IS GRADUATE SOCIAL WORK EDUCATION PROMOTING A CRITICAL APPROACH TO MENTAL HEALTH PRACTICE? Jeffrey R. Lacasse Florida State University
Tomi Gomory Florida State University
A sample of 71 psychopathology course syllabi from 58 different graduate schools of social work was analyzed to determine whether different viewpoints and the concomitant empirical evidence were presented regarding 4 significant mental health topics: concepts of mental disorder, reliability and validity of psychiatric diagnoses, biological etiology, and drug treatment. There is little evidence that graduate psychopathology courses cover viewpoints other than the most conventional and institutional—that of biomedical psychiatry. A small handful of secondary (textbooks) rather than primary (research articles) sources provide the majority of the mental health content in these courses. Implications are discussed. The article includes an overview of both the relevant conventional and critical literature.
People make too big a deal out of DSM and spend too much time studying it. DSM is a labeling system that is inherently superficial, and it is a convenient fiction to suppose that patients’ problems can be broken down into discrete categories. We don’t understand the etiology of mental illness, and lab findings are practically never found that are diagnostically useful. —Michael First, DSM–IV–TR co-chair and editor No longer do we seek to understand whole persons in their social contexts— rather we are there to realign our patients’ neurotransmitters. The problem is that it is very difficult to have a relationship with a neurotransmitter—whatever its configuration. —Loren Mosher, former chief of the Center for Schizophrenia Studies, National Institute of Mental Health
HISTORICALLY, the field of social work has
alternative viewpoints furthering its goal of a
been committed to work toward a “just soci-
just society. In addition, evidence-based prac-
ety” (Reamer, 1993). This means that social
tice suggests that professional decision mak-
work has often rejected mainstream institu-
ing should rest on the evaluation of well-tested
tional views and has offered or supported
empirical evidence, not prevailing dogma
Journal of Social Work Education Vol. 39, No. 3 (Fall 2003). © Copyright 2003 Council on Social Work Education, Inc. All rights reserved.
383
384
JOURNAL OF SOCIAL WORK EDUCATION
(Gambrill, 1999). This commitment to explor-
to the status quo by critically analyzing cur-
ing nonmainstream positions is exemplified
rent policies. For example, in their widely used
by the range of arguments found in popular
chemical dependency text, McNeece and
social policy texts authored by social work
DiNitto (1998) find the government’s broadly
academics. For example, almost every social
marketed “War on Drugs” deeply flawed—
policy textbook presents the entire range of
after carefully analyzing this policy and dis-
arguments for and against the paradigm of
cussing the pros and cons of drug legalization
welfare reform, epitomized by the Personal
(pp. 192–195).
Responsibility and Work Opportunity Reconcilia-
The present study seeks to assess whether
tion Act of 1996 (e.g., Gilbert & Terrell, 2002;
mental health content in social work education
Karger & Stoesz, 2002; DiNitto, 2000;
is subject to the same critical academic atten-
Sherraden, 1991). Some authors present de-
tion as the policy topics just mentioned. Social
tailed alternatives to the conventional wisdom
work has become “the dominant profession
(i.e., Fox Piven, Acker, Hallock & Morgen,
staffing mental health services in the United
2002). Typical examples appear in one popu-
States” (Miller, 2002, p. 66). Are the more than
lar textbook which questions the currently
35,000 MSW students receiving training in a
accepted wisdom of for-profit provision of
given year (West et al., 2000, p. 293) receiving
human services: “[d]espite the undesirable
the best scientific information, evaluation of
attributes of proprietary human service pro-
evidence, and critical analysis currently avail-
viders, they are likely to continue to play an
able on mental health?
active role in defining social welfare” (Karger & Stoesz, 2002, p. 197). This textbook also
Background Literature
questions the utility of the entire Temporary
Research on mental health topics relevant
Assistance to Needy Families (TANF) pro-
to social work education has largely focused
gram driving much of our current government
on the beliefs of students (Shera & Delva-
policy on poverty:
Tauiliili, 1996), the opinions of practitioners (Kutchins & Kirk, 1988), or on specific meth-
The TANF program is predicated on a
ods for teaching psychopathology (Drolen,
kind of welfare behaviorism: an attempt
1993). In our review of the literature, we found
to reprogram the behaviors of the poor.
only one prior article directly bearing on the
Current welfare reform efforts, however,
present study. Raffoul and Holmes (1986) used
are unlikely to deliver on the promises of
a national survey of accredited master’s pro-
this approach. (p. 291)
grams to examine the integration of Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders–III
Additionally, social work academics have of-
(American Psychiatric Association, 1980) con-
ten critiqued the use of interventions that have
tent within social work psychopathology
failed empirical tests, for instance, the use of
courses. Raffoul and Holmes noted an ongo-
psychological debriefing (Lewis, 2003). Social
ing controversy among academics regarding
work academics have promoted alternatives
advantages and disadvantages of using the
IS MENTAL HEALTH EDUCATION CRITICAL?
385
DSM. Their work pointed to the range of “pre-
Neuroscience has revolutionized our un-
ferred treatment models, philosophies of treat-
derstanding of human behavior. . . mental
ment, and beliefs about psychopathology” (p.
health and mental illness. . . during the
25) held by academics who teach social work
past two decades. The principle that for-
students about mental health. This study, com-
merly ran counter to prevailing wisdom
pleted at the dawn of the modern DSM age,
has now become axiomatic, that is, that
found significant academic support for the
mental illnesses are biological illnesses
inclusion of DSM-related content in social work
in the brain. (Johnson, 2002, p. 362)
curricula; of the 57 schools in the sample, 33% (n =19) had a course “devoted exclusively to
Mental illness involves behavior that is
DSM-III” (p. 27), and 83% (n =47) included
labeled as deviant by someone. If not
some DSM-III content in nondedicated
troubling to the person said to have it, it
courses. The article also noted some per-
is troubling to those who are affected by
ceived disadvantages of the DSM, such as
it. The integral relationship between men-
value conflicts over labeling and a lack of
tal illness and deviance explains the
utility for treatment planning (p. 29). We are
changing view of unusual, disliked, or
not aware of any other study assessing psy-
disruptive behavior from sinful, to crimi-
chopathology education in social work since
nal, to reflective of mental illness.
this 1986 Raffoul and Holmes study.
(Gambrill, 1994, p. ix)
Since then the debate about how best to conceptualize human “mental disorderliness”
In short, academic social work recognizes
has continued. The field of social work con-
a variety of explanatory models for disordered
tains some of the most vocal proponents (Wil-
behavior ranging from deviance to disease.
liams & Spitzer, 1995) of the DSM-organized
Proponents of these models all claim the mod-
medical model of psychopathology, as well as
els are “supported” by some “evidence-based”
some of its most prominent critics (Kutchins &
literature (i.e., Cohen, 1989, 1990; Gomory,
Kirk, 1995; Wakefield, 1992a). Beyond the is-
1999, 2002; Johnson 1999b; Kirk & Kutchins,
sue of the value of the DSM for teaching and
1992; Kutchins & Kirk, 1997; Rubin, Cardenas,
clinical practice, there exists an intellectual
Warren, Pike, & Wambach, 1998; Thyer &
tension in our field between different models
Wodarski, 1998; Wakefield, 1992b). The present
of deviant behavior (Miller, 2002), such as the
study sought to determine whether the diver-
biomedical (e.g., mental illness as brain dis-
sity of theory and research found in the mental
ease treated primarily by medication) and the
health literature reached social work students
psychosocial (e.g., problematic volitional/in-
in their psychopathology classes.
fluenced behavior amenable to change via talk and behavioral approaches) models. Two quotes from social work academics who have taught psychopathology courses illustrate differing positions in the debate:
Method Sampling Frame and Response Rate Psychopathology syllabi were solicited during the academic year 2001–2002 from all
386
JOURNAL OF SOCIAL WORK EDUCATION
graduate schools of social work listed in the
ings assigned as required readings (on con-
top-80 of the annual U.S. News and World Re-
tent analysis see, Royse, 1999, pp. 211–221).
port rankings (2000). The schools’ websites
For the most part we did not analyze recom-
were examined for course listings of psycho-
mended readings assuming that optional ma-
pathology classes and their instructors. In-
terial was not deemed essential.
structors or administrators or both were
We searched for content which presented
emailed an initial psychopathology syllabus
different positions and well-tested evidence
request letter. Respondents were offered the
(evidence-based) in each of the four content
option to reply via email or to send a paper
areas. For instance, in the content area of bio-
copy of the requested syllabus via postal mail.
logical etiology of mental disorder, if informa-
One follow-up email solicitation and a final
tion was offered about depression resulting
request for nonresponders through conven-
from chemical imbalances in the brain that are
tional post were made in spring 2002. Addi-
corrected by antidepressant medication, then
tional syllabi were also collected through
we sought to determine if alternative view-
informal networking methods.
points were presented. For example, professor
We collected 71 syllabi from 58 different
of social work Harriette Johnson states in her
institutions. At least one psychopathology syl-
text Psyche, Synapse, Substance (1999a) that “The
labus was received from 58 of the 79 valid
SSRIs [selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors]
institutions (73.4%), including syllabi from 23
relieve symptoms of depression. . . by rem-
of the top 25 schools ranked.
edying the inadequate supply of serotonin that is causing the symptoms” (p. 35). Whereas
Procedure
professor of psychology and neuroscience Elliot
Each syllabus together with assigned read-
Valenstein (1998) takes a different position:
ings were then analyzed for general critical
“Although the often-repeated statement that
mental health content (i.e., types of texts re-
antidepressants work by correcting the bio-
quired, format of syllabus, whether treatments
chemical deficiency that is the cause of depres-
were included or not) as well as for specific
sion may be an effective promotional tack, it
content in four important but contentious ar-
cannot be justified by the evidence” (p. 110).
eas where social work scholars have done sig-
Findings
nificant critical work: (1) concepts of mental disorder, (2) measurement of mental disorder
Sample
(or reliability and validity of psychiatric diag-
Instructor background. We received syllabi
nosis), (3) biological etiology of mental disor-
from instructors with an assortment of educa-
der, and (4) drug treatment of mental disorder.
tional backgrounds, ranging from master’s-
We examined keywords, sentences, para-
level adjunct professors to distinguished full
graphs, and items found in various sections of
professors. Of the respondents, 1.4% (n =1)
the syllabi, such as course descriptions, goals
had only a master’s degree listed, 15.5% (n =11)
and objectives, lecture topics, syllabus orga-
listed themselves as licensed MSW-level prac-
nization and structure, and textbooks/read-
titioners, 46.5% (n =33) listed a PhD, 15.5%
IS MENTAL HEALTH EDUCATION CRITICAL?
