International Journal of Computer Science Issues

7 downloads 0 Views 2MB Size Report
... Technology, India. • Mr. Anil Kumar, Sikkim Manipal Institute of Technology, India .... Mr. Akhilesh Tiwari, Madhav Institute of Technology & Science, India. • Mr. Dakshina ...... [9] D. Johnson, Dave Maltz, Y Hu, Jorjeta Jetcheva, “The Dynamic.
                 

IJCSI

         

 

International Journal of Computer Science Issues

Volume 7, Issue 3, No 10, May 2010 ISSN (Online): 1694-0784 ISSN (Print): 1694-0814

© IJCSI PUBLICATION www.IJCSI.org

IJCSI proceedings are currently indexed by:

© IJCSI PUBLICATION 2010 www.IJCSI.org

IJCSI Publicity Board 2010

Dr. Borislav D Dimitrov Department of General Practice, Royal College of Surgeons in Ireland Dublin, Ireland

Dr. Vishal Goyal Department of Computer Science, Punjabi University Patiala, India

Mr. Nehinbe Joshua University of Essex Colchester, Essex, UK

Mr. Vassilis Papataxiarhis Department of Informatics and Telecommunications National and Kapodistrian University of Athens, Athens, Greece

EDITORIAL In this third edition of 2010, we bring forward issues from va rious dynam ic co mputer science areas ranging from system perf ormance, computer vision, artifi cial intelligence, software engineering, m ultimedia , pattern recognition, info rmation retrieval, d atabases, security and networking among others. As always we thank all our review ers for prov iding constructive comm ents on papers sent to them for review. This helps enorm ously in improving the quality of papers published in this issue. IJCSI will maintain its policy of sending print copies of the journal to all co rresponding authors worldwide free of charg e. Apart from availability of the f ull-texts from the jou rnal website, all published p apers are d eposited in open-access repositories to m ake access eas ier and ensur e continuous availability of its proceedings. The trans ition from the 2 nd issu e to the 3 rd on e has been m arked with an agreem ent signed between IJCSI and ProQuest an d EBSCOHOST, two lead ing directo ries to help in th e dissemination of our published papers. W e believe further indexing and more dissem ination will definitely lead to further citations of our authors’ articles. We are pleased to present IJCSI Volum e 7, Issu e 3, May 2010, split in el even numbers (IJCSI Vol. 7, Issue 3, No. 10). The acceptance rate for this issue is 37.88%. We wish you a happy reading! IJCSI Editorial Board May 2010 Issue ISSN (Print): 1694-0814 ISSN (Online): 1694-0784 © IJCSI Publications www.IJCSI.org 

IJCSI Editorial Board 2010

Dr Tristan Vanrullen Chief Editor LPL, Laboratoire Parole et Langage - CNRS - Aix en Provence, France LABRI, Laboratoire Bordelais de Recherche en Informatique - INRIA - Bordeaux, France LEEE, Laboratoire d'Esthétique et Expérimentations de l'Espace - Université d'Auvergne, France Dr Constantino Malagôn Associate Professor Nebrija University Spain Dr Lamia Fourati Chaari Associate Professor Multimedia and Informatics Higher Institute in SFAX Tunisia Dr Mokhtar Beldjehem Professor Sainte-Anne University Halifax, NS, Canada Dr Pascal Chatonnay Assistant Professor MaÎtre de Conférences Laboratoire d'Informatique de l'Université de Franche-Comté Université de Franche-Comté France Dr Yee-Ming Chen Professor Department of Industrial Engineering and Management Yuan Ze University Taiwan

Dr Vishal Goyal Assistant Professor Department of Computer Science Punjabi University Patiala, India Dr Natarajan Meghanathan Assistant Professor REU Program Director Department of Computer Science Jackson State University Jackson, USA Dr Deepak Laxmi Narasimha Department of Software Engineering, Faculty of Computer Science and Information Technology, University of Malaya, Kuala Lumpur, Malaysia Dr Navneet Agrawal Assistant Professor Department of ECE, College of Technology & Engineering, MPUAT, Udaipur 313001 Rajasthan, India Prof N. Jaisankar Assistant Professor School of Computing Sciences, VIT University Vellore, Tamilnadu, India

IJCSI Reviewers Committee 2010  Mr. Markus Schatten, University of Zagreb, Faculty of Organization and Informatics, Croatia  Mr. Vassilis Papataxiarhis, Department of Informatics and Telecommunications, National and Kapodistrian University of Athens, Athens, Greece  Dr Modestos Stavrakis, University of the Aegean, Greece  Dr Fadi KHALIL, LAAS -- CNRS Laboratory, France  Dr Dimitar Trajanov, Faculty of Electrical Engineering and Information technologies, ss. Cyril and Methodius Univesity - Skopje, Macedonia  Dr Jinping Yuan, College of Information System and Management,National Univ. of Defense Tech., China  Dr Alexis Lazanas, Ministry of Education, Greece  Dr Stavroula Mougiakakou, University of Bern, ARTORG Center for Biomedical Engineering Research, Switzerland  Dr Cyril de Runz, CReSTIC-SIC, IUT de Reims, University of Reims, France  Mr. Pramodkumar P. Gupta, Dept of Bioinformatics, Dr D Y Patil University, India  Dr Alireza Fereidunian, School of ECE, University of Tehran, Iran  Mr. Fred Viezens, Otto-Von-Guericke-University Magdeburg, Germany  Dr. Richard G. Bush, Lawrence Technological University, United States  Dr. Ola Osunkoya, Information Security Architect, USA  Mr. Kotsokostas N.Antonios, TEI Piraeus, Hellas  Prof Steven Totosy de Zepetnek, U of Halle-Wittenberg & Purdue U & National Sun Yat-sen U, Germany, USA, Taiwan  Mr. M Arif Siddiqui, Najran University, Saudi Arabia  Ms. Ilknur Icke, The Graduate Center, City University of New York, USA  Prof Miroslav Baca, Faculty of Organization and Informatics, University of Zagreb, Croatia  Dr. Elvia Ruiz Beltrán, Instituto Tecnológico de Aguascalientes, Mexico  Mr. Moustafa Banbouk, Engineer du Telecom, UAE  Mr. Kevin P. Monaghan, Wayne State University, Detroit, Michigan, USA  Ms. Moira Stephens, University of Sydney, Australia  Ms. Maryam Feily, National Advanced IPv6 Centre of Excellence (NAV6) , Universiti Sains Malaysia (USM), Malaysia  Dr. Constantine YIALOURIS, Informatics Laboratory Agricultural University of Athens, Greece  Mrs. Angeles Abella, U. de Montreal, Canada  Dr. Patrizio Arrigo, CNR ISMAC, italy  Mr. Anirban Mukhopadhyay, B.P.Poddar Institute of Management & Technology, India  Mr. Dinesh Kumar, DAV Institute of Engineering & Technology, India  Mr. Jorge L. Hernandez-Ardieta, INDRA SISTEMAS / University Carlos III of Madrid, Spain  Mr. AliReza Shahrestani, University of Malaya (UM), National Advanced IPv6 Centre of Excellence (NAv6), Malaysia  Mr. Blagoj Ristevski, Faculty of Administration and Information Systems Management - Bitola, Republic of Macedonia  Mr. Mauricio Egidio Cantão, Department of Computer Science / University of São Paulo, Brazil  Mr. Jules Ruis, Fractal Consultancy, The Netherlands

