (e-mail, cell-phone texting, and Facebook Wall posting) affect people’s use of those media?
Uses and gratifications As a reaction to an effects-driven, passive audience model, the uses and gratifications approach emphasizes active roles
PARK ET AL. of audiences in media choice and use.24,25 This approach posits that individual differences in media selection and consumption originate from their specific needs and motivations.26 Examining the motivations behind adoption and use is particularly important for new technologies.27 A new technology that can neither satisfy needs already fulfilled by old technologies nor create novel needs will not survive. The success of a new technology is contingent on the adoption and utilization of users, rather than its own attributes. For instance, a newly invented device that features a variety of complicated functions may fail if its potential adopters do not see the unique benefits out of its use and are not motivated to try it out. The uses and gratifications framework has been extensively employed to explore individuals’ motivations for using a specific medium. The three communication technologies under scrutiny in this study are no exception. Stafford et al.28 found that the main reasons behind e-mail use are interpersonal communication, personal gain, and business reasons. In terms of cell-phone use, Wei and Lo29 identified six motivations: information seeking, social utility, affection, fashion status, mobility, and accessibility. Lastly, for the use of social networking sites, including Facebook, Raacke and Bonds-Raacke30 and Joinson31 identified motivations such as social connection, shared identities, sharing photographs, content gratifications, social investigation, social network surfing, and status updating. Although previous studies have identified motivations for using the three technologies under scrutiny, the extent to which motivations differentially affect use behaviors in each technology has been largely undocumented. Individuals’ motivations may play a stronger role for a certain medium compared with others in that the use of easy-to-access and spontaneous medium (such as texting) can be less constrained by situational and technical factors and more directly affected by individuals’ needs for communication. In the current study, we investigate the three most popular text-based technologies for interpersonal communication and compare motivations that drive their use: RQ2: How do individual users’ motivations affect their use of communication technologies (e-mail, cell-phone texting, and Facebook Wall posting)?
Theory of network effects There has been an increasing emphasis on social influence as a determinant of technology use. Rogers32 suggested perceived social norms as a measure of one’s perceptions about others’ expectations influencing the diffusion of innovation. In organizational settings, social influence has been proposed as being an important predictor of technology use.6–8 Schmitz and Fulk8 found that the use of e-mail by supervisors and coworkers and the extent to which they think of it as useful influenced the focal actor’s use of e-mail. Given that most of today’s communication technologies exhibit network externalities (i.e., if everybody else is using a certain technology, I also need to use it in order to communicate with others), this study extends the theory of network effects to the context of interpersonal text-based communication. From an economic perspective, the utility from consuming a good increases with the number of other users.33 In other words, the willingness to adopt a new technology positively
USE OF TEXT-BASED COMMUNICATION MEDIA
359
Table 1. Factor Analysis for Motivations Factor loading E-mail Statements Factor 1: Strong-tie communication I use e-mail (cell-phone texting, Facebook Wall posting) to feel closer to friends. I use e-mail (cell-phone texting, Facebook Wall posting) to improve relations with friends. I use e-mail (cell-phone texting, Facebook Wall posting) to let others know I care for them. I use e-mail (cell-phone texting, Facebook Wall posting) to feel or express caring. I use e-mail (cell-phone texting, Facebook Wall posting) to show others encouragement. I use e-mail (cell-phone texting, Facebook Wall posting) for fun or pleasure of communicating. Factor 2: Weak-tie communication I use e-mail (cell-phone texting, Facebook Wall posting) to keep in contact with people I do not have enough time to see in person. I use e-mail (cell-phone texting, Facebook Wall posting) for coordinating social events with people I know. I use e-mail (cell-phone texting, Facebook Wall posting) to keep in contact with others who live far away. I use e-mail (cell-phone texting, Facebook Wall posting) to keep in touch with people. I use e-mail (cell-phone texting, Facebook Wall posting) to give and receive information with people I know. Eigenvalue Cronbach’s alpha
correlates with the number of existing adopters.34 Communication technologies, in particular, cannot be studied in isolation from their users’ social environment. The social network approach has been also used to examine the behavior of the focal person and their social surroundings. Especially, the notion of personal network threshold captures the idea that individuals decide to engage in a behavior such as adopting an innovation depending on the proportion of previous adopters in the social system.35 In the study of network effects, several studies34,36 have adopted perceptions about the popularity of a given technology as a measure of social network effects and found that such perceptions affects individuals’ adoption and use of the new technology. Communication technologies, social media in particular, are increasingly used in the context of social systems. Unlike e-mail and text messaging, which are one-toone interpersonal communication, Facebook Wall postings facilitate interpersonal communication in the presence of a larger number of social relations. However, a few studies have examined the varying influences of network effects on the use of new communication technologies. The current study proposes a closer examination into such varying influences: RQ3: How do perceived network effects affect people’s use of communication technologies (e-mail, cell-phone texting, and Facebook Wall posting)?
