Gender-related differences in moral judgments - Semantic Scholar

7 downloads 34 Views 232KB Size Report
Aug 30, 2009 - chological theory assumes that moral stages are universal and their development is invariable (Piaget 1932). In addition, Kohlberg' theory ...
Cogn Process (2010) 11:219–226 DOI 10.1007/s10339-009-0335-2

RESEARCH REPORT

Gender-related differences in moral judgments M. Fumagalli Æ R. Ferrucci Æ F. Mameli Æ S. Marceglia Æ S. Mrakic-Sposta Æ S. Zago Æ C. Lucchiari Æ D. Consonni Æ F. Nordio Æ G. Pravettoni Æ S. Cappa Æ A. Priori

Received: 2 April 2009 / Accepted: 10 August 2009 / Published online: 30 August 2009  Marta Olivetti Belardinelli and Springer-Verlag 2009

Abstract The moral sense is among the most complex aspects of the human mind. Despite substantial evidence confirming gender-related neurobiological and behavioral differences, and psychological research suggesting gender specificities in moral development, whether these

M. Fumagalli  R. Ferrucci  F. Mameli  S. Marceglia  S. Mrakic-Sposta  S. Zago  A. Priori (&) Dipartimento di Scienze Neurologiche, Fondazione IRCCS Ospedale Maggiore Policlinico, Mangiagalli e Regina Elena, Universita` di Milano, Via F. Sforza, 35, Milan 20122, Italy e-mail: [email protected] M. Fumagalli  R. Ferrucci  F. Mameli  S. Marceglia  S. Mrakic-Sposta  A. Priori Centro Clinico per le Neuronanotecnologie e la Neurostimolazione, Fondazione IRCCS Ospedale Maggiore Policlinico, Mangiagalli e Regina Elena, Milan, Italy M. Fumagalli  R. Ferrucci  F. Mameli  S. Marceglia  S. Mrakic-Sposta  S. Zago  A. Priori Unita` Operativa di Neurologia, Fondazione IRCCS Ospedale Maggiore Policlinico, Mangiagalli e Regina Elena, Milan, Italy

differences arise from cultural effects or are innate remains unclear. In this study, we investigated the role of gender, education (general education and health education) and religious belief (Catholic and non-Catholic) on moral choices by testing 50 men and 50 women with a moral judgment task. Whereas we found no differences between the two genders in utilitarian responses to non-moral dilemmas and to impersonal moral dilemmas, men gave significantly more utilitarian answers to personal moral (PM) dilemmas (i.e., those courses of action whose endorsement involves highly emotional decisions). Cultural factors such as education and religion had no effect on performance in the moral judgment task. These findings suggest that the cognitive–emotional processes involved in evaluating PM dilemmas differ in men and in women, possibly reflecting differences in the underlying neural mechanisms. Gender-related determinants of moral behavior may partly explain gender differences in real-life involving power management, economic decision-making, leadership and possibly also aggressive and criminal behaviors.

C. Lucchiari  G. Pravettoni Dipartimento di Scienze Sociali e Politiche, Centro interdipartimentale di Ricerca e Intervento sui Processi Decisionali (IRIDe), Universita` di Milano, Milan, Italy

Keywords Moral judgment  Morality  Utilitarianism  Gender differences

D. Consonni  F. Nordio Unita` di Epidemiologia, Fondazione IRCCS Ospedale Maggiore Policlinico, Mangiagalli e Regina Elena, Milan, Italy

Introduction

F. Nordio Dipartimento di Clinica Medica, Nefrologia e Scienze della Prevenzione, Universita` degli Studi, Parma, Italy S. Cappa Dipartimento di Neuroscienze, Centro di Neuroscienze Cognitive, Istituto Scientifico San Raffaele, Universita` Vita-Salute San Raffaele, Milan, Italy

The moral sense is central to the human mind and crucial in determining human behavior. Whereas philosophers have been speculating about the origin and determinants of morality since antiquity (Thomson 1986), psychological interest began in the 1960s and scientific investigation into the neurological mechanisms underlying moral choice began only recently.