 Mr. Mohammad Iftekhar Husain, University at Buffalo, USA  Dr. Deepak Laxmi Narasimha, Department of Software Engineering, Faculty of Computer Science and Information Technology, University of Malaya, Malaysia  Dr. Paola Di Maio, DMEM University of Strathclyde, UK  Dr. Bhanu Pratap Singh, Institute of Instrumentation Engineering, Kurukshetra University Kurukshetra, India  Mr. Sana Ullah, Inha University, South Korea  Mr. Cornelis Pieter Pieters, Condast, The Netherlands  Dr. Amogh Kavimandan, The MathWorks Inc., USA  Dr. Zhinan Zhou, Samsung Telecommunications America, USA  Mr. Alberto de Santos Sierra, Universidad Politécnica de Madrid, Spain  Dr. Md. Atiqur Rahman Ahad, Department of Applied Physics, Electronics & Communication Engineering (APECE), University of Dhaka, Bangladesh  Dr. Charalampos Bratsas, Lab of Medical Informatics, Medical Faculty, Aristotle University, Thessaloniki, Greece  Ms. Alexia Dini Kounoudes, Cyprus University of Technology, Cyprus  Mr. Anthony Gesase, University of Dar es salaam Computing Centre, Tanzania  Dr. Jorge A. Ruiz-Vanoye, Universidad Juárez Autónoma de Tabasco, Mexico  Dr. Alejandro Fuentes Penna, Universidad Popular Autónoma del Estado de Puebla, México  Dr. Ocotlán Díaz-Parra, Universidad Juárez Autónoma de Tabasco, México  Mrs. Nantia Iakovidou, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, Greece  Mr. Vinay Chopra, DAV Institute of Engineering & Technology, Jalandhar  Ms. Carmen Lastres, Universidad Politécnica de Madrid - Centre for Smart Environments, Spain  Dr. Sanja Lazarova-Molnar, United Arab Emirates University, UAE  Mr. Srikrishna Nudurumati, Imaging & Printing Group R&D Hub, Hewlett-Packard, India  Dr. Olivier Nocent, CReSTIC/SIC, University of Reims, France  Mr. Burak Cizmeci, Isik University, Turkey  Dr. Carlos Jaime Barrios Hernandez, LIG (Laboratory Of Informatics of Grenoble), France  Mr. Md. Rabiul Islam, Rajshahi university of Engineering & Technology (RUET), Bangladesh  Dr. LAKHOUA Mohamed Najeh, ISSAT - Laboratory of Analysis and Control of Systems, Tunisia  Dr. Alessandro Lavacchi, Department of Chemistry - University of Firenze, Italy  Mr. Mungwe, University of Oldenburg, Germany  Mr. Somnath Tagore, Dr D Y Patil University, India  Ms. Xueqin Wang, ATCS, USA  Dr. Borislav D Dimitrov, Department of General Practice, Royal College of Surgeons in Ireland, Dublin, Ireland  Dr. Fondjo Fotou Franklin, Langston University, USA  Dr. Vishal Goyal, Department of Computer Science, Punjabi University, Patiala, India  Mr. Thomas J. Clancy, ACM, United States  Dr. Ahmed Nabih Zaki Rashed, Dr. in Electronic Engineering, Faculty of Electronic Engineering, menouf 32951, Electronics and Electrical Communication Engineering Department, Menoufia university, EGYPT, EGYPT  Dr. Rushed Kanawati, LIPN, France  Mr. Koteshwar Rao, K G Reddy College Of ENGG.&TECH,CHILKUR, RR DIST.,AP, India

 Mr. M. Nagesh Kumar, Department of Electronics and Communication, J.S.S. research foundation, Mysore University, Mysore-6, India  Dr. Ibrahim Noha, Grenoble Informatics Laboratory, France  Mr. Muhammad Yasir Qadri, University of Essex, UK  Mr. Annadurai .P, KMCPGS, Lawspet, Pondicherry, India, (Aff. Pondicherry Univeristy, India  Mr. E Munivel , CEDTI (Govt. of India), India  Dr. Chitra Ganesh Desai, University of Pune, India  Mr. Syed, Analytical Services & Materials, Inc., USA  Dr. Mashud Kabir, Department of Computer Science, University of Tuebingen, Germany  Mrs. Payal N. Raj, Veer South Gujarat University, India  Mrs. Priti Maheshwary, Maulana Azad National Institute of Technology, Bhopal, India  Mr. Mahesh Goyani, S.P. University, India, India  Mr. Vinay Verma, Defence Avionics Research Establishment, DRDO, India  Dr. George A. Papakostas, Democritus University of Thrace, Greece  Mr. Abhijit Sanjiv Kulkarni, DARE, DRDO, India  Mr. Kavi Kumar Khedo, University of Mauritius, Mauritius  Dr. B. Sivaselvan, Indian Institute of Information Technology, Design & Manufacturing, Kancheepuram, IIT Madras Campus, India  Dr. Partha Pratim Bhattacharya, Greater Kolkata College of Engineering and Management, West Bengal University of Technology, India  Mr. Manish Maheshwari, Makhanlal C University of Journalism & Communication, India  Dr. Siddhartha Kumar Khaitan, Iowa State University, USA  Dr. Mandhapati Raju, General Motors Inc, USA  Dr. M.Iqbal Saripan, Universiti Putra Malaysia, Malaysia  Mr. Ahmad Shukri Mohd Noor, University Malaysia Terengganu, Malaysia  Mr. Selvakuberan K, TATA Consultancy Services, India  Dr. Smita Rajpal, Institute of Technology and Management, Gurgaon, India  Mr. Rakesh Kachroo, Tata Consultancy Services, India  Mr. Raman Kumar, National Institute of Technology, Jalandhar, Punjab., India  Mr. Nitesh Sureja, S.P.University, India  Dr. M. Emre Celebi, Louisiana State University, Shreveport, USA  Dr. Aung Kyaw Oo, Defence Services Academy, Myanmar  Mr. Sanjay P. Patel, Sankalchand Patel College of Engineering, Visnagar, Gujarat, India  Dr. Pascal Fallavollita, Queens University, Canada  Mr. Jitendra Agrawal, Rajiv Gandhi Technological University, Bhopal, MP, India  Mr. Ismael Rafael Ponce Medellín, Cenidet (Centro Nacional de Investigación y Desarrollo Tecnológico), Mexico  Mr. Supheakmungkol SARIN, Waseda University, Japan  Mr. Shoukat Ullah, Govt. Post Graduate College Bannu, Pakistan  Dr. Vivian Augustine, Telecom Zimbabwe, Zimbabwe  Mrs. Mutalli Vatila, Offshore Business Philipines, Philipines  Dr. Emanuele Goldoni, University of Pavia, Dept. of Electronics, TLC & Networking Lab, Italy  Mr. Pankaj Kumar, SAMA, India  Dr. Himanshu Aggarwal, Punjabi University,Patiala, India  Dr. Vauvert Guillaume, Europages, France