1
Cell-phone texting 2
1
2
Facebook Wall 1
0.883
0.861
0.785
0.857
0.845
0.735
0.840
0.833
0.849
0.806
0.790
0.769
0.782
0.792
0.781
0.740
0.696
0.471
6.32 0.93
2
0.731
0.873
0.801
0.721
0.607
0.615
0.688
0.843
0.855
0.636
0.689
0.700
0.598
0.589
0.619
1.20 0.82
6.40 0.93
1.25 0.85
5.87 0.89
1.12 0.82
Method Survey administration An online survey was conducted in a Southwestern public university in the United States in April 2011. From the university-issued directory of all students, 1,500 were randomly selected. An e-mail invitation including a link to the survey on SurveyMonkey (www.surveymonkey.com) was sent. The study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the university, and the information sheet for the consent to participate was provided in the second page of the survey questionnaire to the selected students. After the initial invitation, two follow-up reminders were sent. Of the 1,500 students, 414 (27.6 percent) completed the survey and received a $5 credit to their on-campus spending account. Measures Media richness was measured with eight statements modified from Daft and Lengel,37 including ‘‘E-mail (cell-phone texting/Facebook Wall posting) helps me communicate quickly with my communication partner(s), giving and receiving timely feedback’’ and ‘‘With e-mail (cell-phone texting/Facebook Wall posting), I can easily deliver a variety of different cues beyond written messages’’ (a = 0.70, 0.78, and 0.80 for e-mail, cell-phone texting, and Facebook Wall
360
PARK ET AL.
FIG. 1. Mean comparisons of four independent variables by each medium. posting, respectively). Each statement was anchored by a 5-point Likert scale ranging from ‘‘strongly disagree (1)’’ to ‘‘strongly agree (7)’’ (Other variables were anchored by the same scale unless otherwise indicated). Perceived network effect was measured with six statements modified from Slyke et al.,38 including ‘‘Of the people I communicate with regularly, many use e-mail (cell-phone texting/Facebook Wall posting)’’ and ‘‘People whose opinion I value think I should use e-mail (cell-phone texting/Facebook Wall posting)’’ (a = 0.87, 0.86, and 0.89 for e-mail, cellphone texting, and Facebook Wall posting, respectively). Motivations were measured with 11 statements that were gleaned and modified from past studies and factor analyzed. By using an orthogonal Varimax rotation, two types of motivations were extracted—strong-tie communication and weak-tie communication. Factor loadings, eigenvalues, and reliability are presented in Table 1. Technology use was measured with the following question: ‘‘On average, how much time do you spend on e-mail (cellphone texting/Facebook Wall posting) in a typical day?’’ Additionally, age and sex were included as control variables. Results Descriptive statistics Among 414 participants, 280 provided valid responses; the remaining completed less than half of the survey. There were 189 female (67.5 percent) and 88 male participants (31.4 per-
cent), while 3 did not indicate their sex. The average age of the participants was 22.54 (SD = 5.75). The ethnic composition of the sample was as follows: 70.7 percent Whites, 3.2 percent African Americans, 6.8 percent Hispanics, 5.0 percent American Indians/Alaskan Natives, and 8.9 percent Asians or Pacific Islanders. Fifteen participants did not indicate their ethnicity. On average, participants used 92.59 minutes (SD = 90.64, Median = 60.00, skewness = 2.36, kurtosis = 7.08) of e-mail, 155.24 minutes (SD = 179.34, Median = 120.00, skewness = 2.32, kurtosis = 5.80) of cell-phone texting, and 110.85 minutes (SD = 140.16, Median = 60.00, skewness = 2.46, kurtosis = 6.81) of Facebook Wall posting. Due to high skewness and kurtosis, they were log transformed to achieve normality. Figure 1 shows the respondents’ perception of each medium regarding media richness, motivations, and perceived network effects. Respondents considered cell-phone texting to be superior than the other