123

220

Philosophical and psychological studies have different objectives. Whereas philosophers are interested in developing a normative theory, psychologists primarily want to describe moral development and behavior. Without pursuing philosophers’ more ambitious goal of giving a complete normative account, psychological studies aim to pinpoint the major specific factors. The groundwork of moral psychology hinges on the theories proposed by Piaget and Kohlberg. Piaget’s psychological theory assumes that moral stages are universal and their development is invariable (Piaget 1932). In addition, Kohlberg’ theory assumes that morality is universal, equal for men and women and for all cultures (Kohlberg 1964). This idea has nevertheless been repeatedly questioned; the role of factors such as gender, education and religion opens the more general question about innateness of morality (Dupoux and Jacob 2007; Hauser et al. 2007). One of the major factors considered in evaluating moral reasoning and behavior is gender. According to ‘‘common sense’’, women were described as inclined to sentimentalism, to intuition and to emotion. Terms such as ‘‘angel of the hearth’’ convey an idea of women characterized by feelings, vocation to motherhood and care and empathy. Despite this cliche´, history contradicts this motherly image. Take for example witches, prostitutes, and son-killers. Even Kohlberg himself identified a strong interpersonal bias in the women’s moral judgments. Other researchers also questioned the possibility of a sex-related bias in Kohlberg’s theory (Gilligan 1982; Haan 1975; Holstein 1976). In particular, Gilligan pointed out that the Kohlberg model considers justice as the fundamental principle for moral behavior and penalizes women, according to whom the central moral problem is the conflict between self and other. Whereas men solve moral dilemmas in a rational way, respecting law and order, women are driven by emotion, empathy and care for others. These differences led Gilligan to describe two divergent modes of moral reasoning: an ethic of care and an ethic of justice (Gump et al. 2000). Other studies confirmed these differences in various tests for evaluating moral judgment (Aldrich and Kage 2003; Bjorklund 2003; Eisenman 1967; Gump et al. 2000; Indick et al. 2000; Skoe 1995). Despite these reports, others found no gender differences (Brabeck and Shore 2003; Jaffee and Hyde 2000; Keasey 1972; Lifton 1985; McGraw and Bloomfield 1987), leaving this topic open to debate. In the neuroscience field, neuroimaging (Greene and Haidt 2002; Greene et al. 2001, 2004) and lesional studies in patients with focal brain lesions (Koenigs et al. 2007) have now documented the neurobiological determinants of moral behavior. Except for Harenski et al. (2008) who studied the neural correlates of moral sensitivity in females

123

Cogn Process (2010) 11:219–226

and males and found greater posterior cingulate cortex and anterior insula activation in females, and greater inferior parietal cortex activation in males, these studies neglected to consider the gender factor. Research, expanding gender factors to other fields of psychology, disclosed gender differences in empathic ability (Baron-Cohen and Wheelwright 2004; Eisenberg 2005) and in other functions having a causal role in moral judgment, such as emotions (Aleman and Swart 2008; Hareli et al. 2009). These differences correlate also with the well-documented gender differences in brain structures (Cahill 2006; Gur et al. 1999; Luders et al. 2002; Raz et al. 2004; Tranel et al. 2005), neurotransmitter systems (Cahill 2006), cognitive performance (Bolla et al. 2004; Kimura 1996; Overman et al. 2004, 2006; Overman 2004; Reavis and Overman 2001; Vecchi and Girelli 1998; Voyer and Saunders 2004) and brain activation during cognitive tasks (Bell et al. 2006; Fischer et al. 2004; Lee et al. 2005; Weiss et al. 2003). Among other factors influencing moral judgment is education. Several studies investigated its role in relation to age, specifically in college students, and to a lesser extent, graduate students (Rest et al. 1999; Thoma 1993). Studying both undergraduate and graduate students, Maeda et al. (2009) found that students in medical schools give higher average moral judgment levels than students in business and other majors. These results suggested that such schools are preparing students for a profession or calling that is essentially a moral enterprise that serves as an environmental factor that influences level of moral judgment (Bebeau and Monson 2008). Among the few studies interested in addressing the relation between religion and morality, Hauser et al. (2007) found no differences in moral judgment according to religious affiliation. Moral judgment depends also crucially on cognitive and emotional processes. In their recent research about neural basis of morality, Greene et al. (2001) investigated this topic according to the utilitarian theory stating that an action is morally right if it produces the highest utility of any available alternative action. The morality and duty of an act were irrelevant. In this perspective, Greene distinguished between ‘‘personal’’ and ‘‘impersonal’’ moral actions, on the basis of the dual-process theory (Greene 2009; Greene et al. 2001; McGuire et al. 2009). This theory distinguishes a fast, unconscious and effortless affective system and a slow, conscious and effortful cognitive system. Whereas the affective system is activated by personal moral (PM) judgments, cognitive system is preferentially activated by impersonal conflicts. The term ‘‘utilitarian responses’’ denotes the decision to perform an action directed toward achieving the greatest advantage and the minimum disadvantage for aggregate welfare. People