387
(n =11) listed both a PhD and a social work
judged the syllabus to be organized and struc-
license, and 21.1% (n =15) had no degree listed.
tured by the DSM. Using that standard, 88.7%
The majority of the instructors, 61.9% (n =44),
(n =63) of the syllabi were organized along
listed a PhD.
DSM disorder categories. The remaining 11.3%
Course format. The vast majority of the
(n =8) of the syllabi typically set aside a class or
courses were in the 3-credit-hour format. For
two to cover “DSM–IV: Uses and Critiques”
43 (74.1%) of the responding schools we were
(Case 3), and spent the bulk of the class ses-
able to ascertain whether or not they required
sions on various theories of psychopathology
a psychopathology course in their master’s
and practice methods. Another similarity found
curriculum; 58.1% (n =25) of these required a
among the syllabi was that 76.0% (n =54) cov-
course for their clinical students, 13.9% (n =6)
ered some treatment issues in addition to theo-
of these schools did not, and 27.9% (n =12)
ries, assessment, and classification.
offered them as “cluster” electives. Syllabus structure. The majority of the syl-
Assigned Readings Within Syllabi
labi (57.7%, n =41) were titled using the term
Required texts. Since 88.7% of the class
“psychopathology” or similar expressions such
syllabi were found to follow the DSM, it was
as “psychosocial disorders,” followed by “as-
unsurprising to find that the DSM–IV–TR
sessment” (12.6%, n =9), “human behavior”
(American Psychiatric Association, 2000) was
(7.0%, n =5), and various other terms used in
the most often assigned text, with 74.6% (n =53)
three or less syllabi (22.5%, n =16). Despite
of the syllabi requiring it, including 16.9%
these variations, the sample was composed of
(n =12) of the total sample requiring only the
largely homogenous syllabi stating essentially
DSM–IV–TR.
the same course goals. For instance, “This is a
The next two most popular texts, both
survey course which teaches the student to
authored by psychiatrists, were Kaplan and
identify and understand the etiologies and
Saddock’s (1998) Synopsis of Psychiatry (25.4%,
symptomatology of the major groups of emo-
n =18) and Maxmen and Ward’s (1995) Essen-
tional and mental disorders as described by
tial Psychopathology and Its Treatment (14.1%,
the DSM-IV. It highlights the social, cultural,
n =10). Kaplan and Saddock is the only text
and ethnic factors which impact upon the di-
assigned in 8.4% (n =6) of the syllabi and
agnosis and treatment of these conditions”
Maxmen and Ward is the only text assigned in
(quoted from Case 13).
5.6% (n =4) of the syllabi. Although several
We evaluated how many courses orga-
psychopathology texts authored by social
nized their classes along formal DSM disorder
workers are listed—Austrian (2000) by 7.0%
categories. If we found more than 50% of the
(n =5), Turner (1999) by 2.8% (n =2), and Zide
total available class meetings identified with
and Gray (2001) by 2.8% (n =2) of the syllabi,
corresponding DSM headings (such as “DSM-
none serve as the stand-alone text for any
IV Introduction,” “Psychotic Disorders: Schizo-
syllabus. There are 13 different texts authored
phrenia,” or “Mood Disorders: Mania, Bipolar
or edited by social workers in a total of 26.7%
Disorder, and Cyclothymia,” in Case 4), we
(n =19) syllabi; 11 assigned in one or two syl-
388
JOURNAL OF SOCIAL WORK EDUCATION
labi and one (Bentley & Walsh, 2001), assigned
trist Thomas Szasz’s conceptualization of trou-
in three. (A list of all required textbooks is
bling or troubled behavior as “problems in
available upon request from the authors.)
living” (Szasz, 1987, 2001; for a critique see
We found that 28.2% (n =20) of the syllabi
Schoenfeld, 1976). On the other end of the
listed a class reader. Only 47.9% (n =34) re-
continuum, representing a medical “disorder”
quire even one article of any sort, and 42.3%
perspective on problematic behaviors as “de-
(n =30) had a bibliography attached. Given
termined motion” or symptoms, lies social
the extremely dynamic nature of mental
work academic Jerome Wakefield’s view of
health research and education (and related
mental disorder as “harmful dysfunction,”
topics such as psychotropic drug effects), it
consisting of the failure of a mental mecha-
is a striking finding that over half of the
nism to function as “designed” plus a soci-
students in our sample leave their psychopa-
etal judgment that the failure led to harm
thology course without being required to read
(Wakefield, 1992b; for a critique see Boyle,
a single journal article.
2002, pp. 224–231). Although Szasz’s and
Social work mental health literature content.
Wakefield’s models hold opposing views
Nearly half the syllabi (45.0%, n =32) do not
about the conceptual validity of the domain
require any readings (including texts) by a
of psychopathology, they are both, in different
social work author, and 33.8% (n =24) neither
ways, very critical of the DSM nosology and
require nor recommend any readings by any
mental disorder definition.
social work authors. This may be due to the
Other models exist along this continuum.
interdisciplinary nature of psychopathology
Closer to Szasz’s approach but omitting de-
research, a reliance on psychiatric sources,
finitive judgments on etiology lies sociologist
limited acquaintance with the empirical litera-
Thomas Scheff’s (1999) labeling theory, a so-
ture, or all three.
ciological perspective which views mental illness as a residual category of deviance made
Issue 1: Conceptualizations of Mental Disorder
up of individuals’ reactions to norms and atti-
Various models for explaining “mental
chiatric diagnoses. Discontinuity theory
disorderliness” that may be important to so-
developed by the current president of the
cial work’s practice and policy approaches fall
American Psychological Association, Philip
on a continuum. At one end lies the “rhetori-
Zimbardo (1999), is another. This theory ar-
cal” model of deviant or disvalued behavior
gues that societally disapproved explanations
(Vatz & Weinberg, 1994). This model views the
propounded by ordinary people through uni-
behaviors (including language use) labeled by
versal behavioral search processes in attempts
the DSM “symptomatology,” as socially and
to understand and explain personally painful
politically disvalued, volitional, goal-oriented,
experiences (called discontinuities), which are
meaningful conduct open to potential change
rigidly defended by the person despite such
ideally through voluntary effort of client and
disapproval, over time lead to mental illness.
worker. The model is represented by psychia-
We were interested in seeing if these, or other
tudes held by others toward those with psy-
IS MENTAL HEALTH EDUCATION CRITICAL?
389
well-argued and more familiar alternatives
(n =45) of the syllabi and use the DSM defini-
along the continuum (i.e., psychodynamic,
tion of mental disorder (e.g. Maxmen & Ward,
interpersonal, cognitive–behavioral, family
1995, p. 5). The abnormal psychology texts
systems, etc.), were offered for review.
used in some courses are more comprehen-
How mental disorder is portrayed in the syllabi and assigned literature. Many of the syllabi
sive, but all of them together are only assigned in 8.4% (n =6) of course syllabi.
(42.3%, n =30) assert an intention to address a
Although the ideas of Thomas Szasz are
range of views about “what is mental disor-
detailed in an academic career spanning hun-
der” either by explicitly stating this or making
dreds of journal articles, over 25 books, and 50
reference to terms such as “deviance” or “psy-
years and have been cited widely in the health,
chosocial dysfunction.” We examined the
legal, ethics, and social science literature, Szasz
course description and objectives for the use of
is generally dismissed in psychiatric texts. In
the words “political” (which we saw as con-
the second most-used text after the DSM in our
gruent with Szasz’s views), or “sociological”
sample of syllabi, Maxmen and Ward (1995)
(which we saw as reflective of Scheff’s view-
make only this mistaken statement about
point), or some variation of the two terms in
Szasz’s perspective:
relation to causes and conceptualizations of “mental disorderliness;” 23.9% (n =17) included
Critics like Thomas Szasz argue that be-
the term “sociological” and 18.3% (n =13) used
cause the line between psychopathology
the term “political.” To review the actual course
and normality is hazy, psychopathology
content on alternate mental disorder perspec-
is a myth. That’s nonsense. Day and night
tives (as differentiated from statements about
exist, even though at dusk it is hard to
the intention to address the issue) we reviewed
know which it is. Psychopathology is no
all assigned readings. We found that 5.6%
less real for its relativity. (p. 5; for a more
(n =4) of syllabi required a reading by Szasz
accurate description of Szasz’s position,
and 2.8% (n =2) contained his work in either
see Vatz & Weinberg, 1983)
their recommended list or bibliography. Neither Scheff’s nor Wakefield’s work was re-
Some other perspectives or practice mod-
quired in any of the syllabi we examined;
els were contained within some of the courses
one syllabus (1.4%) required an article by
we examined. We assumed content merely if
Levy (1981), which incorporated labeling
the name of a perspective or model was listed
theory. Wakefield’s work on harmful dys-
anywhere in the syllabus. We found that 39.4%
function was found in two bibliography en-
(n =28) of the courses named at least one alter-
tries (2.8%). Two (2.8%) syllabi contained an
native perspective. Psychodynamic was by far
article by Widiger and Sankis (2000), which
the most prevalent (n =22, 31.0%), followed by
offered valuable in-depth content on the con-
ecosystems/family systems (n =9, 12.7%), cog-
cept of mental disorder.