two due to its richness and abilities to satisfy the needs to communicate with both strong and weak tie networks. In addition, respondents reported the greatest amount of social pressure to use cell-phone texting compared with the other two communication technologies. Compared with the other two, the respondents did not use e-mail much to communicate with their strong-tie network. Answering research questions Before answering the research questions, a correlation analysis was conducted (see Tables 2–4). All pairwise associations were positive. All correlation coefficients were below the recommended threshold of 0.70,39 suggesting the absence of severe multicollinearity problems. A series of hierarchical regression analyses were conducted with the blocks of control variables and independent variables. RQ1 addressed the effect of media richness on the use of the three technologies. As shown in Table 5, media richness was not significantly associated with e-mail use, yet it was significantly associated with both cell-phone texting ( p < 0.05) and Facebook Wall use ( p < 0.001). RQ2 dealt with the impact of motivations (strong-tie communication and weak-tie communication) on the use of the three technologies. Strong-tie communication did not have any significant impact on the use of the technologies. In contrast, weak-tie communication was significantly associated with cell-phone texting ( p < 0.05). RQ3 addressed the effect of perceived network on the use of the technologies. It was associated with e-mail use with a marginal significance ( p < 0.10), but not significantly associated with cell-phone texting and Facebook Wall use. Control variables, age and sex, were statistically significant in explaining the use of the three technologies. Specifically,
Table 2. Zero-Order Correlations Between Variables (E-Mail) Variable 1. 2. 3. 4. 5.
Media richness Strong-tie communication Weak-tie communication Perceived network effect Use (E-mail) M SD
1
2
3
4
5
1 0.43** 0.34** 0.27** 0.17** 3.33 0.54
1 0.68** 0.40** 0.26** 2.74 0.96
1 0.45** 0.27** 3.60 0.85
1 0.23** 3.63 0.75
1 1.80 0.39
Note: **significant at the 0.01 level (two-tailed).
USE OF TEXT-BASED COMMUNICATION MEDIA
361
Table 3. Zero-Order Correlations Between Variables (Cell-Phone Texting) Variable 1. 2. 3. 4. 5.
Media richness Strong-tie communication Weak-tie communication Perceived network effect Use (cell-phone texting) M SD
1
2
3
4
5
1 0.61** 0.47** 0.41** 0.35** 4.00 0.63
1 0.66** 0.56** 0.34** 4.12 0.76
1 0.50** 0.36** 4.17 0.71
1 0.29** 4.11 0.65
1 1.93 0.54
Note: **significant at the 0.01 level (two-tailed).
younger participants were more likely to use cell-phone texting and Facebook Wall postings, while older respondents were more likely to use e-mail. With regard to sex, women reported spending more time on all three technologies than men. In terms of R2, it was 13.1 percent, 34.1 percent, and 26.7 percent for the use of e-mail, cell-phone texting, and Facebook Wall use, respectively. In the case of cell-phone texting, the variance explained by control variables (22.1 percent) was greater than that by independent variables (12.0 percent). Discussion The communication landscape has changed drastically over the last decade. Particularly among young adults and teenagers, large parts of their communication are based on texts and other multimedia forms.12,13,15 Beyond voice-based conversations, cell phones embody various functions, including the Internet, e-mail, taking and sharing pictures, and playing music, video, and games. Moreover, social networking sites have emerged as novel platforms for sharing and communicating within close and distant social connections. With a wider variety of communication devices and rich features, it is important to understand the forces driving individuals to choose and use one medium compared with others for different purposes. To understand such forces, the present study employed media richness theory, the uses and