Cogn Process (2010) 11:219–226

usually choose this course of action when faced with a nonmoral (NM) choice (for example, choosing to take the train instead of bus to avoid arriving late at an important meeting), or an emotionally non-salient decision. Conversely, when choice involves, for example, inflicting direct harm to one person to save the lives of others (a PM dilemma), most people decide against the action or to endorse it (utilitarian response) only after a high emotional conflict. Previous research in moral judgments, therefore, leaves gender-related differences and the relation between this factor and education and religion unclear. Neither does it specify the role of reason and emotion in the complex decisional processing involved in moral judgment nor can it be modulated by gender, religion and education. Having more complete information is essential in understanding the role of culture and nature in moral thinking and behavior. We designed this study to find out whether moral judgments differ between genders and differ according to cultural factors such as religious belief and education. To do so, we evaluated moral judgments with a computercontrolled procedure, the moral judgment task, testing NM, impersonal moral (IM) and PM dilemmas (Greene et al. 2001, 2004; Koenigs et al. 2007). As dependent variables, we assessed response times (RTs) and percentage of utilitarian responses. Utilitarian responses were defined as the decision to perform an action directed toward achieving the greatest advantage and the minimum disadvantage for aggregate welfare.

221 Table 1 Demographic data Gender

Education

Female

Health

Religion

N

Age Mean (±SE)

Catholic

16

23.25 (0.83)

9

24.11 (0.93) 24.92 (0.82)

Non-Catholic General Male

Health General

Catholic

13

Non-Catholic

12

22.41 (0.65)

Catholic

17

26.82 (1.50)

Non-Catholic

8

24.00 (0.85)

Catholic

9

24.66 (1.04)

16

24.75 (0.80)

Non-Catholic SE standard error

Moral judgment task We used the moral judgment task proposed by Greene and colleagues (Fumagalli et al. 2009; Greene et al. 2001, 2004). We used a battery of 30 practical dilemmas randomly extracted from 60 scenarios (Greene et al. 2004) translated into Italian. The task consisted of 20 NM dilemmas and two classes of ‘‘moral’’ scenarios subdivided into IM (18 scenarios) and PM (22 scenarios) dilemmas. RTs and percentage of utilitarian responses were recorded. In agreement with the utilitarian theory, we distinguished utilitarian and non-utilitarian responses. A utilitarian response implies judging actions by their ability to maximize good consequences, so that any harm to one person can be justified by a greater gain to others: individuals’ interests can be sacrificed for the sake of the community.

Materials and methods

Procedure section

Participants

The experimental procedure took 30 min for each subject. After participants gave their informed consent, we asked about age, school and religious belief. We evaluated handedness using the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (Oldfield 1971) and we also ensured the absence of neurological and psychiatric diseases. Subjects were seated in a quiet room in front of a personal computer. The moral judgment task was administered in accordance with Koenigs et al. (2007). Each dilemma was presented in a series of three screens of text. The first two screens each presented a paragraph describing the context and details of the dilemma. The third screen posed a question about a hypothetical action related to the scenario (‘‘Would you…in order to…?’’). Participants were allowed to read through screens 1 and 2 at their own pace, pressing the space bar to advance to the next screen. In the third screen, participants had a maximum of 25 s to read the final question and press the left (YES) or the right (NO) mouse button (Fig. 1). Stimuli were presented on a

One hundred healthy volunteers (50 males and 50 females, aged 20–32 years; education 14–22 years; Table 1) participated in the study. Subjects were recruited from among students at the University of Milan and received no reward for taking part in the experiments. All participants spoke native Italian, were right handed and had no history of neurological or psychiatric disorders. All participants gave their informed consent and the procedures were approved by the local ethical committee. The experimental procedure was in accordance with the declaration of Helsinki. We distinguished between Catholics and non-Catholics by asking subjects about their religious belief. The nonCatholic group included both agnostics and atheists. We also distinguished between health education and general education in agreement with how the degree courses are subdivided at the Italian Department for Education, University and Research.