 Prof Yee Ming Chen, Department of Industrial Engineering and Management, Yuan Ze University, Taiwan  Dr. Constantino Malagón, Nebrija University, Spain  Prof Kanwalvir Singh Dhindsa, B.B.S.B.Engg.College, Fatehgarh Sahib (Punjab), India  Mr. Angkoon Phinyomark, Prince of Singkla University, Thailand  Ms. Nital H. Mistry, Veer Narmad South Gujarat University, Surat, India  Dr. M.R.Sumalatha, Anna University, India  Mr. Somesh Kumar Dewangan, Disha Institute of Management and Technology, India  Mr. Raman Maini, Punjabi University, Patiala(Punjab)-147002, India  Dr. Abdelkader Outtagarts, Alcatel-Lucent Bell-Labs, France  Prof Dr. Abdul Wahid, AKG Engg. College, Ghaziabad, India  Mr. Prabu Mohandas, Anna University/Adhiyamaan College of Engineering, india  Dr. Manish Kumar Jindal, Panjab University Regional Centre, Muktsar, India  Prof Mydhili K Nair, M S Ramaiah Institute of Technnology, Bangalore, India  Dr. C. Suresh Gnana Dhas, VelTech MultiTech Dr.Rangarajan Dr.Sagunthala Engineering College,Chennai,Tamilnadu, India  Prof Akash Rajak, Krishna Institute of Engineering and Technology, Ghaziabad, India  Mr. Ajay Kumar Shrivastava, Krishna Institute of Engineering & Technology, Ghaziabad, India  Mr. Deo Prakash, SMVD University, Kakryal(J&K), India  Dr. Vu Thanh Nguyen, University of Information Technology HoChiMinh City, VietNam  Prof Deo Prakash, SMVD University (A Technical University open on I.I.T. Pattern) Kakryal (J&K), India  Dr. Navneet Agrawal, Dept. of ECE, College of Technology & Engineering, MPUAT, Udaipur 313001 Rajasthan, India  Mr. Sufal Das, Sikkim Manipal Institute of Technology, India  Mr. Anil Kumar, Sikkim Manipal Institute of Technology, India  Dr. B. Prasanalakshmi, King Saud University, Saudi Arabia.  Dr. K D Verma, S.V. (P.G.) College, Aligarh, India  Mr. Mohd Nazri Ismail, System and Networking Department, University of Kuala Lumpur (UniKL), Malaysia  Dr. Nguyen Tuan Dang, University of Information Technology, Vietnam National University Ho Chi Minh city, Vietnam  Dr. Abdul Aziz, University of Central Punjab, Pakistan  Dr. P. Vasudeva Reddy, Andhra University, India  Mrs. Savvas A. Chatzichristofis, Democritus University of Thrace, Greece  Mr. Marcio Dorn, Federal University of Rio Grande do Sul - UFRGS Institute of Informatics, Brazil  Mr. Luca Mazzola, University of Lugano, Switzerland  Mr. Nadeem Mahmood, Department of Computer Science, University of Karachi, Pakistan  Mr. Hafeez Ullah Amin, Kohat University of Science & Technology, Pakistan  Dr. Professor Vikram Singh, Ch. Devi Lal University, Sirsa (Haryana), India  Mr. M. Azath, Calicut/Mets School of Enginerring, India  Dr. J. Hanumanthappa, DoS in CS, University of Mysore, India  Dr. Shahanawaj Ahamad, Department of Computer Science, King Saud University, Saudi Arabia  Dr. K. Duraiswamy, K. S. Rangasamy College of Technology, India  Prof. Dr Mazlina Esa, Universiti Teknologi Malaysia, Malaysia

 Dr. P. Vasant, Power Control Optimization (Global), Malaysia  Dr. Taner Tuncer, Firat University, Turkey  Dr. Norrozila Sulaiman, University Malaysia Pahang, Malaysia  Prof. S K Gupta, BCET, Guradspur, India  Dr. Latha Parameswaran, Amrita Vishwa Vidyapeetham, India  Mr. M. Azath, Anna University, India  Dr. P. Suresh Varma, Adikavi Nannaya University, India  Prof. V. N. Kamalesh, JSS Academy of Technical Education, India  Dr. D Gunaseelan, Ibri College of Technology, Oman  Mr. Sanjay Kumar Anand, CDAC, India  Mr. Akshat Verma, CDAC, India  Mrs. Fazeela Tunnisa, Najran University, Kingdom of Saudi Arabia  Mr. Hasan Asil, Islamic Azad University Tabriz Branch (Azarshahr), Iran  Prof. Dr Sajal Kabiraj, Fr. C Rodrigues Institute of Management Studies (Affiliated to University of Mumbai, India), India  Mr. Syed Fawad Mustafa, GAC Center, Shandong University, China  Dr. Natarajan Meghanathan, Jackson State University, Jackson, MS, USA  Prof. Selvakani Kandeeban, Francis Xavier Engineering College, India  Mr. Tohid Sedghi, Urmia University, Iran  Dr. S. Sasikumar, PSNA College of Engg and Tech, Dindigul, India  Dr. Anupam Shukla, Indian Institute of Information Technology and Management Gwalior, India  Mr. Rahul Kala, Indian Institute of Inforamtion Technology and Management Gwalior, India  Dr. A V Nikolov, National University of Lesotho, Lesotho  Mr. Kamal Sarkar, Department of Computer Science and Engineering, Jadavpur University, India  Dr. Mokhled S. AlTarawneh, Computer Engineering Dept., Faculty of Engineering, Mutah University, Jordan, Jordan  Prof. Sattar J Aboud, Iraqi Council of Representatives, Iraq-Baghdad  Dr. Prasant Kumar Pattnaik, Department of CSE, KIST, India  Dr. Mohammed Amoon, King Saud University, Saudi Arabia  Dr. Tsvetanka Georgieva, Department of Information Technologies, St. Cyril and St. Methodius University of Veliko Tarnovo, Bulgaria  Dr. Eva Volna, University of Ostrava, Czech Republic  Mr. Ujjal Marjit, University of Kalyani, West-Bengal, India  Dr. Prasant Kumar Pattnaik, KIST,Bhubaneswar,India, India  Dr. Guezouri Mustapha, Department of Electronics, Faculty of Electrical Engineering, University of Science and Technology (USTO), Oran, Algeria  Mr. Maniyar Shiraz Ahmed, Najran University, Najran, Saudi Arabia  Dr. Sreedhar Reddy, JNTU, SSIETW, Hyderabad, India  Mr. Bala Dhandayuthapani Veerasamy, Mekelle University, Ethiopa  Mr. Arash Habibi Lashkari, University of Malaya (UM), Malaysia  Mr. Rajesh Prasad, LDC Institute of Technical Studies, Allahabad, India  Ms. Habib Izadkhah, Tabriz University, Iran  Dr. Lokesh Kumar Sharma, Chhattisgarh Swami Vivekanand Technical University Bhilai, India  Mr. Kuldeep Yadav, IIIT Delhi, India  Dr. Naoufel Kraiem, Institut Superieur d'Informatique, Tunisia

 Prof. Frank Ortmeier, Otto-von-Guericke-Universitaet Magdeburg, Germany  Mr. Ashraf Aljammal, USM, Malaysia  Mrs. Amandeep Kaur, Department of Computer Science, Punjabi University, Patiala, Punjab, India  Mr. Babak Basharirad, University Technology of Malaysia, Malaysia  Mr. Avinash singh, Kiet Ghaziabad, India  Dr. Miguel Vargas-Lombardo, Technological University of Panama, Panama  Dr. Tuncay Sevindik, Firat University, Turkey  Ms. Pavai Kandavelu, Anna University Chennai, India  Mr. Ravish Khichar, Global Institute of Technology, India  Mr Aos Alaa Zaidan Ansaef, Multimedia University, Cyberjaya, Malaysia  Dr. Awadhesh Kumar Sharma, Dept. of CSE, MMM Engg College, Gorakhpur-273010, UP, India  Mr. Qasim Siddique, FUIEMS, Pakistan  Dr. Le Hoang Thai, University of Science, Vietnam National University - Ho Chi Minh City, Vietnam  Dr. Saravanan C, NIT, Durgapur, India  Dr. Vijay Kumar Mago, DAV College, Jalandhar, India  Dr. Do Van Nhon, University of Information Technology, Vietnam  Mr. Georgios Kioumourtzis, University of Patras, Greece  Mr. Amol D.Potgantwar, SITRC Nasik, India  Mr. Lesedi Melton Masisi, Council for Scientific and Industrial Research, South Africa  Dr. Karthik.S, Department of Computer Science & Engineering, SNS College of Technology, India  Mr. Nafiz Imtiaz Bin Hamid, Department of Electrical and Electronic Engineering, Islamic University of Technology (IUT), Bangladesh  Mr. Muhammad Imran Khan, Universiti Teknologi PETRONAS, Malaysia  Dr. Abdul Kareem M. Radhi, Information Engineering - Nahrin University, Iraq  Dr. Mohd Nazri Ismail, University of Kuala Lumpur, Malaysia  Dr. Manuj Darbari, BBDNITM, Institute of Technology, A-649, Indira Nagar, Lucknow 226016, India  Ms. Izerrouken, INP-IRIT, France  Mr. Nitin Ashokrao Naik, Dept. of Computer Science, Yeshwant Mahavidyalaya, Nanded, India  Mr. Nikhil Raj, National Institute of Technology, Kurukshetra, India  Prof. Maher Ben Jemaa, National School of Engineers of Sfax, Tunisia  Prof. Rajeshwar Singh, BRCM College of Engineering and Technology, Bahal Bhiwani, Haryana, India  Mr. Gaurav Kumar, Department of Computer Applications, Chitkara Institute of Engineering and Technology, Rajpura, Punjab, India  Mr. Ajeet Kumar Pandey, Indian Institute of Technology, Kharagpur, India  Mr. Rajiv Phougat, IBM Corporation, USA  Mrs. Aysha V, College of Applied Science Pattuvam affiliated with Kannur University, India  Dr. Debotosh Bhattacharjee, Department of Computer Science and Engineering, Jadavpur University, Kolkata-700032, India  Dr. Neelam Srivastava, Institute of engineering & Technology, Lucknow, India  Prof. Sweta Verma, Galgotia's College of Engineering & Technology, Greater Noida, India  Mr. Harminder Singh BIndra, MIMIT, INDIA  Dr. Lokesh Kumar Sharma, Chhattisgarh Swami Vivekanand Technical University, Bhilai, India  Mr. Tarun Kumar, U.P. Technical University/Radha Govinend Engg. College, India  Mr. Tirthraj Rai, Jawahar Lal Nehru University, New Delhi, India