gratifications perspective, and theory of network effects. Findings from this study support the view that one theory alone cannot adequately explain the use of technologies. We next discuss the findings and implications in greater detail. First, media richness was significantly associated with the use of cell-phone texting and Facebook Wall posting, but not with e-mail. Given the wide use of e-mail in institutional or work-related settings, the finding is understandable. In addition, the finding that the perceived network effect is the sole significant factor which affects people’s e-mail use indirectly
supports this explanation; that is, e-mail use, often associated with instrumental purposes, and the reasons for people to use e-mail, might be better explained by the fact that it makes necessary communication with other users possible than by the features of the medium itself. In contrast, in the case of cell-phone texting and Facebook Wall, media richness of the technologies played an essential role, indicating that technological characteristics drive the use of these technologies. It is somewhat surprising that motivation variables were significantly associated with cell-phone texting only. While previous studies have proved the role of motivations in explaining people’s use of technologies in question, it could be that users’ motivations lose significance when other factors are controlled for, such as media attributes in the case of Facebook Wall postings and perceived network effects in the case of e-mail. In addition, it is possible that the social circle of cell-phone texting might be different from that of Facebook or e-mail. In Facebook, there are generally a few hundred of ‘‘friends,’’ while in e-mail, it is possible to receive messages from strangers. In contrast, the social circle in cell phones largely consists of the people whom one already knows. Thus, the motives for communication may be weaker in Facebook Wall posting and e-mail than in cell-phone texting. In addition, it is notable that only weak-tie communication was significantly associated with texting use. The results show that when other factors are controlled, the motivations to maintain ties with distant others and to communicate for instrumental goals are significant factors which explain people’s use of texting. On the other hand, strong-tie communication did not significantly explain any of the technologies examined in this study. While this study does not provide a direct account for the reasons, it could be that face-to-face or voice-based communication channels that were not included in the current study better satisfy motivations for strong-tie communication, such as showing expressive feelings and strengthening relationships with close friends.
Table 4. Zero-Order Correlations Between Variables (Facebook Wall) Variable 1. 2. 3. 4. 5.
Media richness Strong-tie communication Weak-tie communication Perceived network effect Use (Facebook Wall) M SD
1
2
3
4
5
1 0.60** 0.49** 0.47** 0.43** 3.65 0.63
1 0.68** 0.53** 0.32** 3.88 0.72
1 0.53** 0.26** 4.10 0.64
1 0.27** 3.70 0.70
1 1.73 0.57
Note: **significant at the 0.01 level (two-tailed).
362
PARK ET AL. Table 5. Results of Hierarchical Regression Analyses Technology use E-mail
Variable Control block Age Sex Incremental R2 Independent variables Media richness Strong-tie communication Weak-tie communication Perceived network effect Incremental R2 Total R2
Cell-phone texting
Facebook Wall
b
t
b
t
b
t
0.17** - 0.16** 0.06***
2.83 - 2.75
- 0.40*** - 0.23*** 0.22***
- 7.42 - 4.29
- 0.17** - 0.27*** 0.10***
- 2.89 - 4.59
0.07 0.06 0.11 0.13 + 0.08*** 0.14***
1.11 0.69 1.38 1.96
0.19** - 0.01 0.20** 0.04 0.12*** 0.34***
3.02 - 0.11 2.82 0.63
0.35*** 0.01 0.03 0.09 0.17*** 0.27***
5.10 0.01 0.40 1.33
Note: +significant at the 0.10 level, **significant at the 0.01 level, ***significant at the 0.001 level. Betas refer to standardized regression coefficients in a respective equation.