123

222

Cogn Process (2010) 11:219–226

Data analysis

Fig. 1 Task sequence representative of personal moral dilemma. The sequence is the same for non-moral and impersonal moral dilemmas

personal computer screen using E-Prime Version 1.1 (Psychology Software Tools, Inc, Pittsburgh, USA). Impersonal moral dilemmas included the following example: You are at the wheel of a runaway trolley quickly approaching a fork in the tracks. On the tracks extending to the left is a group of five railway workmen. On the tracks extending to the right is a single railway workman. If you do nothing the trolley will proceed to the left, causing the deaths of the five workmen. The only way to avoid the deaths of these workmen is to hit a switch on your dashboard that will cause the trolley to proceed to the right, causing the death of the single workman. Would you hit the switch in order to avoid the deaths of the five workmen? Personal moral dilemmas included the following: A runaway trolley is heading down the tracks toward five workmen who will be killed if the trolley proceeds on its present course. You are on a footbridge over the tracks, in between the approaching trolley and the five workmen. Next to you on this footbridge is a stranger who happens to be very large. The only way to save the lives of the five workmen is to push this stranger off the bridge and onto the tracks below where his large body will stop the trolley. The stranger will die if you do this, but the five workmen will be saved. Would you push the stranger on to the tracks in order to save the five workmen? Among the non-moral dilemmas was the following example: You are a farm worker driving a turnip-harvesting machine. You are approaching two diverging paths. By choosing the path on the left you will harvest ten bushels of turnips. By choosing the path on the right you will harvest twenty bushels of turnips. If you do nothing your turnip-harvesting machine will turn to the left. Would you turn your turnip-picking machine to the right in order to harvest twenty bushels of turnips instead of ten?

123

We analyzed the effect of three factors on task performance: gender, religious belief and education. Gender differences in the percentage of utilitarian responses given for each scenario type (NM, IM, PM) were tested using a four-way between subjects analysis of variance (ANOVA) with the factors gender (males, females), religion (Catholic, non-Catholic), education (health, general) and type of dilemma (NM, IM, PM). Response times were collected as RTs for utilitarian responses (mean utilitarian RTs for each type of dilemmas for each subject) and non-utilitarian RTs (mean non-utilitarian RTs for each type of dilemmas for each subject). To evaluate between-group differences in utilitarian and nonutilitarian RTs we used a five-way between subjects ANOVA with gender (males, females), education (health, general), religion (Catholic, non-Catholic), utilitarian or non-utilitarian responses and type of dilemma (NM, IM, PM) as factors. Bonferroni corrected t tests were used for post hoc analysis. We checked the normality assumption for all the response variables with skewness and kurtosis test for normality. A two-tailed P value \0.05 was considered statistically significant. All statistical analyses were performed in STATISTICA (StatSoft, Inc, Tulsa, USA).

Results Gender had a highly significant effect on percentage utilitarian responses (F2,176 = 3.78, P = 0.0248). Post hoc analysis showed that despite no differences in IM and NM responses, utilitarian responses to PM dilemmas were significantly more frequent in men than in women [t(98) = -3.28, P = 0.0014; Fig. 2]. Conversely, no differences were found in RTs for utilitarian and non-utilitarian responses between the two sexes for the same class of dilemmas. Education and religious beliefs neither had significant effect on RTs for utilitarian and non-utilitarian responses or on utilitarian response percentages, nor did they significantly interact with gender (Tables 2, 3, 4).

Discussion The first finding in our study is that moral judgments differ between genders. We found that responses to PM dilemmas differ specifically and selectively in men and in women, showing that gender-related differences in moral judgment specifically influence those dilemmas involving emotionally salient actions. Under these circumstances, men more frequently make a ‘‘pragmatic’’ choice regardless of putting others at risk of danger or harm. The prevalence of

Cogn Process (2010) 11:219–226

223

Fig. 2 Percentage of utilitarian responses in males and females in the moral judgment task. Error bars are standard error of the mean. We used three classes of stimuli: non-moral dilemmas (NM, 20 scenarios), impersonal moral dilemmas (IM, 18 scenarios) and personal moral dilemmas (PM, 22 scenarios). Sex influenced the percentage of utilitarian responses (F2,176 = 3.78, P = 0.0248). Although ANOVA disclosed no differences between impersonal moral and non-moral responses, utilitarian responses in personal moral dilemmas were significantly more frequent in men than in women [t(98) = -3.28, P = 0.0014]

non-utilitarian responses to PM dilemmas in females supports the gender-related distinction between ethics of care and ethics of justice proposed by Gilligan (1982). These