 Mr. Akhilesh Tiwari, Madhav Institute of Technology & Science, India  Mr. Dakshina Ranjan Kisku, Dr. B. C. Roy Engineering College, WBUT, India  Ms. Anu Suneja, Maharshi Markandeshwar University, Mullana, Haryana, India  Mr. Munish Kumar Jindal, Punjabi University Regional Centre, Jaito (Faridkot), India  Dr. Ashraf Bany Mohammed, Management Information Systems Department, Faculty of Administrative and Financial Sciences, Petra University, Jordan  Mrs. Jyoti Jain, R.G.P.V. Bhopal, India  Dr. Lamia Chaari, SFAX University, Tunisia  Mr. Akhter Raza Syed, Department of Computer Science, University of Karachi, Pakistan  Prof. Khubaib Ahmed Qureshi, Information Technology Department, HIMS, Hamdard University, Pakistan  Prof. Boubker Sbihi, Ecole des Sciences de L'Information, Morocco  Dr. S. M. Riazul Islam, Inha University, South Korea  Prof. Lokhande S.N., S.R.T.M.University, Nanded (MH), India  Dr. Vijay H Mankar, Dept. of Electronics, Govt. Polytechnic, Nagpur, India  Dr. M. Sreedhar Reddy, JNTU, Hyderabad, SSIETW, India  Mr. Ojesanmi Olusegun, Ajayi Crowther University, Oyo, Nigeria  Ms. Mamta Juneja, RBIEBT, PTU, India  Dr. Ekta Walia Bhullar, Maharishi Markandeshwar University, Mullana Ambala (Haryana), India  Prof. Chandra Mohan, John Bosco Engineering College, India  Mr. Nitin A. Naik, Yeshwant Mahavidyalaya, Nanded, India  Mr. Sunil Kashibarao Nayak, Bahirji Smarak Mahavidyalaya, Basmathnagar Dist-Hingoli., India  Prof. Rakesh.L, Vijetha Institute of Technology, Bangalore, India  Mr B. M. Patil, Indian Institute of Technology, Roorkee, Uttarakhand, India  Mr. Thipendra Pal Singh, Sharda University, K.P. III, Greater Noida, Uttar Pradesh, India  Prof. Chandra Mohan, John Bosco Engg College, India  Mr. Hadi Saboohi, University of Malaya - Faculty of Computer Science and Information Technology, Malaysia  Dr. R. Baskaran, Anna University, India  Dr. Wichian Sittiprapaporn, Mahasarakham University College of Music, Thailand  Mr. Lai Khin Wee, Universiti Teknologi Malaysia, Malaysia  Dr. Kamaljit I. Lakhtaria, Atmiya Institute of Technology, India  Mrs. Inderpreet Kaur, PTU, Jalandhar, India  Mr. Iqbaldeep Kaur, PTU / RBIEBT, India  Mrs. Vasudha Bahl, Maharaja Agrasen Institute of Technology, Delhi, India  Prof. Vinay Uttamrao Kale, P.R.M. Institute of Technology & Research, Badnera, Amravati, Maharashtra, India  Mr. Suhas J Manangi, Microsoft, India  Ms. Anna Kuzio, Adam Mickiewicz University, School of English, Poland  Dr. Debojyoti Mitra, Sir Padampat Singhania University, India  Prof. Rachit Garg, Department of Computer Science, L K College, India  Mrs. Manjula K A, Kannur University, India  Mr. Rakesh Kumar, Indian Institute of Technology Roorkee, India

TABLE OF CONTENTS

1. Music Sound and Picture Perception: Topography of the Human Brain Electrical Activity Wichian Sittiprapaporn

Pg 1-9

2. Fast Clustering of Self-Similar Network Traffic Using Wavelet Aqil Burney, Akhter Raza Syed, Afzal Saleemi

Pg 10-17

3. Quotient Based Multiresolution Image Fusion of Thermal and Visual Images Using Daubechies Wavelet Transform for Human Face Recognition Mrinal Kanti Bhowmik, Debotosh Bhattacharjee, Mita Nasipuri, Dipak Kumar Basu, Mahantapas Kundu

Pg 18-27

4. Extraction of Dominant Attributes and Guidance Rules for Scholastic Achievement Using Rough Set Theory in Data Mining P.G. JansiRani, R. Bhaskaran

Pg 28-37

5. Performance Evaluation of Two Reactive Routing Protocols of MANET using Group Mobility Model Harminder S. Bindra, Sunil K. Maakar, A. L. Sangal

Pg 38-43

6. Policy based Decentralized Group key Security for Mobile Ad-hoc Networks Sugandha Singh, Navin Rajpal, Ashok Kale Sharma, Ritu Pahwa

Pg 44-49

IJCSI International Journal of Computer Science Issues, Vol. 7, Issue 3, No 10, May 2010 ISSN (Online): 1694-0784 ISSN (Print): 1694-0814

1

Music Sound and Picture Perception: Topography of the Human Brain Electrical Activity Wichian Sittiprapaporn College of Music, Mahasarakham University Khamriang Sub-District, Kantharawichai District, Mahasarakham 44150, THAILAND

Abstract The functional properties and topographic distribution of eventrelated potential (ERP) components elicited by simultaneously music sound and picture discrimination were investigated. Simultaneous audio-visual stimulus in the oddball paradigm was used to re-examine the MMN occurrence in auditory, visual and audiovisual modalities. This study was designed to investigate whether task-related processing of visual and auditory features was independent or task-related processing in one modality might influence the processing of the other. The grand-average deviant-related components producing deviant-related negativities (DRNs) divided into and early DRN1 around 100200 ms and late DRN2 around 200-300 ms. Two ERP components were found: MMN associated with DRN1, and N2b associated with DRN2. MMN and N2b were more negative when a stimulus was a target, showing the selection negativity effect. Feature-specific effects on component amplitude or topography varied by component. ANOVA shows that the interaction between electrode site and modality of MMN amplitudes at 100200 ms was statistically significant. The difference waves with 100-200 ms latency at the anterior sites were markedly different to the posterior sites. In visual modality, there was no MMN elicitation in the posterior sites compared to the auditory and audiovisual modalities. The emergence of posterior negativity (MMN) in the present study is thus not to be attributed to visual discrimination process. These data provide topographic evidence that ERP components in the 100-300 ms time domain can be differentiated on the basis to proceeding of specific stimuli features, and reflect neurophysiologically distinct auditory and visual pathways in the human cortex.