The influence of perceived network effect was also not substantial, contrary to expectation. It was significantly associated only with e-mail use, suggesting that e-mail might be used in a more passive way to connect to other people for work-related communication. Respondents reported spending the least amount of time on e-mail. Their perceptions of e-mail for both providing a rich environment for interpersonal communication and meeting their needs for communication were low. This is in line with the claim that other media, such as cell-phone texting and Facebook Wall use, are preferred over e-mail for quicker and more informal communication.40 It is notable that control variables, age and sex, were important factors that explained the use of the three different technologies. In terms of age, there was a positive relationship with e-mail use, whereas there were negative relationships with cell-phone texting and Facebook Wall use. However, given that the population of the current study is college students, the interpretation of this finding should be cautious. It was also found that women were more likely to use all three technologies. This finding is consistent with the general trend shown in previous studies: Female teenagers were more likely than men to interact frequently via text messaging,14 and women comprise more than 56 percent of the overall Facebook population.41 The overall findings indicate that the factors affecting the use of communication technologies are not identical. It can be assumed that the use of a given technology can be maximized when there is a match among technological characteristics, the goal achieved by using the technology, and social circumstances. In particular, while recent studies have placed less emphasis on media richness theory than on other explanations of media use, the present study demonstrated that it is too early to discard the value of media richness theory compared with others. Technological attributes played a more influential role in explaining the use of relatively recent technologies examined in the present study—cell-phone texting and Facebook Wall postings—suggesting that media attributes remain an important aspect of individuals’ decision making. The limitations include the following: First, while a random sampling method was utilized to maintain representativeness of the sample, the present study was conducted with
college students in a single university, which limits external validity. For instance, the imbalance of sex in the study might affect the findings of the present study. Second, the use of each technology was measured with the amount of time spent, which made it difficult to capture more diverse and reliable usage patterns. Analyzing the frequency of sending and receiving cell-phone text messages or the number of Facebook Wall messages would have provided useful information about the actual use of these technologies. In conclusion, the present study examined the factors associated with people’s use of three types of text-based technologies based on three different theoretical perspectives. The study found that the theoretical constructs from these theories play different roles when applied to different technologies. Thus, in order to fully understand the dynamics of selection and the use of a given technology, more relevant theoretical perspectives need to be employed, considering technological attributes, users’ motivations, and the social circumstances in which users select and use the technology. Acknowledgments This work was supported by the Faculty Enrichment Grant of the Gaylord College of Journalism and Mass Communication, the University of Oklahoma. Author Disclosure Statement No competing financial interests exist. References 1. Short J, Williams E, Christie B. (1976) The social psychology of telecommunications. London: Wiley. 2. Daft RL, Lengel RH. (1984) Information richness: a new approach to managerial behavior and organizational design. In Staw BM, Cummings LL, eds. Research in organizational behavior, Vol. 6. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press, pp. 101–233. 3. Flanagin AJ, Metzger MJ. Internet use in the contemporary media environment. Human Communication Research 2001; 27:153–181. 4. Fulk J, Boyd B. Emerging theories of communication in organizations. Journal of Management 1991; 17:407–466.
USE OF TEXT-BASED COMMUNICATION MEDIA 5. Markus ML. Electronic mail as the medium of managerial choice. Organization Science 1994; 5:502–527. 6. Fulk J. Social construction of communication technology. Academy of Management Journal 1993; 36:921–950. 7. Fulk J, Schmitz J, Steinfield CW. (1990) A social influence model of technology use. In Fulk J, Steinfield CW, eds. Organizations and communication technology. Newbury Park, CA: Sage, pp. 117–140. 8. Schmitz J, Fulk J. Organizational colleagues, media richness, and electronic mail: a test of the social influence model of technology use. Communication Research 1991; 18:487–523. 9. Katz E, Blumler JG, Gurevitch M. (1974) Utilization of mass communication by the individual. In Blumler JG, Katz E, eds. The uses of mass communication: current perspectives on gratifications research. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage, pp. 19–32. 10. Rubin AM. (2009) Uses-and-gratifications perspective on media effects. In Bryant J, Oliver MB, eds. Media effects: advances in theory and research, 3rd ed. New York: Routledge, pp. 165–184. 11. Ramirez A Jr., Dimmick J, Feaster J, et al. Revisiting interpersonal media competition: The gratification niches of instant messaging, e-mail, and the telephone. Communication Research 2008; 35:529–547. 12. Bryant J, Sanders-Jackson A, Smallwood AK. IMing, text messaging, and adolescent social networks. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 2006; 11:577–592. 13. Lenhart A, Madden M, Hitlin P. (2005) Teens and technology: youth are leading the transition to a fully wired and mobile nation. Washington, DC: Pew Internet & American Life Project. 14. Lenhart A, Ling R, Campbell S, et al. (2010) Teens and mobile phones. Washington, DC: Pew Internet & American Life Project. 15. Smith A. (2011) Americans and text messaging. Washington, DC: Pew Internet & American Life Project. 16. Walther JB, Van Der Heide B, Kim S-Y, et al. The role of friends’ appearance and behavior on evaluations of individuals on Facebook: are we known by the company we keep? Human Communication Research 2008; 34:28–49. 17. Perse EM, Courtright JA. Normative images of communication media: mass and interpersonal channels in the new media environment. Human Communication Research 1993; 19:485–503. 18. Rice RE. Media appropriateness: using social presence theory to compare traditional and new organizational media. Human Communication Research 1993; 19:451–484. 19. Perse EM, Burton P, Kovner E, et al. Predicting computermediated communication in a college class. Communication Research Reports 1992; 9:161–170. 20. Trevino LK, Lengel RH, Daft RD. Media symbolism, media richness and media choice in organization: A symbolic interactionist perspective. Communication Research 1987; 14:553–574. 21. Trevino LK, Lengel RH, Bodendsteiner W, et al. The richness imperative and cognitive style: The role of individual differences in media choice behavior. Management Communication Quarterly 1990; 4:176–197. 22. Sheer VC, Chen L. Improving media richness theory: A study of interaction goals, message valence, and task complexity in manager-subordinate communication. Management Communication Quarterly 2004; 18:76–93. 23. Rice RE, Shook D. Relationships of job categories and organizational levels to use of communication channels, including electronic mail: a meta analysis and extension. Journal of Management Studies 1990; 27:195–229. 24. Ruggiero TE. Uses and gratifications theory in the 21st century. Mass Communication & Society 2000; 3:3–37.