nitive/cognitive–behavioral (n =7, 9.9%), femi-
The DSM and various psychiatric text-
nist (n =5, 7.0%), and behavioral (n =3, 4.2%).
books are the only assigned texts in 63.4%
Existential, social learning, and narrative
390
JOURNAL OF SOCIAL WORK EDUCATION
perspectives were listed in two or fewer syl-
completely invalid. Most people agree about
labi, accounting for no more than 2.8% of the
what a “sunrise” looks like. Such judgments
total each.
are reliable (i.e., consistent across observers), but such judgments are wrong, the observa-
Issue 2: Reliability and Validity of the DSM
tions are invalid and in error. The sun does not rise; the earth rotates on its axis and orbits
For science to grow it is essential that the
around the sun, creating the optical illusion of
problems we wish to tackle be real. That is, in
the sun’s rise. Even Nancy Andreasen, chair of
order to make progress toward problem reso-
the DSM-IV Schizophrenia Work Group and a
lution in the real world, we need to be sure that
leading researcher and proponent of biologi-
a concept or entity related to a problem exists
cal explanations for schizophrenia, agrees with
and that we can consistently agree on how to
Kirk and Kutchins’s criticisms. She states in
identify it. For example, if we wish to explain
her
an occurrence of violent and threatening be-
(Andreasen & Black, 2001) that
coauthored
psychiatric
textbook
havior for which the perpetrator offers an explanation “that the devil commanded me to do
The DSM criteria are simple provisional
it,” by calling the occurrence the result of the
agreements, by a group of experts, on
DSM-defined mental disorder schizophrenia,
what characteristic features must be
then we would want to make sure that this
present to make a diagnosis. Although
disorder actually exists (Boyle, 2002). This en-
the criteria are based on data whenever
tails the interrelated problems of the reliability
possible, the available data are often in-
and validity of putative scientific entities, in
adequate for building the criteria. Thus,
this case of a DSM mental disorder. Social
the selection of signs and symptoms is
work academics Stuart Kirk and Herb Kutchins
often relatively arbitrary. The diagnoses
strongly critique the reliability of DSM mental
themselves are certainly arbitrary. They
disorders. They have found the claims for di-
will remain arbitrary as long as we are
agnostic reliability of the DSM to be signifi-
ignorant about pathophysiology and eti-
cantly flawed in all three of the modern versions
ology. Biologically oriented psychiatrists
(DSM–III, –III-R, and –IV), each of which were
have objected to the lack of validity in the
specifically designed to address the problems
DSM as well. In this instance, they point
of reliability found in earlier versions (Kirk &
to the arbitrary nature of the definitions,
Hsieh, in press; Kirk & Kutchins, 1992, 1994;
which are not rooted in information about
Kutchins & Kirk, 1997).
biological causes. (pp. 35–36)
Kirk and Kutchins have focused their research on the issue of reliability not only be-
Another social work academic engaged criti-
cause that is what psychiatrists have addressed,
cally with issues of mental health and treat-
but also because “a classification system that is
ment, David Cohen, together with his coauthor
unreliable. . . cannot be valid” (Kirk & Kutchins,
argues in a recent article that the DSM is inter-
1992, p. 32). Of course, a reliable concept can be
nally inconsistent and cannot be scientifically
IS MENTAL HEALTH EDUCATION CRITICAL?
391
justified according to the standards articu-
to the fourth volume of the DSM–IV Sourcebook
lated by the medical paradigm adopted by
series (American Psychiatric Association,
contemporary psychiatry itself (Jacobs &
1998), which contains the data reanalyses and
Cohen, in press).
field trials relating to the diagnostic categories.
How reliability and validity are discussed in
The fourth volume published in 1998 is cur-
class syllabi and assigned literature. We searched
rently out of print and has been for several
each syllabus (including course assignments)
years, complicating scholarly analysis of the
for the words “reliability” and “validity.”
improved reliability of DSM mental disorders
Nearly 13% (12.7%, n =9) of the syllabi con-
claimed by its proponents.
tained the word “reliability” by stating, for
Nearly 30% (28.1%, n =20) of the syllabi
example, that one course objective was to “un-
assigned a psychiatric textbook containing no
derstand problems of reliability of mental
empirical content on the problems of DSM
health labeling systems” (Case 11). Almost 6%
reliability and validity. Also, these syllabi did
(5.6%, n =4) of the syllabi contained the word
not assign any articles with such content. Ten
“validity.” Overall, 87.3% (n =62) of the syllabi
syllabi (14.1%) required no relevant articles
we examined did not contain these words,
but required textbooks that had some content
which we saw as possible indicators of a criti-
on reliability and validity; these ranged from a
cal or scientific outlook on DSM. However,
short acknowledgment of the extant critiques
content on reliability/validity, even not ex-
(Zide & Gray, 2001) to briefly summarizing the
plicitly named, might still be found under
empirical literature (Brubeck, 1999). About 30%
other syllabus subheadings, such as “Contro-
(29.6%, n =21) of the syllabi required at least
versies around DSM”, or in the course read-
one reading which offered some critical dis-
ings. To that end, we performed a more detailed
cussion relevant to the practice of psychiatric
analysis on how required readings might have
diagnosis; the most frequently assigned article
addressed the issues of reliability/validity.
was the Journal of Social Work Education’s Point/
Reliability and validity content of assigned
Counterpoint debate, “Should DSM Be the
material. We reviewed assigned textbooks and
Basis for Teaching Social Work Practice in
procured all journal articles that were assigned
Mental Health?” between Kutchins and Kirk
reading when diagnosis was addressed in the
(1995) and Williams and Spitzer (1995), re-
syllabi. Of the 71 syllabi collected, we were
quired in nine syllabi (12.7%). Other articles
able to examine all the relevant readings for
assigned include a general critique of the DSM
95.8% (n =68).
(Wylie, 1995; 8.5%, n =6) as well as classic
Of the syllabi we examined for this analy-
critiques, such as Rosenhan’s (1973) “On Being
sis, 15.5% (n =11) used the DSM as the sole
Sane in Insane Places” (4.2%, n =3). While some
required text. The DSM itself does not contain
of these articles offer compelling information
any substantive information about reliability
regarding some problems with psychiatric di-
or validity; in fact, the words “reliability” and
agnosis, none offer empirical data-driven cri-
“validity” do not appear in the index. Readers
tiques of reliability and validity. Only 8.5%
who are interested in these issues are referred
(n =6) of class syllabi required even one publi-
392
JOURNAL OF SOCIAL WORK EDUCATION
cation empirically critiquing the reliability of
pear worse. . . . Despite the scientific
the DSM (i.e., Kirk & Kutchins, 1992, 1994;
claims of great success, reliability ap-
Kutchins & Kirk, 1997).
pears to have improved very little in
Some assigned readings overstated the
three decades. (p. 83)
reliability and validity of the DSM. For instance, Hales and Hales (1995) state, “Because
Kirk and Hsieh (in press) have more recently
of its widespread use, clinicians today agree
examined the percent agreement among expe-
on diagnoses approximately eighty percent of
rienced clinicians (social workers, clinical psy-
the time, far more frequently than in the past”
chologists, and psychiatrists) diagnosing
(p. 8). No citation for this claim is provided in
conduct disorder according to DSM–IV crite-
the text. Andreasen and Black (2001) also as-
ria. Their research found “substantial dispar-
sert without providing a citation that “the
ity in the DSM diagnoses used by experienced
kappa statistics for most diagnoses are ap-
clinicians. [S]electing either the same or a dif-
proximately 0.8 or greater, which is consid-
ferent diagnosis, the nearly 50/50 split dem-
ered very good” (p. 34). Another assigned text
onstrates no more than a chance agreement,
referenced Kirk and Kutchins for the claim
the mere flipping of a coin” (p. 16). The diffi-
that “On average, experienced clinicians only
culties of the reliability of DSM disorders ap-
agreed on their diagnoses using these manuals
pear to continue in the latest version of the
about 70 percent of the time” (Nolen-
“psychiatric Bible.”
Hoeksema, 1998, p. 52). Our review failed to find such a claim in any of Kirk and Kutchin’s
Issue 3: Biological Etiology
works and when we contacted the author of
Regarding the etiology and treatment of
the text, the author was unable to indicate
mental disorder, there are two distinct points
where in the referenced work she found the
of view. First, there is the hypothesis that
70% agreement figure (Nolen-Hoeksema, 2002,
certain collections of problematic behaviors
personal communication, December 11, 2002).
psychiatrically labeled “mental disorders” are
Kirk and Kutchin’s (1992, 1994) work sug-
caused by malfunctioning brain neurochemis-
gests that the average kappa means are closer
try, a position held by the National Institute of
to .60 across reliability studies of DSM–III and
Mental Health (2000):
–III–R diagnoses on patient samples and far worse, .37 on non-patient samples (1994, p. 81,
Since. . . 50 years ago, much has been
figure 2 and p. 82, figure 3). The authors
learned about mental disorders and their
conclude:
effects on the brain. Revolutionary scientific advances in neuroscience, molecular
The findings of this elaborate reliability
biology, genetics, and brain imaging have
study were disappointing even to the
provided some of the greatest insights
investigators. The kappa values are not
into the complex organ that is the seat of
that different than those from the pre-
thought, memory, and emotion. Thanks
DSM–III studies and in some cases ap-
to these new tools, the scientific evidence
IS MENTAL HEALTH EDUCATION CRITICAL?