Keywords: Event-related potentials, Sound, Music, Mismatch negativity, bisensory processing

1. Introduction Event-related potential (ERP) recordings have bought new insight to the neuronal events behind auditory change detection in audition of the human brain function. Theoretically, ERPs components reflect the conscious

detection of a physical, semantic, or syntactic deviation from the expected sounds [1]. The ERP recordings thus allow one to probe the neural processes preceding the involvement of the attentional mechanisms. For instances, ERPs have been recorded that reflect memory traces representing sounds composed of several simultaneous or successive tonal elements [2-4]. Event-related Potential (ERP) components are here labeled by the polarity (negativity or positive) and temporal order of appearance. The ERP components elicited by visual discrimination stimuli with long inter-stimulus intervals (ISI > 500 ms) include the P1, N1, P2, N2, and P3 components [5]. In visual ERP components elicited during discrimination tasks, the peak latency ranges for the ERP components are typically in the following ranges: P1 (80-145 ms), N1 (100-200 ms), P2 (200-300 ms), N2 (200-300 ms), and P3 (280-600 ms) [5]. Previous studies demonstrated that attention directed toward a specific visual region enhances the P1 and N1 components to stimuli in that region [5]. Importantly, this effect was also largest over posterior electrode sites contralateral to the attended region. The electrical sources of P1 and N1 probably include both striate and lateral occipital cortex [6-15]. In addition to the exogenous N1 and P2 components, the N2 and P3 components increase with the difficulty of discrimination and classification [11,16-22]. The occurrence of both N2 and P3 components reflect operations on a representation of the stimulus [16,23-28]. The auditory system has a remarkable ability to establish memory traces for invariant features of acoustic sounds in the environment such as human speech sound and music, in order to correct the interpretation of these natural acoustic sounds heard [1]. Mismatch negativity (MMN) component of ERP elicited in the auditory cortex when incoming sounds are detected as deviating from a neural representation of acoustic regularities. It is mainly generated in the auditory cortex occurring between 100 to 250 ms [29] and thus long been regarded as specific to the auditory modality [30-31]. MMN implies the existence of an auditory sensory memory that stores a neural

IJCSI International Journal of Computer Science Issues, Vol. 7, Issue 3, No 10, May 2010 www.IJCSI.org representation of a standard against which any incoming auditory input is compared [32-33]. Although it is clear that the MMN can be elicited in auditory modality in the absence of attention, it remains somewhat unclear whether there is an analogous automatic deviant-related negativity (DRN) elicited outside the auditory modality. Even though Näätänen [34] stated that “no MMN appears to occur in the visual modality”, several studies have shown that visual stimuli deviating from repetitive visual standards can also elicit a visual analogue of the MMN in the same latency range. This visual MMN seems to be mainly generated in occipital areas [35] with possibly a more anterior component [36-37]. Cammann’s study [38] also showed a widely distributed MMN change between 150 and 350 ms, with a parietal maximum suggesting that this MMN may occur in the visual modality. Cross-modal attention studies clearly showed that deviant visual stimuli elicited MMN, largest over the inferior temporal cortex. This visual MMN increased in amplitude with attention, but it was also evident during inattention [39-40]. Moreover, Pazo-Alvarez et al., [41] reviewed several previous reports to provide convincing evidence for the existence of this visual MMN. However, Alho et al., [42] suggested that the effect of target-specific negativity is a considerable contamination factors in which the impact of simultaneous memory traces in different modalities could also be considered. The present study compared attentional non-musician brain processes during the discrimination of the different synthesized acoustic sounds by using a modified (four acoustic sounds) auditory oddball paradigm to record ERP components in a group of healthy subjects, hoping to find evidence for specific brain signatures of acoustic sound and picture processing in the human brain. The simultaneous audio-visual stimulus in the oddball paradigm was used to re-examine the effects of attention on MMN in auditory, visual and audiovisual dimensions. Attentional ERP components were analyzed in a situation where target stimuli were combinations of both auditory and visual features. Interactive processing of stimulus features would then be indicated by the absence, reduction or early termination of the attention-related components [43] as a function of processing of the other feature. If visual-specific components are evoked by visual deviances, then the present audio-visual paradigm will help to separate them from the effect of visual information on the auditory-specific MMN process by facilitating the focus of attention on auditory and visual MMNs elicited with bimodal features. The audio-visual paradigm was also designed to investigate whether task-related processing of visual and auditory features was independent or taskrelated processing in one modality might influence the

2

processing of the other. This study used a 28 channel electrode montage to characterize the scalp topography of these effects. Advanced reference-independent topographical analysis was used to identify the ERP components. The topographic analysis was used to locate multiple non-dipolar sources particularly involved in the discrimination of these different synthesized acoustic sounds and picture perception as well as characterizing the cortical distribution of the ERP electrical generators.

2. Materials and Methods 2.1 Subjects Sixteen subjects (9 female and 7 male) between the ages of 22 and 26 were evaluated. All subjects were right-handed (handedness assessed according to Oldfield [45]), with no history of neurological or psychiatric hospitalization, substance or alcohol abuse, or medical illness that might affect visual function. All subjects had normal or corrected-to-normal vision (self reported). None of them had more than three years of formal musical training and none had any musical training within the past five years. Approval of the institutional committee on human research and written consent from each subject were obtained prior to data acquisition. After a complete description of the intended study, written informed consent was obtained. The subjects were paid for their participation.

2.2 Materials Stimuli were presented on a computer screen (640 x 480 pixel resolutions) placed 100 cm from the subject for simultaneously audiovisual presentation and discrimination tasks. A stimulus system (STIM2, Neurosoft, Inc. Sterling, USA) was employed for controlling the presentation of the stimuli. A set of four synthesized acoustic sounds were distinguished by frequencies (Hz). All acoustic sounds were digitally edited to have an equal maximum energy level in dB SPL with the remaining intensity level within each of the stimuli scaled accordingly. The stimuli were digitally edited using the Cool Edit Pro v. 2.0 (Syntrillium Software Cooperation) with 300 ms duration. All four acoustic sounds were binaurally presented through headphones at a comfortable listening level of ~85 dB, with different pitch contour: Tone 1 with high-level, Tone 2 with high-rising, and Tone 3 with low-dipping (10ms rising/falling time, 200 ms plateau). The sound pressure levels of stimuli were then measured at the output of headphones using a Brüel and Kjaer 2230 sound level meter. ERPs were recorded during a four-tone auditory oddball paradigm. The deviant was ‘X’ with 1,800Hz tone (VTAT; Visual Target

IJCSI International Journal of Computer Science Issues, Vol. 7, Issue 3, No 10, May 2010 www.IJCSI.org Auditory Target) in 20% probability, and the standard was ‘Y’ with 800Hz tone (VSAS; Visual Non-target Auditory Non-target) in 60% probability. Additionally, the ‘X’ with 800Hz tone (VTAS; Visual Target Auditory Non-target) and ‘Y’ with 1,800Hz tone (VSAT; Visual Non-target Auditory Target) were used in 10% probabilities. To be able to check that subjects were attending the stimuli, the number of target stimuli was reported at the end of the experiment. EEG signal recording was time-locked to the onset of the stimuli. Subjects were instructed to press button when reacting to the target stimuli only. The experiment lasted 1-2 h, including breaks.

2.3 Test Procedures Subjects were tested in a quiet, dimly lit and electrically shielded room. The subject was seated in an adjustable reclining chair, with his or her forehead 100 cm from the computer screen where the stimuli were presented. At the beginning of each block, the subject was instructed to press a key with his or her right hand when the target stimulus appeared (e.g., VTAT), and to ignore other stimuli. Prior to the experimental session, a practice block was administrated to ensure that the subjects understood the task. The subject was also asked to respond as quickly and accurately as possible. The experiment was consisted of 3 blocks and each block had 300 trials. Every stimulus was presented with 300ms exposure duration and interstimulus interval was 1,800ms in every condition. Stimuli were randomly presented with a block. The accuracy of each trial was recorded, and mean value were compared between conditions. There was a one to two minute interval between blocks. Tasks and blocks were counterbalanced across subjects.