findings may be connected to the gender-differences in empathic ability (Baron-Cohen and Wheelwright 2004; Eisenberg 2005) that make females more resistant to decisions that despite being rationally viable entail directly inflicting physical or moral pain to other individuals. Hence, female moral reasoning seems directed to avoid harming other people, to place high value on social relationships and to fulfill other individuals’ expectations. Conversely, male moral thought hinges on the abstract principles of justice and fairness and on an individualistic stance (Jaffee and Hyde 2000; Rest 1979). Overall, our findings support Greene’s dual-process theory, confirming that non-utilitarian choices in response to PM dilemmas are driven more by emotions than by cognition. Whether gender differences in behavioral measures arise from cultural effects or reflect innate differences remains unclear. Although the answer is probably more complex than the traditional dichotomous nature-nurture debate envisages (Lippa 2005), several observations support a possible role of neural mechanisms in clarifying this question. Unlike gender, cultural factors such as education and religious belief had no effect on how the university students we studied performed the moral judgment task. Hence, we conjecture that gender differences are probably

Table 2 Percentage of utilitarian responses as function of gender, education and religion % of Utilitarian responses

N

Personal moral

Impersonal moral

Mean (±SE)

Mean (±SE)

Mean (±SE)

Gender

Education

Female

Health

Catholic

16

88.75 (1.80)

25.38 (3.51)

77.71 (3.24)

General

Non-Catholic Catholic

9 13

87.78 (3.24) 82.31 (3.43)

22.22 (5.05) 29.76 (4.83)

69.14 (3.60) 69.85 (3.43)

Non-Catholic

12

90.00 (2.75)

32.32 (5.61)

69.87 (5.65)

Catholic

17

83.53 (2.42)

37.14 (4.03)

78.02 (3.52)

Non-Catholic

8

85.00 (4.23)

39.77 (9.09)

71.84 (6.33)

Catholic

9

85.56 (3.77)

39.06 (6.28)

70.03 (5.28)

16

89.38 (1.93)

40.97 (4.48)

81.63 (3.81)

Male

Religion

Non-moral

Health General

Non-Catholic SE standard error

Table 3 Reaction times of utilitarian responses as function of gender, education and religion RTs utilitarian responses Gender

Education

Female

Health

N Religion Catholic Non-Catholic

General Male

Health General

Non-moral

Personal moral

Impersonal moral

Mean (±SE)

Mean (±SE)

Mean (±SE)

16

4311.23 (249.04)

3889.62 (565.33)

3020.67 (210.55)

9

4273.68 (409.55)

4259.94 (1206.41)

3485.65 (678.61)

Catholic

13

4403.48 (421.38)

4938.99 (514.18)

3358.08 (327.24)

Non-Catholic

12

4100.29 (355.48)

3990.96 (613.28)

3334.79 (385.18)

Catholic

17

4296.30 (300.18)

3713.75 (316.05)

3490.63 (348.59)

Non-Catholic

8

5273.59 (814.23)

4001.69 (667.2)

3986.67 (420.25)

Catholic

9

3745.79 (303.87)

3592.43 (339.44)

2764.19 (166.19)

16

4274.29 (286.14)

4517.52 (711.31)

3841.40 (364.41)

Non-Catholic SE standard error, RTs reaction times

123

224

Cogn Process (2010) 11:219–226

Table 4 Reaction times of non-utilitarian responses as function of gender, education and religion RTs non-utilitarian responses Gender

Education

Female

Health

N Religion Catholic

Male

Health General

Personal moral

Impersonal moral

Mean (±SE)

Mean (±SE)

Mean (±SE)

16

5309.00 (798.53)

3250.22 (213.56)

2933.19 (301.94)

9

5492.43 (1061.65)

3364.37 (401.43)

3048.91 (351.88)

Catholic

13

5467.64 (307.36)

3395.46 (253.3)

3740.54 (357.00)

Non-Catholic

12

5613.57 (715.84)

3472.36 (281.57)

4318.49 (592.36)

Catholic

17

6683.13 (1134.21)

3531.19 (316.61)

3365.26 (381.46)

Non-Catholic

8

7177.57 (1575.71)

4023.5 (595.84)

4243.42 (988.24)

Catholic Non-Catholic

9 16

5030.64 (1296.77) 6634.75 (1303.74)

2997.88 (185.22) 3794.65 (439.23)

3574.24 (288.11) 3800.25 (468.93)