393
that mental illnesses are brain disorders
This is the first empirical demonstration
now exists. (p. 1)
that chronic administration of a selective serotonin reuptake blockade can have
The advocacy organization for families of
significant personality and behavioral
clients who are diagnosed as mentally ill,
effects in normal humans in the absence
the National Alliance for the Mentally Ill
of baseline depression or other psycho-
(n.d.), agrees:
pathology. (p. 378)
During the last decade. . . scientific re-
A recent discussion with psychiatrist David
search has greatly expanded our under-
Burns, well known both for his work on cogni-
standing and firmly established that mental
tive–behavioral therapy and as a winner of the
illnesses like major depression are biologi-
prestigious A. E. Bennett Award (granted by
cally-based brain diseases. (p. 1)
the Society for Biological Psychiatry for his research on serotonin) yielded the following:
Some social work academics agree with this view reductively attributing the cause of
I spent the first several years of my career
many mental disorders (complex global be-
doing full-time research on brain seroto-
havioral phenomena) to misfiring neurotrans-
nin metabolism, but I never saw any con-
mitters, such as dopamine and serotonin. For
vincing evidence that any psychiatric
instance, social work professor Harriette
disorder, including depression, results from
Johnson’s (1999a) text, Psyche, Synapse, and
a deficiency of brain serotonin. In fact, we
Substance, is devoted to explaining the neuro-
cannot measure brain serotonin levels in
biology of addiction and mental disorder. Psy-
living human beings so there is no way to
chiatric texts such as Kaplan and Saddock
test this theory. Some neuroscientists would
(1998) contain detailed discussions about the
question whether the theory is even viable,
various neurotransmitter systems, the mental
since the brain does not function in this
disorders they are said to be correlated with,
way, as a hydraulic system. By way of an
and the medications that act upon these neu-
analogy, if you had a problem with your
rotransmitters in the brain.
computer, it would probably result from a
However, significant critiques of the dis-
software glitch, and not from a silicon defi-
ease model exist (in social work see Cohen,
ciency in one of the chips. Physicians who
1989, 1990; Gomory, 1998; Tyson, 1991). An
tell their patients that their depression and
article (Knutson et al., 1998) reporting on SSRI
anxiety result from a chemical imbalance in
effects on normal populations in a recent
the brain are confusing theories with facts.
double-blind randomized controlled trial con-
Of course, the drug companies encourage
tradicts the claimed disease-specific effects by
this type of message, because it stimulates
such drugs, undermining the global claim that
enormous sales for the popular and costly
SSRIs are ameliorating a putative biochemical
SSRI antidepressants. (Burns, personal
imbalance found only in the mentally ill:
communication, April 29, 2003)
394
JOURNAL OF SOCIAL WORK EDUCATION
The surgeon general’s recent mental health
We next examined the syllabi to see if
report (U.S. Department of Health and Human
published academic critiques of these biologi-
Services, 1999) also confirms the conjectural
cal theories were offered anywhere in the syl-
nature of claims about the biological basis of
labi
mental disorderliness:
recommended readings, and bibliographies.
including
required
readings,
We were looking for any classic critiques of [T]here is no definitive lesion, laboratory
biological psychiatry (Breggin, 1991; Fancher,
test, or abnormality in brain tissue that can
1995; Ross & Pam, 1995) as well as literature on
identify [mental] illness. . . . All too fre-
specific disorders/treatments (Valenstein,
quently, a biological change in the brain (a
1998; Glenmullen, 2000), critical responses to
lesion) is purported to be the “cause” of a
articles positing a disease model view (i.e.,
mental disorder. The fact is that any simple
Cohen, 1989), or empirical articles from the
association—or correlation—cannot and
field of biological psychiatry which found, for
does not. . . mean causation. (pp. 44–51)
instance, that brain changes in those diagnosed with schizophrenia may be attributed
How biological etiology is portrayed in syllabi
to psychotropic medication rather than to
and assigned literature. Slightly more than 25.3%
schizophrenia-related neuropathology
(n =18) of the syllabi included course goals or
(Corson, Nopoulos, Miller, Arndt &
descriptions representative of chemical imbal-
Andreasen, 1999). We located five syllabi
ance theory. For instance, the course descrip-
containing such readings (7.0% of the
tion for Case 1 reads, “individuals who are
sample); of those, three placed these cri-
licensed. . . should demonstrate that they rec-
tiques in recommended readings, and only
ognize the differences between biologically-
two listed such readings as required. Cri-
based brain diseases.” Other syllabi used terms
tiques of biological reductionism were found
like “neurochemistry” (Cases 18 and 47), “neu-
in very few of the course syllabi in our sample.
rochemical substrates” (Cases 36 and 55), and
Some required texts contained poten-
“neurobiological” (Case 58) to describe poten-
tially misleading content. For example,
tial causes of mental disorder. An additional
Johnson (1999b) contains photographs of
43.6% (n =31) of assigned textbooks contain
positron emission tomography (PET) scans
bioreductionistic content, such as “norepi-
from Zametkin et al. (1990) comparing an
nephrine and serotonin are tied to depres-
“ADHD” brain with a “normal” brain, which
sion. . . . Depression occurs when the
appear dramatically different in the illustra-
regulatory function of the neuroreceptors
tion. Johnson reports that a malfunction in
breaks down” (Austrian, 2000, p. 47). Two
cerebral glucose metabolism results in “im-
courses assigned an article by Taylor (1987)
pulsive behavior arising from deficits in inhi-
entitled “Biological Basis of Schizophrenia.”
bition” (Johnson, 1999b, p. 59, emphasis in
All told it appears that at least 71.8% (n =51)
original). Hales and Hales (1995, p. 338) re-
of the courses included some content on
port similarly. To students unaware of the
chemical imbalance theory.
critical literature against which to evaluate
IS MENTAL HEALTH EDUCATION CRITICAL?
395
such brain imaging photographs and author-
Zametkin and colleagues’ (1993) later
ity-based assertions, it certainly might appear
study found that “as assessed by PET scan-
that the controversy regarding the psychiatric
ning, global cerebral glucose metabolism in
diagnosis of ADHD has been resolved; that it
hyperactive teens was not significantly dif-
is a valid disease entity for which disease-
ferent from that of the control group” (p.
specific neuropathology has been located.
336). The National Institutes of Health (1998)
However, Alan Zametkin himself reported in 1991 (cited in Breggin, 1998):
Consensus Statement on ADHD states, “an independent diagnostic test for ADHD does not exist.”
One commonly asked question of our brain imaging studies is whether or not PET
Issue 4: Psychotropic Medications
scanning can be used to diagnose ADHD.
The debate about the utility, safety, and
Unfortunately, this is not currently pos-
efficacy of psychiatric medications is conten-
sible because there is considerable overlap
tious. From the mental-disorder-as-brain-dis-
in our study between normal and ADHD
ease perspective, medications are thought to
brain metabolism. (p. 169)
be invaluable in the treatment of mental disorders. For instance, in Hales and Hales (1995),
The brain-imaging studies relevant to
“New psychiatric medications are correcting
ADHD have been rife with confounds and
chemical imbalances in the brain with far fewer
have not resulted in any significant neurobio-
side effects than older drugs” (p. 8). Psychotro-
logical findings, despite widespread reports to
pic medications are a growth industry—even
the contrary (Breggin, 1998; Leo & Cohen,
for children. The use of antidepressant medi-
2003). Less than 1 year after Alan Zametkin’s
cations for that group has more than doubled
much heralded 1990 study was published, so-
in 10 years (Goode, 2002). Antidepressants
cial work professor Katherine Tyson (1991)
are generally viewed as safe and effective
published this in response:
treatments for depression and are routinely prescribed by general practitioners and psy-
The abnormal distribution pattern of ce-
chiatrists: “the effectiveness of antidepres-
rebral glucose metabolism in the experi-
sants is unquestioned” (Andreasen & Black,
mental sample. . . is not associated with
2001, p. 720). They are said to be 60–70%
variations in the behavioral variable of
effective in the treatment of depression
attention. . . the authors found that even
(Bentley & Walsh, 2001). At least five social
when the control group and experimen-
workers have published texts disseminating
tal group differed in cerebral glucose
information on psychiatric medications, gen-
metabolism in some areas of the brain,
erally echoing these claims (Austrian, 2000;
these groups had no functional differ-
Bentley & Walsh, 2001; Dziegielewski, 2001;
ences on the measure of attention. (p. 140,
Turner, 1999).
emphasis in the original)
The critical literature disagrees. For example, some of the empirical literature on
396
JOURNAL OF SOCIAL WORK EDUCATION
SSRIs, one of the latest group of antidepres-
SSRIs are only marginally more effective than
sants which are asserted to have fewer adverse
inactive placebos and no more effective than
effects than earlier antidepressants, has noted
active placebos. A very recent study (Kirsch,
significant problems associated with their use
Moore, Scoboria, & Nicholls, 2002) reviewed
and claimed antidepressant-specific effects.