2.4 EEG Recordings Electroencephalographic (EEG) was recorded from a Quick-Cap equipped with 28 channels according to the international 10-20 system using Scan system (Scan 4.2, Neurosoft, Inc. Sterling, USA). Linked mastoids were used as reference. Horizontal and vertical electro-oculograms (HEOG and VEOG) were also recorded for artifact correction. Impedance was maintained at 5kΩ or less. During the experiment, EEG was amplified with a band pass of 0.05 - 100 Hz, sampled at 1,000 Hz and stored on a hard disk for off-line analysis. ERPs were averaged separately for each types of stimulus. They were digitally filtered with a band pass of 0.1 – 30 Hz. The averaging epoch was 900 ms, and the 100 ms before the onset of the presenting stimuli served as baseline. The artifacts rejection was conducted in all channels with threshold of ± 100 µV before averaging. Single trial ERPs were saved

3

and subsequently processed off-line. Off-line processing included baseline correction using the averaged baseline voltage for epoch at each recording channel.

2.5 Data Analysis Four midline electrodes (Fz, Cz, Pz and Oz) were used to assess anterior-posterior ERP component topography. Two electrodes on each side of the head were chosen from the left (F7, T3, T5, O1) and right (F8, T4, T6, O2) lateral electrode chains. The lateral electrodes were symmetrically placed in the region of largest component amplitude, which allowed testing for lateralized differences in voltage topography. For analysis of processing time, ERP component latency was measured at the electrode site where the mean component amplitude was maximal or near maximal across conditions. Peak amplitude and latency values were used to measure components in this study. These data provide a topographic description of these peak-related components. To analyze the deviant-related components (DRC), difference potentials were calculated where responses elicited by the VSAS stimuli were subtracted from responses to VTAS and VSAT stimuli referred to visual (Vi) and auditory (Au) modalities, respectively. In the audiovisual (AV) modality, VTAT minus VSAS difference was also calculated. MMNs were statistically assessed by t-tests comparing the averaged amplitude of the deviant minus standard difference waveform to zero in the 40 ms time-window around the latency of the peak in the grand-average responses. To compare these components, MMN amplitudes were assessed via two-way analyses of variance (ANOVA) with repeated measurements. The factors were modality (three levels: Vi, Au and AV), and electrode site (two levels: anterior sites at F3, Fz, F4, C3, Cz, C4, and posterior sites at P3, Pz, P4, O1, Oz, O2). All amplitude measures were taken relative to average baseline voltage in the 100 ms interval prior to stimulus onset. A p value criterion of ≤ .05 was used for significance testing of main effects and interaction. When significant stimulus main effects or stimulus X electrode interactions were detected, interpolated topographic maps were generated from the mean peak voltage values across subjects in a condition to assist in interpretation of the stimulus effect. Stimulus X Electrode interactions were statistically evaluated using t-testing between conditions at electrode sites included in the ANOVA, and t-tests between electrode sites within a condition. Significance level for all statistical tests was set at p ≤ 0.05 (two-tailed).

IJCSI International Journal of Computer Science Issues, Vol. 7, Issue 3, No 10, May 2010 www.IJCSI.org

3. Results Reaction times (RTs) and response accuracy (mean ± standard deviation: SD) is shown in Table 1. No main effect was observed in behavioral data. Table 1 Mean reaction time and response accurate rates for the VTAT

4

and AV difference potentials at Fz site. The difference was statistically significant [F(2,78) = 8.75, P < 0.0001]. Like the MMN, they showed similar significant effect on the N2b amplitude at Oz [F(2,78) = 6.50, P < 0.0001]. The voltage topographies of components sensitive to feature differences of all stimuli (MMN, N2b, and P3) are shown in figure 1.

condition

------------------------------------------------------------------------------Mean SD ------------------------------------------------------------------------------Reaction time (ms) 455 102 Accurate rate (%) 98.24 2.0 -------------------------------------------------------------------------------

The grand-average deviant-related components in the Au, Vi and AV modalities producing deviant-related negativities (DRNs). The deviant-related negativities were divided into an early DRN1 around 100-200 ms and a late DRN2 around 200-300 ms. According to the previous study showing that MMN appears between 100 to 250 ms [30] and the characteristics of DRN2 match with those of N2b component [44], the present study thus associated DRN1 mainly with MMN in which we focus in this report, and DRN2 with a mixed wave of MMN and N2b.

As shown in Figure 1, the topographies with 100-200 ms latency (MMN component) sensitive to all modalities at the anterior sites were markedly different to the posterior sites. There was no MMN elicitation for the visual modality at the posterior electrode sites compared to the auditory (Au) and audiovisual (AV) modalities. The voltage topographies of the MMN component to different modalities is shown in figure 2.

Fig. 2 Voltage distribution maps showing the voltage distribution of the MMN component (blue color) to auditory (Au) and audiovisual (AV) modalities, but not in visual (Vi) modality, markedly different to the posterior sites.

Fig. 1 Topographic maps showing the interpolated voltage distributions of MMN, N2b, and P3 to auditory (Au), visual (Vi), audiovisual (AV) and audiovisual additivity (AuVi) modalities. Voltages at each of the 28 recording sites were obtained using the mean peak voltage across subjects for that component. The maps are oriented so that the posterior of the map is at the base of each map, and viewer right is subject right.

ANOVA shows that the interaction between electrode site and modality of MMN amplitudes at 100-200 ms was significant [F(11,429) = 8.27, P < 0.0001]. At 200-300 ms, significant levels were also reached in the same interaction for N2b component [F(11,429) = 6.50, P < 0.0001]. We then compared the MMN mean amplitude values of Au, Vi

On the other hand, the Vi MMN was significantly larger only in the anterior sites, being maximum at F3 (t(39) = -68.04, P < 0.0001). This result consistent with the previous study showing no posterior selection negativity elicitation in the difficulty discrimination task [46]. The emergence of posterior negativity (MMN) in the present study is thus not to be attributed to visual discrimination process. We also compared the mean amplitude values of all Au, Vi and AV difference potentials at anterior locations. ANOVA shows that the interaction between anterior electrode site and modality was significant [F(17,663) = 52.37, P < 0.0001] and significant level was also reached in the interactions between posterior electrode site and modality [F(17,663) = 27.52, P < 0.0001]. The identical N2b components were elicited by Vi and AV modalities, whereas in case of Au modality, latency of this component was longer than that of the Vi and AV modalities. The voltage topographies of the N2b component elicited by Vi and AV is shown in figure 3.

IJCSI International Journal of Computer Science Issues, Vol. 7, Issue 3, No 10, May 2010 www.IJCSI.org

5

t(39) = -230.41, p < 0.0001). The voltage topographies of the MMN component to audiovisual (AV) and audiovisual additivity (AuVi) is shown in figure 4.

Fig. 3 Voltage distribution maps showing the voltage distribution of the N2b component (blue color) to visual (Vi) and audiovisual (AV) modalities.

Moreover, the N2b to the AV modality peaked earlier than the N2b to the Vi and Au modalities, respectively. The shorter N2b latency to AV modality further suggests that the attention-related components reflect activity following elementary discrimination process. Such activity would have been different upon the different discrimination demand [43,46]. Therefore, the emergence of N2b and MMN in this study supports this view.

Fig. 4 Voltage distribution maps showing the voltage distribution of the MMN component to audiovisual (AV) and audiovisual additivity (AuVi) modalities.

The additivity of the MMN was also examined by adding together the Au and Vi MMNs and comparing this ‘modeled’ (AuVi) MMN with the AV MMN in order to see the possible attention effects on the additivity of MMN. If the processing of Au and Vi is independent of the others, the sum of the MMNs to both modalities should be equal to the MMN elicited by the AV modality. The results will suggest complex interactions between brains processes involved in analyzing several simultaneous deviant features [47]. Additivity of the Au and Vi MMNs produced slightly larger amplitude than did the corresponding AV modality, being maximum at P3 (mean amplitude; AuVi vs. AV: -1.56 (0.02) vs. -1.12 (0.02) µV,

Fig. 5 Voltage distribution maps showing the voltage distribution of the N2b component to audiovisual (AV) and audiovisual additivity (AuVi) modalities.