Non-Catholic General

Non-moral

SE standard error, RTs reaction times

better explained by biological than by cultural differences. In a neurophysiological perspective, Greene et al. (2004) underline that assessment of moral dilemmas involves brain areas associated with social–emotional and cognitive processes such as affect, motivation, working memory, abstract reasoning and problem solving. They also show that brain activation patterns differ in the three classes of dilemmas, and also in utilitarian and non-utilitarian responses to PM dilemmas. In discussing the findings from their lesional study, Koenigs et al. (2007) concluded that a combination of intuitive–affective and conscious–rational mechanisms operate to produce moral judgment, contributing in different proportion to moral choices. The fact that patients with bilateral ventromedial prefrontal cortex (VMPFC) lesions are more utilitarian than healthy subjects in judging PM dilemmas suggests that the VMPFC is a critical area in solving moral conflict in which social emotions are relevant (Koenigs et al. 2007). This portion of the frontal cortex may be the site of sex-related differences in moral sense. In line with this conclusion, Tranel et al. (2005) found gender-related differences in the pattern of left–right asymmetry in the VMPFC. Several anatomical studies also showed gender-related differences in the frontal lobe. For example, women have a larger dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (Chen et al. 2007; Schlaepfer et al. 1995; Sowell et al. 2007) and more gray matter in the orbitofrontal (Gur et al. 1999) and in the left ventral frontal cortex whereas men have thicker right orbitofrontal cortices (Sowell et al. 2007). Another factor that might explain the sex-related difference in moral sense is the specificity of neurotransmitter systems or of hormonal stimulation (Cahill 2006). Our hypothesis about gender-related differences in morality receives support also from a recent functional neuroimaging study (Harenski et al. 2008). Whatever the explanation, our new findings showing gender-related differences in moral sense suggest that previous studies need reappraising. For instance, a given

123

frontal lobe lesion might induce a different moral behavior in men and women, and the pattern of functional brain changes during a moral task might differ between genders. Our findings should nevertheless offer a good starting point to explain, for example, the gender-related inclination to specific job classes, attitudes toward leadership and power management and criminal behaviors. They may also provide clues as to why men are physically and verbally more aggressive than women (Archer 2004; Buss and Perry 1992; Greenfeld and Snell 1999), and why crime statistics in several countries (Australian Institute of Criminology 2007; Home Office 2002, 2003; Sabol et al. 2007) report that the female percentage of total inmates is 6–7%, irrespective of nationality, culture, religion and age. In conclusion, our study showed the existence of genderrelated differences in moral behavior in young adults. Whether and how these differences can change at different ages should be matter of further, specifically designed studies. Acknowledgments Manuela Fumagalli was supported by University of Milan, Department of Neurological Sciences grant. Roberta Ferrucci was supported by FISM-Fondazione Italiana Sclerosi Multipla—Cod. 2007/R/13.

References Aldrich D, Kage R (2003) Mars and Venus at twilight: a critical investigation of moralism, age effects, and sex differences. Pol Psychol 24:23–40 Aleman A, Swart M (2008) Sex differences in neural activation to facial expressions denoting contempt and disgust. PLoS One 3:e3622 Archer J (2004) Sex differences in aggression in real-world settings: a meta-analytic review. Rev Gen Psychol 8:291–322 Australian Institute of Criminology (2007) Australian crime: facts and figures 2006. AIC, Canberra Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S (2004) The empathy quotient: an investigation of adults with Asperger syndrome or high