all the drug efficacy data contained in double-
Psychiatrist Colin Ross has argued that psy-
blind randomized trials submitted by phar-
chiatric medications are not based on amelio-
maceutical companies to the U.S. Food and
rating neurochemical lesions found through
Drug Administration for the six most widely
neuroscience research, but are created through
prescribed antidepressants approved between
a speculative process called “chemical rou-
1987 and 1999 (Prozac, Paxil, Zoloft, Effexor,
lette” and then marketed on the basis of what-
Serzone, and Celexa). They found the mean
ever the current “hot” neurotransmitter is (Ross
difference between the active drug and pla-
& Pam, 1995, pp. 110–111, 117–118). Healy
cebo to be a 2-point difference favoring the
(2000, 2001) found that SSRIs can induce
drug on the Hamilton Depression Scale. This
suicidality and concludes elsewhere that the
2-point differential on the scale translates to no
competing agendas and interests of the vari-
clinical difference. Finally, Joseph Glenmullen
ous participants in the political economy of
(2000) has publicized research indicating that
psychiatric research can result in bad science
SSRIs can cause harmful neurological effects
(Healy, 2002). Social work professor David
such as tardive dyskinesia and drug-induced
Cohen is coauthor of a book (Breggin & Cohen,
Parkinsonism, and cites studies indicating that
1999) which guides clients through the diffi-
SSRI-induced sexual dysfunction occurs in 58–
cult withdrawal process when discontinuing
86% of patients studied (pp. 29–63, pp. 106–134).
psychiatric medications (including SSRIs). This
How the issue of psychotropic medication is
problem has received public attention recently
portrayed in syllabi and assigned literature. Slightly
when the FDA temporarily prohibited the use
less than 50% (47.9%, n =34) of the syllabi con-
of ads for Paxil which claimed that the antide-
tained statements directly indicating that learn-
pressant was not habit-forming (Peterson, 2002;
ing about medications was part of the course
White, 2002). Cohen (1988, 2002) has also been
content. Medication content was identified by
active in calling for the field of social work to
phrases such as “understanding the role of
consider the use of psychiatric drugs within
psychotropic medication” (Case 7), “uses of
the broader context of the political economy of
most commonly used psychotropic medica-
psychiatry, especially the pharmaceutical
tions” (Case 13), or “benefits and problems
industry’s role and impact. He also has called
associated with pharmacological interven-
for a closer scrutiny of the clinical trials used to
tions” (Case 24). An additional 29.6% (n =21)
assert the efficacy of certain psychotropic drugs
syllabi did not mention medications within
as “proven” or “evidence-based.”
the text of the syllabus but required a textbook and
with psychotropic medication content (such as
O’Donahue (2000) reviewed the evidence for
Austrian, 2000; Bentley & Walsh, 2001; Kaplan
antidepressant efficacy and concluded that
& Saddock, 1998; Maxmen & Ward, 1995). We
Antonuccio,
Burns,
Danton,
IS MENTAL HEALTH EDUCATION CRITICAL?
397
assessed the level of critical content on psycho-
the 153 Council on Social Work Education
tropic medications by examining each sylla-
accredited master’s programs were surveyed.
bus, the required and recommended readings,
We are unaware of any systematic factors that
as well as all the bibliographies, some in excess
would lead non–top-80 schools to present more
of 10 pages. We looked for readings by such
critical mental health content than those in our
authors as David Cohen (1988, 1989, 1997,
sample, but we cannot exclude this possibility.
2002), David Healy (1997, 2000, 2001, 2002),
In our preliminary research we received a
Joseph Glenmullen (2000), David Jacobs (1995,
psychopathology syllabus from a university
1999), Peter Breggin (1991, 1997, 1998), Elliot
outside of our sampling frame that contained
Valenstein (1998), Seymour Fisher and Roger
substantial critical content; there may be oth-
Greenberg (1989, 1997), David Antonuccio
ers. Further, content analysis in part requires
(1995), Irving Kirsch and Guy Sapirstein (1998),
interpretation, and although we tried to mini-
Sidney Gelman (1999), Lucy Johnstone (2000),
mize this by self-critical methodological rigor
Richard DeGrandpre (1999), and also looked
(Gomory, 2001, pp. 28–34), subjective bias may
for coauthored projects by the above listed
have distorted the study’s findings.
authors (i.e., Cohen & Jacobs, 2000; Antonuccio
One additional limitation was that, for
et al., 2000). After analyzing all the syllabi,
some syllabi, it was difficult to determine which
only 4.2% (n =3) of the syllabi contained a
readings were required, suggested, or recom-
critical reading. Two of these syllabi listed
mended. Some listed many readings and noted
Valenstein (1998) only, a third listed Gelman
that the instructor would require a portion of
(1999); and required the article by Corson et al.
them; others gave students the option to pick
(1999) briefly discussed earlier in the biologi-
from a list. When it was unclear, we decided to
cal etiology section. Disturbingly, this latter
code the readings as “required.” Our results
study was the single required critical article on
therefore tend to overestimate the degree to
negative effects of medication in the entire
which alternative or critical viewpoints are
sample and was required in just one syllabus.
presented in these course syllabi. Overall, the courses we examined do an
Discussion Limitations
excellent job of presenting the psychiatric information regarding the diagnosing and treat-
The representativeness of this study is
ment methods of mental disorders as
limited by its cross-sectional nature and also
represented by the American Psychiatric
by the variable character of academic courses
Association’s DSM medical model. However,
in general (e.g., if another instructor takes over
put in the most positive light, very few of the
the course, the content may change). While we
alternate explanatory theories and latest scien-
believe that most social work academics would
tific research critical of the mainstream psychi-
agree that the schools in the study are repre-
atric approaches on questions of reliability
sentative of quality schools of social work and
and validity of mental disorders, their puta-
adequately serve the intended purpose of iden-
tive biological etiology, or claims of effective
tifying a reasonable study sample, only 79 of
treatment via psychiatric drugs appear to be
398
JOURNAL OF SOCIAL WORK EDUCATION
well-represented in these classes (see Table 1).
teaching psychopathology courses not rou-
It is our anecdotal experience that many stu-
tinely offer the full range of available critical
dents enter our (the authors’) psychopathol-
material, much as some of our social work col-
ogy classes with some awareness of the
leagues appear to do in the field of social welfare,
biological theories about mental disorders—
as we argued at the outset? We conjecture below
many believe they are factual. Some mention
some possible answers for future discussion.
that they have been exposed to them through articles in the general print media or through
Subservience to Psychiatry
drug company advertisements. Cohen (2002)
Social work’s lack of critical content in
notes, “the idea that the distress and disorders
mental health has much to do with the
we refer to as mental illness are genuine physi-
profession’s inability to set itself apart ideo-
cal diseases completely pervades our culture”
logically from psychiatry. From its origin, so-
(p. 230). Given that many social work students
cial work relied on identifying pathology and
are “prepped” by conventional wisdom about
diagnosing, very much like psychiatry (i.e.,
these issues before they attend classes, and the
Kirk, Siporin, & Kutchins, 1989; Lubove, 1983;
role of higher education is, in part, to replace
Margolin, 1997). The profession of social work
simple “commonsense” belief by empirical
developed partly from the medical model’s
knowledge, why do social work academics
startling 19th-century reconstruction of what
TABLE 1. Percent of Syllabi Reviewed That Met Specific Criteria (N =71) Criteria Were organized by DSM categories Covered treatment issues Did not require students to read a single journal article Listed the DSM–IV–TR as the only required reading Used some variation of the term ”sociological” when describing mental disorders Used some variation of the term “political” when describing mental disorders Listed at least one alternative conceptualization of mental disorder and treatment Required a reading by Thomas Szasz (prominent critic of institutional psychiatry) Required a reading by Jerome Wakefield (mental disorder as “Harmful Dysfunction”) Used the term “reliability” an indicator of critical outlook on DSM Used the term “validity” an indicator of critical outlook on DSM Required a reading which critiqued psychiatric diagnosis Required a reading which empirically critiqued reliability and validity of DSM Addressed biological etiology either directly or in assigned readings Required a reading which critiqued biological etiology Recommended or listed in bibliography a reading which critiqued biological etiology Addressed psychiatric medications either directly or in assigned readings Required a reading which critiqued the use of psychiatric medications Recommended or listed in the bibliography a reading which critiqued the use of psychiatric medications
%
n
88.7 76.0 52.1 15.5 23.9 18.3 39.4 5.6 0.0 12.7 5.6 29.6 8.5 71.8 1.4 5.6 77.5 1.4
63 54 37 11 17 13 28 4 0 9 4 21 6 51 1 4 55 1
4.2
3
IS MENTAL HEALTH EDUCATION CRITICAL?