363 25. Rubin AM. (2002) The uses and gratifications perspective of media effects. In Bryant J, Zillmann D, eds. Media effects: advances in theory and research, 2nd ed. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, pp. 525–548. 26. Blumler JG, Katz E. (1974) The uses of mass communications: current perspectives on gratifications research. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. 27. Bagozzi RP. The legacy of the technology acceptance model and a proposal for a paradigm shift. Journal of the Association for Information Systems 2007; 8:244–254. 28. Stafford L, Kline SL, Dimmick J. Home e-mail: relational maintenance and gratification opportunities. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media 1999; 43:659–669. 29. Wei R, Lo VH. Staying connected while on the move: cell phone use and social connectedness. New Media & Society 2006; 8:53–72. 30. Raacke J, Bonds-Raacke J. Myspace and Facebook: applying the uses and gratifications theory to exploring friend-networking sites. CyberPsychology & Behavior 2008; 11:169–174. 31. Joinson AN. (2008) Looking at, looking up or keeping up with people?: motives and use of Facebook. Proceeding of the 26th Annual SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems. Florence, Italy, pp. 1027–1036. 32. Rogers EM. (2003) Diffusion of innovations. 5th ed. New York: Free Press. 33. Katz ML, Shapiro C. Network externalities, competition, and compatibility. American Economic Review 1985; 75:424–440. 34. Wang C-C, Hsu Y-H, Fang W-C. Acceptance of technology with network externalities: an empirical study of internet instant messaging services. Journal of Information Technology Theory and Application 2004; 6. http://aisel.aisnet.org/ jitta/vol6/iss4/4 (retrieved Oct. 3, 2011). 35. Valente T. (1995) Network models of the diffusion of innovations. Cresskill, NJ: Hampton Press. 36. Cho H. Theoretical intersections among social influences, beliefs, and intentions in the context of 3G mobile services in Singapore: decomposing perceived critical mass and subjective norms. Journal of Communication 2011; 61:283–306. 37. Daft RL, Lengel RH. Organizational information requirements, media richness and structural design. Management Science 1986; 32:554–571. 38. Slyke CV, Ilie V, Lou H, et al. Perceived critical mass and the adoption of a communication technology. European Journal of Information Systems 2007; 16:270–283. 39. Tabachnik BG, Fidell LS. (2001) Using multivariate statistics. 4th ed. Boston: Allyn & Bacon. 40. Vascellaro JE. (2009) Why email no longer rules, and what that means for the way we communicate. The Wall Street Journal. http://online.wsj.com/article/SB1000142405297020380390457 4431151489408372.html (retrieved October 25, 2011). 41. Smith J. (2010) December data on Facebook’s US growth by age and gender: beyond 100 million. Insidefacebook.com. www.insidefacebook.com/2010/01/04/december-data-onfacebook%E2%80%99s-us-growth-by-age-and-gender-beyond100-million/ (retrieved October 23, 2011).
Address correspondence to: Dr. Namkee Park Gaylord College of Journalism and Mass Communication University of Oklahoma 395 W. Lindsey St. Norman, OK 73019 E-mail:
[email protected]