Cogn Process (2010) 11:219–226 functioning autism, and normal sex differences. J Autism Dev Disord 34:163–175 Bebeau MJ, Monson BE (2008) Guided by theory, grounded in evidence: a way forward for professional ethics education. In: Nucci LP, Narvaez D (eds) Handbook of moral and character education. Routledge, New York, pp 557–582 Bell EC, Willson MC, Wilman AH, Dave S, Silverstone PH (2006) Males and females differ in brain activation during cognitive tasks. Neuroimage 30:529–538 Bjorklund F (2003) Differences in the justification of choices in moral dilemmas: effects of gender, time pressure and dilemma seriousness. Scand J Psychol 44:459–466 Bolla KI, Eldreth DA, Matochik JA, Cadet JL (2004) Sex-related differences in a gambling task and its neurological correlates. Cereb Cortex 14:1226–1232 Brabeck M, Shore E (2003) Gender differences in intellectual and moral development? The evidence that refutes the claim. In: Demick J, Andreoletti C (eds) Handbook of adult development. Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers, New York, pp 351–368 Buss AH, Perry M (1992) The aggression questionnaire. J Pers Soc Psychol 63:452–459 Cahill L (2006) Why sex matters for neuroscience. Nat Rev Neurosci 7:477–484 Chen X, Sachdev PS, Wen W, Anstey KJ (2007) Sex differences in regional grey matter in healthy individuals aged 44–48 years: a voxel-based morphometric study. Neuroimage 36:691–699 Dupoux E, Jacob P (2007) Universal moral grammar: a critical appraisal. Trends Cogn Sci 11:373–378 Eisenberg N (2005) The development of empathy-related responding. Nebr Symp Motiv 51:73–117 Eisenman R (1967) Sex differences in moral judgment. Percept Mot Skills 24:784 Fischer H, Fransson P, Wright CI, Backman L (2004) Enhanced occipital and anterior cingulate activation in men but not in women during exposure to angry and fearful male faces. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 4:326–334 Fumagalli M, Vergari M, Pasqualetti P, Marceglia S, Mameli F, Ferrucci R, Mrakic-Sposta S, Zago S, Sartori G, Pravettoni G, Barbieri S, Cappa S, Priori A (2009) Brain switches utilitarian behavior: does sex make the differences? (submitted) Gilligan C (1982) In a different voice: psychological theory and women’s development. Harvard University Press, Cambridge Greene JD (2009) Dual-process morality and the personal/impersonal distinction: a reply to McGuire, Langdon, Coltheart, and Mackenzie. J Exp Soc Psychol. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2009.01.003 Greene J, Haidt J (2002) How (and where) does moral judgment work? Trends Cogn Sci 6:517–523 Greene JD, Sommerville RB, Nystrom LE, Darley JM, Cohen JD (2001) An fMRI investigation of emotional engagement in moral judgment. Science 293:2105–2108 Greene JD, Nystrom LE, Engell AD, Darley JM, Cohen JD (2004) The neural bases of cognitive conflict and control in moral judgment. Neuron 44:389–400 Greenfeld L, Snell TL (1999) Women offenders. Bureau of Justice Statistics, Washington Gump LS, Baker RC, Roll S (2000) Cultural and gender differences in moral judgment: a study of Mexican Americans and AngloAmericans. Hisp J Behav Sci 22:78–93 Gur RC, Turetsky BI, Matsui M, Yan M, Bilker W, Hughett P, Gur RE (1999) Sex differences in brain gray and white matter in healthy young adults: correlations with cognitive performance. J Neurosci 19:4065–4072 Haan N (1975) Hypothetical and actual moral reasoning in a situation of civil disobedience. J Pers Soc Psychol 32:255–270