399
Andrew Abbot (1988, pp. 280–314) calls “the
To argue her cause, Dix relied on false
Personal Problems Jurisdiction.” Social work
statistics and false claims about the curative
and various other helping professions claimed
powers of treatment. As the historian of men-
expertise and competed for hegemony over
tal health, Gerald Grob (1994) notes, “She was
this ill-defined—but immensely vast—set of
not above employing exaggerated rhetoric or
problems. Psychiatry eventually prevailed in
embellishing facts. . . . She was often mistaken,
this contest by asserting itself as both repu-
and this has thrown doubt over all her state-
table medicine and good science. Social work
ments” (p. 47). She apparently made little ef-
wound up as psychiatry’s “handmaiden” due
fort to “accurately” report or question the
to its highly diffuse and nontechnological na-
validity of the data she used because they
ture lacking domain-specific theories, its rela-
supported her a priori assumptions about in-
tive disinterest in scientifically testing its
sanity being a medical disease. Historian An-
claimed expertise, and the fact that the vast
drew Scull (1981) reports that Dix,
majority of its practitioners were women com-
“Repeatedly. . . informed state legislatures that
peting for professional turf in an era of total
‘all experience shows that insanity reasonably
male domination (Abbot, 1988). In order to
treated is as certainly curable as a cold or a
survive as a “player” in the personal problems
fever’. She drew upon the elaborate statistics
jurisdiction in the new scientific age, social work
provided by. . . the asylum superintendents”
had to look “scientific” and in this domain the
(p. 156). David Rothman (1990), the medical
only “science” in town was psychiatric.
historian further tells us that,
Dorothea Dix, one of social work’s avatars (Trattner, 1979, p. 55, calls her a “great fig-
The. . . [superintendents] aggressively as-
ure”), illustrates both the strong historical ties
serted that properly organized institu-
between psychiatry and social work and the
tions could cure almost every incidence
shortcomings of this relationship. Active in
of the disease. . . . These statistics were
the mid-1800s, she wanted to help a group of
inaccurate and unreliable. Not only was
deeply troubled individuals (“the insane”)
there no attempt to devise criteria for
treated miserably by the prevailing penal au-
measuring recovery other than release
thorities. She argued that insanity was a medi-
from an institution but in some instances
cal disease and its sufferers needed treatment.
a single patient, several times admitted,
Her solution was to “convince the legislators
discharged and readmitted entered the
to fund public insane asylums. She claimed. . .
list as five times cured. . . . Dorothea
that insanity was curable and. . . only the lack
Dix[’s]. . . formula was simple and she
of funding. . . was holding up the elimination
repeated it everywhere: first assert the
of this grave. . . problem” (Leiby, 1978, p. 67).
curability of insanity, link it directly to
However, her method of gathering popular
proper institutional care and then quote
support for mental health services probably
prevailing medical opinion on rates of
set a precedent that today still haunts the
recoveries. (pp. 131–132)
profession of social work.
400
JOURNAL OF SOCIAL WORK EDUCATION
The syllabi we reviewed suggest that, like
course syllabi require even one article of any
Dix, social workers are very interested in help-
sort illuminates a serious problem. We found
ing those labeled as mentally ill and still prefer
in our analyses of some of the secondary
to continue our historical attachment to the
sources, that these are highly fallible interpret-
male-dominated profession of psychiatry and
ers of the primary research material. Two text-
its medical model. We do not appear to have
books written by psychiatrists, without offering
the professional self-confidence to forge our
any references, assert that reliability of psychi-
own separate perspective, even based on the
atric diagnoses are improving and now are at
existent well-tested science that often contra-
a very respectable average kappa value of .8
dicts much that is asserted to be factual by
(Andreasen & Black, 2001; Hales & Hales,
institutional psychiatry.
1995). Another textbook (Nolen-Hoeksema, 1998), citing a primary source, indicates a kappa
DSM’s Utility as a Reimbursement Tool
value of .7, rather than the average kappa
For professional economic survival it is
value of .6 in one sample and of .37 in the other,
imperative that a clear payment mechanism
found in the empirical work of the cited re-
be in place for service reimbursement. The
searchers (see present article p. 392). If syllabi
DSM classification system of coded mental
had assigned the original empirical analysis, a
disorders provides the reimbursement mecha-
comparison would have quickly identified the
nism for mental health services along the lines
error. But since over 91% of the syllabi failed to
developed for medical reimbursement for
assign any empirical articles on reliability by
physical illnesses from private insurance, Med-
Kirk and Kutchins, the leading researchers
icaid, Supplemental Security Income, and other
evaluating DSM reliability, the false “fact” of
government programs. A systematic random-
increased DSM reliability was maintained.
ized national survey found that clinical social
Gomory (2001) has discussed the serious limi-
workers overwhelmingly used it for business
tations of uncritical secondary source utiliza-
purposes and not for its clinical validity
tion by social work academics, and Joseph
(Kutchins & Kirk, 1988). Professional neces-
(2000) has analyzed the content bias and inaccu-
sity strongly influences the focus on the DSM
racy found in textbooks reporting on psychiatric
in mental health classes.
research. We recommend the routine inclusion of primary empirical sources, both those “sup-
Reliance on Secondary Sources for Information The reliance of psychopathology courses on secondary (text books) rather than primary
porting” as well as those critiquing essential content areas, along with survey texts, in order to promote evidence-based practice and critical debate about complex behavioral issues.
sources (theoretical and empirical research articles) to inform mental health course content we believe reduces critical thinking op-
Mental Disorder Promoted as Brain Disorder
portunities while promoting error. Our
The National Institute of Mental Health’s
findings that less than half (n =34, 47.9%) of the
(1999) decision to view “mental disorder” as
IS MENTAL HEALTH EDUCATION CRITICAL?
401
brain disorder promotes a national biological
to the intellectual framework of the primary
research agenda:
funding source. They demonstrate an understandable professional confirmatory bias
The mission of the National Institute of
(Nickerson, 1998) toward providing students
Mental Health (NIMH) is to diminish
information necessary for professional le-
the burden of mental illness. . . .
gitimation and a similarly understandable
Through research in basic neuro-
reluctance to acknowledge conflicting infor-
science, behavioral science, and ge-
mation undermining that effort.
netics we can gain an understanding
Conclusion
of the fundamental mechanisms of what goes wrong in the brain in mental illness. (website welcome page)
Finally, in thinking about the apparent one-sided approach to mental health education in social work master’s-level psychopa-
This institutional emphasis on biological expla-
thology classes found in this study, social
nations for mental disorders by the federal gov-
work educators might want to take cogni-
ernment entity chiefly responsible for funding
zance of some of the findings in the latest
research on mental health in the United States
federal report issued by the Center for Men-
strongly influences the nature of the research
tal Health Services of the Substance Abuse
pursued by the academic institutions attempt-
and Mental Health Services Administration
ing to access the nearly 1 billion NIMH dollars
on the state of mental health knowledge at
available. The assumption of biological causality
the millennium. It offers a refreshing, skep-
nicely meshes with the DSM’s medical view
tical take which social work educators might
(American Psychiatric Association, 2000, pp. xxx–
integrate into their graduate mental health
xxxi). The critical empirical literature we have
course curricula. In a chapter authored by
identified questions the apparently unquestioned
David Mechanic (2000), the director of the
assumption that, “mental illnesses are real dis-
NIMH Center for Research on the Organiza-
eases of an organ—in this case, the brain”
tion and Financing of Care for the Severely
(Hyman, 1998).
Mentally Ill, we find the following on:
But, since the key federal funding source (NIMH) limits research content to that which
Brain research
adheres to the DSM medical disorder paradigm, little incentive exists for academics
New imaging technologies have made it
attempting to survive in a publish/research
possible to directly track changes in the
or perish environment to subscribe to alter-
brain. . . to potentially use. . . for specific
native intellectual positions while attempt-
targeting of drugs. . . [T]he payoffs from
ing to fashion an academic career (Duesberg,
this sophisticated scientific infrastructure
1996, pp. 65–67). Schools of social work in
development are yet to be realized. (p. 56,
their psychopathology classes may simply
emphasis added)
be reflecting the current academic deference
402
JOURNAL OF SOCIAL WORK EDUCATION
Drug therapy
and treatments, many of which are the considered strengths of social work, should we not
[W]e have yet to have fundamental advances
then offer these alternatives for the critical
in drug therapy. . . Newer drugs such as
consideration of our students?
selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) and atypical antipsychotics appear generally to be no more efficacious than earlier medications. (p. 56, emphasis added)
References Abbot, A. (1988). The system of professions. Chicago: University of Chicago. American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders
Mental disorder conceptualization
(3rd ed.). Washington, DC: Author. American Psychiatric Association. (1998). DSM–
[O]ur understanding of the major mental
IV sourcebook. Washington, DC: Author.
disorders is still limited. . . History teaches
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diag-
us that it is easy to make claims of being
nostic and statistical manual of mental disor-
on the threshold. A certain modesty is
ders–IV, text revision (4th ed.). Washington,
needed, as well as a willingness to be
DC: Author.
open to new conceptualizations, theo-
America’s Best Graduate Schools 2004. U.S. News
ries, methods, and approaches. The DSM
and World Report. Retreived July, 2003 from
is an important example. Developed as a
http://www.usnews.com/usnews/edu/
descriptive convenience. . . DSM has been
grad/rankings/hea/brief/socwrk_brief.php.
reified in ways that are not constructive.
Andreasen, N. C., & Black, D. W. (2001). Introduc-
DSM is no more than a convenient instru-
tory textbook of psychiatry (3rd ed.). Washing-
ment and should not be used as a stan-
ton, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing.
dard to limit research on alternate
Antonuccio, D. (1995). Psychotherapy for de-
approaches. . . . Given the many uncer-
pression—no stronger medicine. American
tainties that. . . characterize treatment of
Psychologist, 50, 450–452.
mental disorders there is a concern with
Antonuccio, D., Burns, D., Danton, W., &
the present focus on biological aspects
O’Donahue, W. (2000). Rumble in Reno: The
and the preference of managed care for
psychosocial perspective on depression. Psy-
medication treatments over psycho-
chiatric Times, 8(8). Retrieved January 12,
therapy, counseling or other modalities.