Figure 5 shows voltage maps for the N2b component to audiovisual (AV) and audiovisual additivity (AuVi) modalities. The N2b component, following MMN was also larger than that of the AV modality, being maximum at Fz (mean amplitude: -4.51 (0.04) vs. -3.97 (0.05) µV, t(39) = -230.41, P < 0.0001). N2b was also followed by a positive component identified as P3a [30]. The voltage topographies of the P3 component to auditory (Au), visual (Vi), audiovisual (AV), and audiovisual additivity (AuVi) modalities is shown in figure 6. As shown in Figure 5, the underadditivity of AV modality in these attention-related components suggests either that common neural populations are involved in the controlled processing of changes in different features [48] or that the populations are separate but strongly interacting [47]. This result was in the line of previous studies showing processing of a feature, hierarchically dependent on another feature [43,46].

Fig. 6 Voltage distribution maps showing the voltage distribution of the P3 component to auditory (Au), visual (Vi), audiovisual (AV) and audiovisual additivity (AuVi) modalities.

IJCSI International Journal of Computer Science Issues, Vol. 7, Issue 3, No 10, May 2010 www.IJCSI.org

4. Discussion The main finding of this study indicates that the prominent response to Au, Vi, and Av modalities producing DRNs at 100 to 300 ms from stimulus onset. The deviant-related negativities were divided into an early DRN1 around 100-200 ms associated mainly with the MMN component and a late DRN2 around 200-300 ms associated with the mixed ERP components between the MMN and the N2b. The magnitude of the N2b appearance was followed by a positive component identified as P3. As shown in Figure 1, there was no MMN elicitation for the Vi modality at the posterior electrode sites compared to the Au and Av modalities. These results and those of other researchers suggest that scalp-recorded visual ERP components can tap functionally distinct aspects of visual representation and processing. These data provide further support for previous study [49] that the visual selection negativity encompasses a complex ensemble of information processing operations. An ongoing challenge is to relate these temporally overlapping and transient operations revealed by ERP techniques to local neural generators [50] and to regional patterns of activation observed in rCBF studies [27,51-54]. The identical N2 component was elicited in all modalities. The N2 components measured in this study most closely correspond to the visual N2 component described by O’Donnell et al. [5], Novak et al. [21], Potts et al. [18], Simson et al. [24], and Ritter et al. [22], Luck and Hillyard [55], Harter and Aine [17]. The anterior or posterior N2 component was sensitive to both within and across feature differences in stimulus attributes [5]. Moreover, the sensitivity of N2 to variations in visual feature dimensions, including orientation, color, size, and spatial frequency [5,49,55-56]. The sensitivity of N2 to variations in stimulus features, or task demands requiring responding to different features associated with a stimulus [5]. Therefore, the anterior and posterior N2 components reflect the perceptual representation of task-relevant visual features [5]. Several studies have reported that attended to (rare target) and ignored infrequent (rare non-target) stimuli elicit different P300 components, i.e., P3a and P3b components, reflecting different attentive and integrative processes [57-60]. As shown in the figure 1, the N2b in this study was also followed by a positive component identified as P3a. According to the functional role of the two P300 components, P3a reflects a stimulus-driven attentional shift, while P3b reflects the process of effortful attentional allocation and stimulus evaluation for task relevance [61-67]. The functional significance of the P3

6

component stress its involvement in global revision of expectancies or context, such as working memory reset [28], context updating [16], and stimulus value or meaning [19,68]. In addition, the voltage distribution of the P3 component is relatively insensitive to modality of stimulation when comparing the auditory and visual modalities [5,69-70]. Some studies indicated that the variation of P3 reflected by a function of stimulus features [49,55-56], suggesting that activity in the P3 amplitude range may include subcomponents which index featurespecific processing [5]. The present findings are in the line with the hypothesis that P3a reflects automatic allocation of attention which generated in fronto-parietal regions, the cerebral network for the orienting of attention (i.e., a shift of attention towards new and/or unexpected stimuli [7175]. In the mean time, the P3b is largely independent from response selection and mainly reflects stimulus categorization activity [57,76-78]. The finding of a significantly P3 component in this study supports to the views indicating a particularly strong asymmetry between the activated neuronal generators in the two hemispheres. Therefore, the P300 response topography and latency in the present study may reveal cortical distribution and activity dynamics of the memory trace.

4. Conclusions The present study demonstrates the audio-visual interaction of music sound and picture features following elementary within-modality discrimination processes. MMN and N2b effects suggest the attention-related rechecking of the outcome of within-modality analyses. This findings support the view that the processing of a feature, hierarchically dependent on another feature. The automatic detection of the target music sound and picture may be a useful index of auditory and visual memory traces of music sound and picture attention and cognition in the human cortex. Acknowledgments This research was conducted in cooperation with Clinical Cognitive Neuroscience Center (CCNC) in the Seoul National University, College of Medicine, Seoul, Korea. The author gratefully thanks Prof. Dr. Jun Soo Kwon, Dr. Do-Hyung Kang, Dr. Kyung Whun Kang and Dr. Bo Reom Lee for their guidance and providing the equipment and data sources during this work. In addition, this research was supported by the following organizations; the Brain Research Center of the 21st Century Frontier Research Program (Ministry of Science and Technology of Republic of Korea), and the International Scholar

IJCSI International Journal of Computer Science Issues, Vol. 7, Issue 3, No 10, May 2010 www.IJCSI.org Exchange Fellowship (ISEF) Program, 2006-2007 (Korea Foundation for Advanced Studies).

References [1] R. Näätänen, A.W. Gaillard, and S. Mäntysalo, “Early selectiveattention effect on evoked potential reinterpreted”, Acta Psychol., 42, 1978, pp. 313-329. [2] R. Näätänen, “The perception of speech sounds by the human brain as reflected by the mismatch negativity (MMN) and its magnetic equivalent (MMNm)”, Psychophysiology, 38, 2001, pp. 1-21. [3] M. Jaramillo, P. Paavilainen, and R. Näätänen, “Mismatch negativity and behavioral discrimination in humans as function of the magnitude of change in sound duration”, Neurosci. Lett., 290, 2000, pp. 101-104. [4] G. Dehaene-Lambertz, “Electrophysiological correlates of categorical phoneme perception in adults”, NeuroReport, 8, 1997, pp. 919-924. [5] B.F. O’Donnell, J.M. Swearer, L.T. Smith, H. Hokama, and R.W. McCariey, “A Topographic Study of ERPs Elicited by Visual Feature Discrimination”, Brain Topogr., 10, 1997, pp. 133-143. [6] A. Dukati, E. Fava, and E.D.G. Moti, “Neuronal generators of the visual evoked potentials: intracerebral recording in awake humans”, Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol., 71, 1988, pp. 89-99. [7] S.J. Luck, and S.A. Hillyard, “Spatial filtering during visual search: Evidence from human electrophysiology”, J. Exp. Psychol.: Hum. Percept., 20, 1994, pp. 1000-1014. [8] G.R. Mangun, S.A. Hillyard, and S.J. Luck, “Electrocortical substrates of visual selective attention”, in: D. Meyer and S. Kornblum (Eds.), Attention and Performance, MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 4, 1993, pp. 219-243. [9] H.G. Vaughan, and J.C. Arezzo, “The neuronal basis of eventrelated potentials”, in: T.W. Picton (Ed.), EEG Handbook. Elsevier, New York, 3, 1988, pp. 45-96. [10] S.A. Hillyard, and T.F. Munte, “Selective attention to color and location: An analysis with event-related potentials”, Percept. Psychophys., 36, 1984, pp. 185-198. [11] K.J. Heinze, S.J. Luck, G.R. Mangun, and S.A. Hillyard, “Visual event-related potentials index focused attention within bilateral stimulus arrays. I. Evidence for early selection”, Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol., 75, 1990, pp. 511-527. [12] G.R. Mangun, and S.A. Hillyard, “The spatial allocation of visual attention as indexed by event-related brain potentials”, Hum. Factors, 29, 1978, pp. 195-212. [13] G.R. Mangun, and S.A. Hillyard, “Spatial gradients of visual attention: behavioral and electrophysiological evidence”, Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol., 70, 1988, pp. 417-428. [14] S.V. Van Voorhis, and S.A. Hillyard, “Visual evoked potentials and selective attention to points in space”, Percept. Psychophys., 22, 1977, pp. 54-62. [15] M.D. Rugg, A.D. Milner, C.R. Lines, and R. Phalp, “Modulation of visual event-related potentials by spatial and non-spatial visual selective attention”, Neuropsychologia, 25, 1987, pp. 85-96. [16] E. Donchin, and M.G.H. Coles, “Is the P300 component a manifestation of cortex updating?”, Behav. Brain Sci., 11, 1988, pp. 357-373. [17] M.R. Harter, and C.J. Aine, “Brain mechanisms of visual selective attention”, in: R. Parasuraman and D.R. Davies (Eds.), Varieties of attention. Academic Press, New York, 1984, pp. 283-320.