225 Hareli S, Shomrat N, Hess U (2009) Emotional versus neutral expressions and perceptions of social dominance and submissiveness. Emotion 9:378–384 Harenski CL, Antonenko O, Shane MS, Kiehl KA (2008) Gender differences in neural mechanisms underlying moral sensitivity. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 3:313–321 Hauser M, Cushman F, Young L, Jin RK, Mikhail J (2007) A dissociation between moral judgments and justifications. Mind Lang 22:1–21 Holstein CB (1976) Irreversible, stepwise sequence in the development of moral judgment: a longitudinal study of males and females. Child Dev 47:51–61 Home Office (2002) Prison statistics England and Wales 2002. Home Office, London Home Office (2003) Prison population brief. England and Wales: October 2003. Home Office, London Indick W, Kim J, Oelberger B, Semino L (2000) Gender differences in moral judgement: is non-consequential reasoning a factor? Curr Res Soc Psychol 5:285–298 Jaffee S, Hyde JS (2000) Gender differences in moral orientation: a meta-analysis. Psychol Bull 126:703–726 Keasey CB (1972) The lack of sex differences in the moral judgments of preadolescents. J Soc Psychol 86:157 Kimura D (1996) Sex, sexual orientation and sex hormones influence human cognitive function. Curr Opin Neurobiol 6:259–263 Koenigs M, Young L, Adolphs R, Tranel D, Cushman F, Hauser M, Damasio A (2007) Damage to the prefrontal cortex increases utilitarian moral judgements. Nature 446:908–911 Kohlberg L (1964) Development of moral character and moral ideology. In: Hoffman ML, Hoffman LW (eds) Review of child development research. Russell Sage Foundation, New York Lee TM, Liu HL, Chan CC, Fang SY, Gao JH (2005) Neural activities associated with emotion recognition observed in men and women. Mol Psychiatry 10:450–455 Lifton PD (1985) Individual differences in moral development: the relation of sex, gender, and personality to morality. J Pers 53:306–334 Lippa R (2005) Gender, nature and nurture. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah Luders E, Steinmetz H, Jancke L (2002) Brain size and grey matter volume in the healthy human brain. Neuroreport 13:2371–2374 Maeda Y, Thoma SJ, Bebeau MJ (2009) Understanding the relationship between moral judgment development and individual characteristics: the role of educational contexts. J Educ Psychol 101:233–247 McGraw KM, Bloomfield J (1987) Social influence on group moral decisions: the interactive effects of moral reasoning and sex role orientation. J Pers Soc Psychol 53:1080–1087 McGuire J, Langdon R, Coltheart M, Mackenzie C (2009) A reanalysis of the personal/impersonal distinction in moral psychology research. J Exp Soc Psychol 45:577–580 Oldfield RC (1971) The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9:97–113 Overman WH (2004) Sex differences in early childhood, adolescence, and adulthood on cognitive tasks that rely on orbital prefrontal cortex. Brain Cogn 55:134–147 Overman WH, Frassrand K, Ansel S, Trawalter S, Bies B, Redmond A (2004) Performance on the IOWA card task by adolescents and adults. Neuropsychologia 42:1838–1851 Overman W, Graham L, Redmond A, Eubank R, Boettcher L, Samplawski O, Walsh K (2006) Contemplation of moral dilemmas eliminates sex differences on the Iowa gambling task. Behav Neurosci 120:817–825 Piaget J (1932) The moral judgment of the child. Harcourt, Brace Jovanovich, Oxford

123

226 Raz N, Gunning-Dixon F, Head D, Rodrigue KM, Williamson A, Acker JD (2004) Aging, sexual dimorphism, and hemispheric asymmetry of the cerebral cortex: replicability of regional differences in volume. Neurobiol Aging 25:377–396 Reavis R, Overman WH (2001) Adult sex differences on a decisionmaking task previously shown to depend on the orbital prefrontal cortex. Behav Neurosci 115:196–206 Rest J (1979) Development in judging moral issues. University of Minnesota Press, Minneapolis Rest JR, Narvaez D, Thoma SJ, Bebeau MJ (1999) DIT2: devising and testing a revised instrument of moral judgment. J Educ Psychol 91:644–659 Sabol W, Couture H, Harrison PM (2007) Prisoners in 2006. Bureau of Justice Statistics Bulletin. U.S. Department of Justice, Washington, DC Schlaepfer TE, Harris GJ, Tien AY, Peng L, Lee S, Pearlson GD (1995) Structural differences in the cerebral cortex of healthy female and male subjects: a magnetic resonance imaging study. Psychiatry Res 61:129–135 Skoe EE (1995) Sex role orientation and its relationship to the development of identity and moral thought. Scand J Psychol 36:235–245 Sowell ER, Peterson BS, Kan E, Woods RP, Yoshii J, Bansal R, Xu D, Zhu H, Thompson PM, Toga AW (2007) Sex differences in

123

Cogn Process (2010) 11:219–226 cortical thickness mapped in 176 healthy individuals between 7 and 87 years of age. Cereb Cortex 17:1550–1560 Thoma SJ (1993) The relationship between political preference and moral judgment development in late adolescence. Merrill-Palmer quarterly. J Dev Psychol 39:359–374 Thomson J (1986) Rights, restitution and risk. Harvard University Press, Cambridge Tranel D, Damasio H, Denburg NL, Bechara A (2005) Does gender play a role in functional asymmetry of ventromedial prefrontal cortex? Brain 128:2872–2881 Vecchi T, Girelli L (1998) Gender differences in visuo-spatial processing: the importance of distinguishing between passive storage and active manipulation. Acta Psychol 99:1–16 Voyer D, Saunders KA (2004) Gender differences on the mental rotations test: a factor analysis. Acta Psychol 117:79–94 Weiss E, Siedentopf CM, Hofer A, Deisenhammer EA, Hoptman MJ, Kremser C, Golaszewski S, Felber S, Fleischhacker WW, Delazer M (2003) Sex differences in brain activation pattern during a visuospatial cognitive task: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study in healthy volunteers. Neurosci Lett 344:169–172