2003, from http://www.psychiatrictimes
(pp. 56–57)
.com/p000824.html Austrian, S. G. (2000). Mental disorder, medica-
Given the tentativeness of what is “known”
tions, and clinical social work (2nd ed.). New
about mental disorderliness based on the cur-
York: Columbia University Press.
rent research, and the emphasis placed, even
Bentley, K. J., & Walsh, J. F. (2001). The social
by mainstream psychiatric experts, on not dis-
worker and psychotropic medication: Toward
regarding alternate perspectives, explanations,
effective collaboration with mental health cli-
IS MENTAL HEALTH EDUCATION CRITICAL? ents, families, and providers (2nd ed.). Belmont,
403
Cohen, D. (2002). Research on the drug treatment
CA: Wadsworth/Thomson Learning.
of schizophrenia: A critical appraisal and
Boyle, M. (2002). Schizophrenia: A scientific delu-
implications for social work education. Jour-
sion? (2nd ed.). New York: Taylor & Francis.
nal of Social Work Education, 38, 217–239.
Breggin, P. R. (1991). Toxic psychiatry: Why
Cohen, D., & Jacobs, D. (2000). A model consent
therapy, empathy and love must replace the
form for psychiatric drug treatment. Journal
drugs, electroshock and biochemical theories of
of Humanistic Psychology, 20(1), 59–64.
the “new psychiatry.” New York: St.
Corson, P. W., Nopoulos, P., Miller, D. D., Arndt,
Martin’s.
S., & Andreason, N. C. (1999). Changes in
Breggin, P. R. (1997). Brain-disabling treatments in
basal ganglia volume over 2 years in pa-
psychiatry: Drugs, electroshock, and the role of
tients with schizophrenia: Typical versus
the FDA. New York: Springer.
atypical neuroleptics. American Journal of
Breggin, P. R. (1998). Talking back to Ritalin:
Psychiatry, 156, 1200–1204.
What doctors aren’t telling you about stimu-
DeGrandpre, R. J. (1999). Ritalin nation: Rapid-fire
lants for children. Monroe, ME: Common
culture and the transformation of human con-
Courage Press.
sciousness. New York: W.W. Norton
Breggin, P. R., & Cohen, D. (1999). Your drug may
DiNitto, D. M. (2000). Social welfare: Politics and
be your problem: How and why to stop taking
public policy (5th ed.). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
psychiatric medications. Reading, MA: Per-
Drolen, C. S. (1993). The effect of educational set-
seus Books.
ting on student opinions of mental illness.
Brubeck, M. (1999). Social work and the DSM. In
Community Mental Health Journal, 29, 223–234.
F. Turner (Ed.), Adult psychopathology: A so-
Duesberg, P. (1996). Inventing the AIDS virus.
cial work perspective (pp. 121–135). New York: Free Press.
Washington, DC: Regnery. Dziegielewski, S. F. (2001). Social work practice
Cohen, D. (1988). Social work and psychotro-
and psychopharmacology. New York: Springer.
pic drug treatments. Social Service Review,
Fancher, R. T. (1995). Cultures of healing: Correct-
62, 576–599. Cohen, D. (1989). Biological basis of schizophrenia—the evidence reconsidered. Social Work, 34, 255–257. Cohen, D. (Ed.). (1990). Challenging the therapeutic state: Critical perspectives on psychiatry and the mental health system [Special issue]. Journal of Mind and Behavior, 11, 247–574. Cohen, D. (1997). A critique of the use of neuro-
ing the image of American mental health care. New York: Freeman. Fisher, S., & Greenberg, R. P. (1989). The limits of biological treatments for psychological distress: Comparisons with psychotherapy and placebo. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Fisher, S., & Greenberg, R. P. (1997). From placebo to panacea: Putting psychiatric drugs to the test. New York: Wiley.
leptic drugs in psychiatry. In S. Fisher & R.P.
Fox Piven, F., Acker, J., Hallock, M., & Morgen,
Greenberg (Eds.), From placebo to panacea:
S. (2002). Work, welfare and politics: Confront-
Putting psychotropic drugs to the test (pp. 173–
ing poverty in the wake of welfare reform. Eu-
229). New York: Wiley.
gene, OR: University of Oregon.
404
JOURNAL OF SOCIAL WORK EDUCATION
Gambrill, E. (1994). Preface. In S. A. Kirk & S. D.
Healy, D. (1997). The antidepressant era. Cam-
Einbinder (Eds.), Controversial issues in men-
bridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
tal health (pp. ix–x). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
Healy, D. (2000). Emergence of antidepressant
Gambrill, E. (1999). Evidence-based practice: An
induced suicidality. Primary Care Psychiatry,
alternative to authority-based practice. Family in Society, 80, 341–350 Gelman, S. (1999). Medicating schizophrenia: A
6(1), 23–28. Healy, D. (2001). The SSRI suicides. In C. Newnes, G. Holmes, & C. Dunn (Eds.),
history. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers Uni-
This
versity Press.
Herefordshire, UK: PCCS Books.
Gilbert, N., & Terrell. P. (2002). Dimensions of social welfare policy (5th ed.). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
is
madness
too
(pp.
59–69).
Healy, D. (2002). The creation of psychopharmacology. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University.
Glenmullen, J. (2000). Prozac backlash: Overcom-
Hyman, S. E. (1998). Schizophrenia: Understanding
ing the dangers of Prozac, Zolof, Paxil and other
it, treating it, living with it. Paper presented at
antidepressants with safe, effective alternatives.
the National Institute of Mental Health and
New York: Simon and Schuster.
the Library of Congress Project on the De-
Gomory, T. (1998). Coercion justified? Evaluating
cade of the Brain. Abstract retrieved January
the training in community living model: A
16, 2002, from http:/www.nimh.nih.gov/
conceptual and empirical critique. Dissertation
events/locschiz.cfm
Abstracts International, 59. (UMI No. 9902082).
Jacobs, D. H. (1995). Psychiatric drugging: Forty
Gomory, T. (1999). Programs of assertive commu-
years of pseudo-science, self interest, and
nity treatment (PACT): A critical review. Ethi-
indifference to harm. Journal of Mind and
cal Human Sciences and Services, 1, 147–163.
Behavior, 16, 421–470.
Gomory, T. (2001). A fallibilistic response to
Jacobs, D. H. (1999). A close and critical examina-
Thyer’s theory of theory-free empirical re-
tion of how psychopharmacotherapy re-
search in social work practice. Journal of
search is conducted. Journal of Mind and
Social Work Education, 37, 26–50.
Behavior, 20, 311–349.
Gomory, T. (2002). The origins of coercion in
Jacobs, D. H., & Cohen, D. (in press). Hidden in
assertive community treatment: A review of
plain sight: DSM IV’s rejection of the cat-
early publications from the special treat-
egorical approach to diagnosis. Review of Exis-
ment unit of Mendota state hospital. Ethical Human Sciences and Services, 4(1), 3–16.
tential Psychology and Psychiatry. Johnson, H. C. (1999a). Psyche, synapse, and
Goode, E. (2002, January 14). Study finds jump in
substance: The role of neurobiology in emo-
children taking psychiatric drugs. New York
tions, behavior, thinking, and addiction for
Times, p. A18. Grob, G. N. (1994). The mad among us: A history of the care of America’s mentally ill. New York: Free Press.
non-scientists. Greenfield, MA: Deerfield Valley. Johnson, H. C. (1999b). The biological basis of
Hales, D. R., & Hales, R. E. (1995). Caring for the
psychopathology. In F. Turner (Ed.), Adult
mind: The comprehensive guide to mental
psychopathology: A social work perspective (pp.
health. New York: Bantam.
54–99). New York: Free Press.
IS MENTAL HEALTH EDUCATION CRITICAL?
405
Johnson, H. C. (2002). Emerging knowledge and
Kirsch, I., Moore, T.J., Scoboria, A., & Nicholls, S.
future trends in mental health: Implications
S. (2002). The emperor’s new drugs: An
for social work. In K. J. Bentley (Ed.), Social
analysis of antidepressant medication data
work practice in mental health: Contemporary
submitted to the U.S. Food and Drug Ad-
roles, tasks and techniques (pp. 361–391). Pa-
ministration. Prevention and Treatment, 5. Re-
cific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole.
trieved January 12, 2003, from http://
Johnstone, L. (2000). Users and abusers of psychiatry: A critical look at psychiatric practice (2nd ed.). Philadelphia, PA: Routledge.
journals.apa.org/prevention/volume5/ pre0050023a.html Knutson, B., Wolkowitz, O. M., Cole, S. W., Chan,
Joseph, J. (2000). Inaccuracy and bias in text-
T. Moore, E. A., et al. (1998). Selective alter-
books reporting psychiatric research: The
ation of personality and social behavior by
case of the schizophrenia adoption studies.
serotonergic intervention. American Journal
Politics and the Life Sciences, 19, 89–99.
of Psychiatry, 155, 373–379.
Kaplan, H. I., & Saddock, B. J. (1998). Synopsis of
Kutchins, H., & Kirk, S. (1988). The business of
psychiatry (8th ed.). Baltimore, MD: Will-
diagnosis: DSM–III and clinical social work.
iams and Wilkins.
Social Work, 33, 215–220
Karger, H. J., & Stoesz, D. (2002). American social
Kutchins, H., & Kirk, S. A. (1995). Should DSM be
welfare policy: A pluralist approach (4th ed.).
the basis for teaching social work practice in
Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
mental health? No! Journal of Social Work
Kirk, S. A., & Hsieh, D. K. (in press). Diagnostic
Education, 31, 159–165.
consistency in assessing conduct disorder:
Kutchins, H., & Kirk, S. A. (1997). Making us
An experiment of the effect of social context.
crazy: DSM: The psychiatric bible and the cre-
American Journal of Orthopsychiatry.
ation of mental disorders. New York: Free Press.