7

[18] G.F. Potts, M. Liotti, D.M. Tucker, and M.I. Posner, “Frontal and inferior temporal cortical activity in visual target detection: evidence from high spatially sample event-related potentials”, Brain Topogr., 9, 1996, pp. 3-14. [19] S. Sutton, and D.S. Ruchkin, “The late positive complex: Advances and new problems”, in R. Karrer, J. Cohen, and P. Tueting (Eds.), Brain and Information: Event-Related Potentials. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci., 425, 1984, pp. 1-23. [20] M. Kutas, G. McCarthy, and E. Donchin, “Augmenting mental chronometry: The P300 as a measure of stimulus evaluation time”, Science, 197, 1977, pp. 792-795. [21] G.P. Novak, W. Ritter, H.G. Vaughan, and M.I. Wiznitzer, “Differentiation of negative event-related potentials in an auditory discrimination task”, Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol., 75, 1990, pp. 255-275. [22] W. Ritter, R. Simson, H.G. Vaughan Jr., and M. Macht, “Manipulation of venet-related potential manifestations of information processing stages”, Science, 218, 1982, pp. 909-911. [23] B.F. O’Donnell, T. Hendler, and N.K. Squires, “Visual evoked potentials to illusory reversals of the Necker cube”, Psychophysiology, 25, 1988, pp. 137-143. [24] R. Simson, H.G. Vaughan, and W. Ritter, “The scalp topography of potentials associated with missing visual or auditory stimuli”, Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol., 40, 1976, pp. 33-42. [25] L.G. Ungerleider, and M. Mishkin, “Two cortical visual systems”, in: D.F. Ingle, M.A. Goodale, and R.F.W. Mansfield (Eds), Analysis of Visual Behavior. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 1982. [26] C.L. Grady, J.V. Hax, B. Horwitz, M.B. Schapiro, S.I. Rapoport, L.G. Ungerleider, M. Mishkin, R.E. Carson, and P. Herscovitch, “Dissociation of object and spatial vision in human extrastriate cortex: Age –related changes in activation of regional cerebral blood flow measured with [15O] water and positron emission tomography”, J. Cog. Neurosci., 4, 1992, pp. 23-34. [27] S. Zeki, J.D.G. Watson, C.J. Lueck, K.J. Friston, C. Kennard, and R.S.J. Frackowiak, “A direct demonstration of functional specialization in human visual cortex”, J. Neurosci., 11, 1991, pp. 641-649. [28] S. Grossberg, “Some psychophysiological and pharmacological correlates of a developmental, cognitive, and motivational theory”, In: R. Karrer, J. Cohen, and P. Tueting (Eds.), Brain and Information: Event-Related Potentials. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci., 425, 1984, pp. 58-151. [29] J.D. Kropotov, R. Näätänen, A.V. Sevostianov, K. Alho, K. Reinikainen, and O.V. Kropotova, “Mismatch negativity to auditory stimulus change recorded directly from the human temporal cortex”, Psychophysiology, 32, 1995, pp. 418-422. [30] R. Näätänen, Attention and Brain Function, Hillsdale: Erlbaum, 1992. [31] G. Nyman, K. Alho, P. Iaurinen, P. Paavilainen, T. Radil, K. Rainikainen, M. Sams, and R. Näätänen, “Mismatch negativity (MMN) for sequences of auditory and visual stimuli: evidence for a mechanism specific to the auditory modality”, Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophys., 77, 1990, pp. 436-444. [32] W. Ritter, D Deacon, H. Gomes, D.C. Javitt, and H.G. Jr. Vaughan, “The mismatch negativity of event-related potentials as a probe of transient auditory memory: a review”, Ear Hear., 16, 1995, pp. 52-67. [33] J. Besle, A. Fort, and M. Giard, “Is the auditory sensory memory sensitive to visual information?”, Exp. Brain Res., 166, 2005, pp. 337-344. [34] R. Näätänen, “The role attention in auditory information processing as revealed by event-related potentials and other brain

IJCSI International Journal of Computer Science Issues, Vol. 7, Issue 3, No 10, May 2010 www.IJCSI.org measures of cognitive function”, Behv. Brain Sci., 13, 1990, pp. 201-288. [35] S. Berti, and E. Schroger, “Distraction effects in vision: behavioral and event-related potential indices”, NeuroReport, 15, 2004, pp. 665-669. [36] I. Czigler, L. Balazs, and I. Winkler, “Memory-based detection of task-irrelevant visual changes”, Psychophysiology, 39, 2002, pp. 869-873. [37] J.D. Heslenfeld, “Visual mismatch negativity”, in J. Polish (Ed.), Detection of change: event-related potential and fMRI findings, Kluwer Academic Publishers, Doedrecht, 2003, pp. 41-60. [38] R. Cammann, “Is there a mismatch negativity (MMN) in the visual modality?”, Behav. Brain Sci., 13, 1990, pp. 234-235. [39] D.J. Woods, K. Alho, and A. Algazi, “Intermodal selective attention. I. Effects on event-related potentials to lateralized auditory and visual stimuli”, Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophys., 82, 1992, pp. 341-355. [40] K. Alho, D.L. Woods, A. Algazi, and R. Näätänen, “Intermodal selective attention. II. Effects of attentional load on processing of auditory and visual stimuli in central space”, Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophys., 82, 1992, pp. 356-368. [41] P. Pazo-Alvarez, F. Cadaveira, and E. Amenedo, “MMN in the visual modality: a review”, Biol. Psychology, 63, 2003, pp. 199236. [42] K. Alho, D.L. Woods, and A. Algazi, “Proceeding of auditory stimuli during auditory and visual attention as revealed by eventrelated potentials”, Psychophysiology, 31, 1994, pp. 469-479. [43] H.G.O.M. Smid, A. Jakob, and H.J. Heinzo, “An event-related brain potential study of visual selective attention to conjunctions of color and shape”, Psychophysiology, 36, 1999, pp. 264-279. [44] K. Alho, P. Paavilainen, K. Reinikainen, M. Sams, and R. Näätänen, “Separability of different negative components of the event-related potential associated with auditory stimulus processing”, Psychophysiology, 23, 1986, pp. 613-623. [45] R. Oldfield, “The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory”, Neuropsychologia, 9, 1971, pp. 97-113. [46] I. Czigler, and L. Balazs, “Event-related potentials and audiovisual stimuli: multimodal interactions”, NeuroReport, 12, 2001, pp. 223-236. [47] P. Paavilainen, S. Valppu, and R. Näätänen, “The Additivity of the auditory feature analysis in the human brain as indexed by the mismatch negativity: 1+1≈2 but 1+1+1