Kirk, S. A., & Kutchins, H. (1992). The selling of
Leiby, J. (1978). A history of social welfare and social
DSM: The rhetoric of science in psychiatry.
work in the United States. New York: Colum-
New York: Aldine de Gruyter.
bia University Press.
Kirk, S. A., & Kutchins, H. (1994). The myth of the
Leo, J., & Cohen, D. (2003). Broken brains and
reliability of DSM. Journal of Mind and Behav-
flawed studies? A critical review of ADHD
ior, 15, 1–2.
neuroimaging research. The Journal of Mind
Kirk, S. A., Siporin, M., & Kutchins, H. (1989).
and Behavior, 24(1), 29–56.
The prognosis for social work diagnosis.
Levy, S. (1981). Labeling: The social worker’s
Social Casework: The Journal of Contemporary
responsibility. Social Casework, 62, 332–342.
Social Work, 70, 295–307.
Lewis, S. J. (2003). Do one shot preventive interven-
Kirsch, I., & Sapirstein, G. (1998). Listening to
tions for PTSD work? A systematic research
Prozac but hearing placebo: A meta-analysis
synthesis of psychological debriefings. Ag-
of antidepressant medication. Prevention &
gression and Violent Behavior, 8, 329–343
Treatment, 1, Article 0002a. Retrieved Janu-
Lubove, R. (1983). The professional altruist: The
ary 12, 2003, from http://journals.apa.org/
emergence of social work as a career, 1880–1930.
prevention/volume1/pre0010002a.html
New York: Atheneum.
406
JOURNAL OF SOCIAL WORK EDUCATION
Margolin, L. (1997). Under the cover of kindness: The invention of social work. Charlottesville, VA: University Press of Virginia. Maxmen, J. S., & Ward, N. G. (1995). Essential psychopathology and its treatment (2nd ed.). New York: Norton. McNeece, C. A., & DiNitto, D. M. (1998). Chemical dependency: A systems approach. Boston: Allyn & Bacon. Mechanic, D. (2000). Mental health policy at the
Mental Health (NIMH Publication No. 004500). Bethesda, MD: Author. Nickerson, R. S. (1998). Confirmation bias: A ubiquitous phenomenon in many guises. Review of General Psychology, 2(2), 175–220. Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1998). Abnormal psychology. Boston: McGraw-Hill. Peterson, M. (2002, August 21). Judge orders drug company to alter ads. New York Times, p. C6.
millennium: Challenges and opportunities.
Raffoul, P. R., & Holmes, K. A. (1986). DSM–III
In R. W. Manderscheid & M. J. Henderson
content in social work curricula: Results of a
(Eds.), Mental health, United States, 2000 (pp.
national survey. Journal of Social Work Educa-
53–63). Rockville, MD: United States De-
tion, 22(1), 24–31.
partment of Health and Human Services,
Reamer, F. G. (1993). The philosophical foundations
Substance Abuse and Mental Health Ser-
of social work. New York: Columbia Univer-
vices Administration. Miller, J. (2002). Social workers as diagnosticians. In K. J. Bentley (Ed.), Social work prac-
sity Press. Rosenhan, D. L. (1973). On being sane in insane places. Science, 179, 250–258.
tice in mental health: Contemporary roles, tasks
Ross, C. A., & Pam, A. (1995). Pseudoscience in
and techniques (pp. 43–72). Pacific Grove,
biological psychiatry: Blaming the body. New
CA: Brooks/Cole.
York: Wiley.
National Alliance for the Mentally Ill. (n.d.). Under-
Rothman, D. J. (1990). The discovery of the asylum:
standing major depression: What you need to know
Social order and disorder in the new republic.
about this medical illness. Retrieved January 11,
Boston: Little Brown.
2003, from http://www.nami.org/helpline/ UnderstandingMajorDepression.pdf
Royse, D. D. (1999). Research methods in social work (3rd ed.). Chicago: Nelson-Hall
National Institutes of Health. (1998). Diagnosis
Rubin, A., Cardenas, J., Warren, K., Pike, C., &
and treatment of Attention Deficit Hyper-
Wambach, K. (1998). Outdated practitioner
activity Disorder. NIH Consensus Statement
views about family culpability and severe
Online, 16(2), 1–37. Retrieved January 11, 2003,
mental disorders. Social Work, 43, 412–422.
from http://odp.od.nih.gov/consensus/ cons/110/110_statement.htm. National Institute of Mental Health. (1999). Wel-
Scheff, T. J. (1999). Being mentally ill: A sociological theory (3rd ed.). New York: Aldine de Gruyter.
come page. Retrieved January 23, 2003, from
Schoenfeld, C. G. (1976). An analysis of the views
http://www.nimh.nih.gov/about/
of Thomas S. Szasz. Journal of Psychiatry &
index.cfm
Law, 4, 245–263.
National Institute of Mental Health. (2000). Schizo-
Scull, A. T. (1981). Madhouses, mad-doctors, and
phrenia research at the National Institute of
madmen: The social history of psychiatry in the
IS MENTAL HEALTH EDUCATION CRITICAL? Victorian era. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.
407
Vatz, R. E., & Weinberg, L. S. (1994). The rhetorical paradigm in psychiatric history: Thomas
Shera, W., & Delva-Tauiliili, J. (1996). Changing
Szasz and the myth of mental illness. In M.
MSW students’ attitudes towards the se-
S. Micale & R. Porter (Eds.), Discovering the
verely mentally ill. Community Mental Health
history of psychiatry (pp. 311–330). New York:
Journal, 32(2), 159–169.
Oxford University.
Sherraden, M. W. (1991). Assets and the poor: A
Wakefield, J. C. (1992a). Disorder as harmful
new American welfare policy. Armonk, NY: M.
dysfunction—a conceptual critique of DSM–
E. Sharpe.
III–R’s definition of mental disorder. Psy-
Szasz, T. (1987). Insanity: The idea and its consequences. New York: Wiley. Szasz, T. (2001). Pharmacracy: Medicine and politics in America. Westport, CT: Praeger. Taylor, R. (1987). Biological basis of schizophrenia. Social Work, 32, 115–121.
chological Review, 99, 232–247. Wakefield, J. C. (1992b). The concept of mental disorder. American Psychologist, 47, 373–388. West, J., Kohout, J., Pion, G. M., Wicherski, M. M., Vandivort, R. E., Palmiter, M. L., et al. (2000). Mental health practitioners and train-
Thyer, B. A., & Wodarski, J. S. (Eds.). (1998).
ees. In R. W. Manderscheid & M. J.
Handbook of empirical social work practice. New
Henderson (Eds.), Mental health, United
York: Wiley.
States, 2000 (pp. 279–315). Rockville, MD:
Trattner, W. (1979). From poor law to welfare state:
United States Department of Health and
A history of social welfare in America (2nd ed.).
Human Services, Substance Abuse and Men-
New York: Free Press.
tal Health Services Administration.
Turner, F. J. (Ed.). (1999). Adult psychopathology:
White, R. D. (2002, October 11). California:
A social work perspective (2nd ed.). New York:
Order blocking Paxil ads is lifted. Los An-
Free Press.
geles Times, C2.
Tyson, K. (1991). The understanding and treat-
Widiger, T. A., & Sankis, L. M. (2000). Adult
ment of childhood hyperactivity: Old prob-
psychopathology: Issues and controversies.
lems and new approaches. Smith College Studies in Social Work, 61(2), 133–166.
Annual Review of Psychology, 51, 377–404. Williams, J. B., & Spitzer, R. L. (1995). Should the
U.S. Department of Health and Human Services.
DSM be the basis for teaching social work
(1999). Mental health: A report of the surgeon
practice in mental health? Yes! Journal of
general. Rockville, MD: U.S. Department of
Social Work Education, 31, 148–153.
Health and Human Services, Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration.
Wylie, M. S. (1995). Diagnosing for dollars? Family Therapy Networker, 19(3), 23–33, 65–69.
Valenstein, E. S. (1998). Blaming the brain: The
Zametkin, A. J., Nordahl, T. E., Gross, M.,
truth about drugs and mental health. New York:
King, A. C., Semple, W. E., Rumsey, J., et
Free Press.
al. (1990). Cerebral glucose metabolism in
Vatz, R. E., & Weinberg, L. S. (1983). Thomas
adults with hyperactivity of childhood
Szasz: Primary values and major contentions.
onset. New England Journal of Medicine, 323,
Buffalo, NY: Prometheus Books.
1361–1366.
408
JOURNAL OF SOCIAL WORK EDUCATION
Zametkin, A. J., Liebenauer, L. L., Fitzgerald, G. A., King, A. C., Minkunas, D. V.,
for
social
workers.
Belmont,
CA:
Wadsworth/Thomson Learning.
Herscovitch., P., et al. (1993). Brain me-
Zimbardo, P. C. (1999). Discontinuity theory:
tabolism in teenagers with attention-defi-
Cognitive and social searches for rationality
cit hyperactivity disorder. Archives of General
and normality may lead to madness. In M. P.
Psychiatry, 50, 333–340.
Zanna (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social
Zide, M. R., & Gray, S. W. (2001). Psychopathology: A competency-based assessment model
Psychology, 31, pp. 345–486. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Accepted: 07/03. Jeffrey R. Lacasse is a doctoral student and Tomi Gomory is assistant professor, School of Social Work, Florida State University. Address correspondence to Tomi Gomory, School of Social Work, Florida State University, Office-UCC 2410, Tallahassee, FL 32306-2570; email:
[email protected].