Laboratory Animals

3 downloads 0 Views 208KB Size Report
between the onset of labored breathing and loss of the righting reflex averaged 38.2 Ж 2.4s at a flow rate of 50% versus 59.2 Ж 2.4 s at 20% chamber vol/min of ...
Laboratory Animals http://lan.sagepub.com/

The effect of carbon dioxide flow rate on the euthanasia of laboratory mice CM Moody, B Chua and DM Weary Lab Anim 2014 48: 298 originally published online 5 August 2014 DOI: 10.1177/0023677214546509 The online version of this article can be found at: http://lan.sagepub.com/content/48/4/298

Published by: http://www.sagepublications.com

On behalf of: Laboratory Animals LtdLaboratory Animals Ltd

Additional services and information for Laboratory Animals can be found at: Email Alerts: http://lan.sagepub.com/cgi/alerts Subscriptions: http://lan.sagepub.com/subscriptions Reprints: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.nav Permissions: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav

>> Version of Record - Sep 12, 2014 OnlineFirst Version of Record - Aug 5, 2014 What is This?

Downloaded from lan.sagepub.com by guest on September 15, 2014

l

ab orator y

Original Article

a

n imals

l i m i t e d

The effect of carbon dioxide flow rate on the euthanasia of laboratory mice

Laboratory Animals 2014, Vol. 48(4) 298–304 ! The Author(s) 2014 Reprints and permissions: sagepub.co.uk/ journalsPermissions.nav DOI: 10.1177/0023677214546509 la.sagepub.com

CM Moody1, B Chua2 and DM Weary1

Abstract Laboratory rodents are commonly euthanized by exposure to gradually increasing concentrations of carbon dioxide (CO2). Current recommended flow rates range between 10 and 30% chamber vol/min and result in insensibility before exposure to painful concentrations (3% aversive. This study aimed to assess the effect of CO2 flow rates on time between the onset of dyspnea and various measures of insensibility (recumbency, loss of the righting reflex and loss of the pedal withdrawal reflex) to identify flow rates that minimize the potential experience of dyspnea. The results of this study indicate that a flow rate of 50% chamber vol/min, while holding the CO2 cage concentration just below 40%, minimizes the interval between the onset of labored breathing and recumbency. Using a 50% flow rate this interval averaged (SE) 30.3  2.9 s versus 49.7  2.9 s at 20% chamber vol/min (F3,22 ¼ 7.83, P ¼ 0.0013). Similarily, the interval between the onset of labored breathing and loss of the righting reflex averaged 38.2  2.4 s at a flow rate of 50% versus 59.2  2.4 s at 20% chamber vol/min of CO2 (F3,22 ¼ 13.62, P < 0.0001). We conclude that higher flow rates reduce the duration of dyspnea, but even at the highest flow rate mice experience more than 30 s between the onset of dyspnea and the most conservative estimate of insensibility.

Keywords euthanasia, dyspnea, induction, anesthesia

Carbon dioxide (CO2) gas is commonly used to kill laboratory rodents. Current guidelines1,2 suggest that the chamber should be filled gradually using a flow rate between 10 and 30% chamber vol/min of CO2. Use of flow rates lower than 30% chamber vol/min are thought to reduce the likelihood that CO2 concentration in the chamber will exceed painful levels (>40%) before insensibility is reached.3 Unfortunately, pain is not the only welfare concern associated with exposure to CO2. Humans report sensations of dyspnea, defined by the American Thoracic Society as ‘a subjective experience of breathing discomfort that consists of qualitatively distinct sensations that vary in intensity’.4 Humans describe the experience of dyspnea as distressing5–8 and this sensation has been used to induce fear and panic in humans using CO2 concentrations between 7.5 and 35%.9–12 Different types of dyspneic sensations have been identified resulting from pathological breathlessness, including air hunger, tightness and work.13 In the human literature,

the term dyspnea contains both an affective and behavioral response, as the negative emotional response resulting from dyspneic experiences results in aversion.13,14 In the veterinary literature, dyspnea typically refers to labored breathing in animals, as currently labored breathing is associated with a negative affective experience in mice or rats. However, previous studies in mice and rats have shown that CO2 concentrations ranging from 3–20% are aversive,15–21 concentrations between 10 and 35% have been shown to cause fear responses,22–24 and 10% CO2 may be used as an 1

Animal Welfare Program, Faculty of Land and Food Systems, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, Canada 2 Centre for Comparative Medicine, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, Canada Corresponding author: Daniel M Weary, Animal Welfare Program, University of British Columbia, 2357 Main Mall, Vancouver, BC V6T 1Z4, Canada. Email: [email protected]

Downloaded from lan.sagepub.com by guest on September 15, 2014

Moody et al.

299

unconditioned fear stimulus in mice.24 In the current study, labored breathing was directly assessed but we cannot be certain that this behavior was associated with a negative affect. However, on the basis of human evidence and the rodent research on aversion and fear responses, we posit that this autonomic response is accompanied by a negative affective experience. On this basis, we argue that euthanasia methods that minimize the duration of labored breathing should be considered more humane. When the term dyspnea is used in this paper, we refer to the veterinary definition meaning ‘labored breathing’, while acknowledging that the known rodent aversion to CO2 (as referenced above) is likely accompanied by a negative affective experience. The aim of this study was to examine the effect of CO2 flow rates on the interval from the onset of labored breathing until loss of sensibility. This interval is the period during which the animals may consciously experience a negative affect associated with dyspnea. Since the moment mice are rendered insensible is unknown, several indicators of insensibility were assessed, as examination of only one parameter may be misleading.25 There are at least three progressive measures of loss of sensibility during euthanasia: recumbency, loss of the righting reflex and loss of the pedal withdrawal reflex (Table 1). The onset of recumbency is the first and easiest indicator to identify, and is observed as loss of muscle tone.26 Loss of the righting reflex is commonly assessed during rodent euthanasia by tilting the euthanasia box to roll the animal, or placing the animal on its back to examine self-righting behavior;27 this measure cannot be performed practically when enclosures are large and heavy, such as in some automated systems. Loss of the pedal withdrawal reflex is an autonomic response of the hind limb that is assessed by pinching the hind paws.28 This reflex is commonly used to determine a surgical plane of

Table 1. Definitions of mouse behaviors used in this study to assess dyspnea and various levels of insensibility. Behaviour

Definition

Onset of labored breathing Onset of recumbency

Deep rapid breathing

Loss of righting reflex Loss of pedal withdrawal reflex

Head resting on cage floor, head and body motionless, loss of muscle tone Unable to self-right when placed on back The first of three consecutive non-responses to alternating hind paw pinches

anesthesia, when an animal cannot experience pain and surgery may be performed.28 We hypothesized that of the flow rates tested in this study (20, 30, 40 and 50%), the higher flow rates would minimize the experience of dyspnea, as measured from the onset of labored breathing to recumbency, loss of the righting reflex, and loss of the pedal withdrawal reflex, for mice euthanized using the gradual-fill method of CO2 euthanasia. The 20 and 30% flow rates were chosen because they are commonly recommended, and the 40 and 50% rates were chosen based on the prediction that these would shorten the interval between the onset of dyspnea and insensibility. Throughout the study, cage concentration was not allowed to exceed painful levels (>40%) via a gas holding technique, until all three measures of insensibility had been satisfied.

Materials and methods Animals and housing We used 23 surplus naive female albino C57BL/6J-Tyr mice (British Columbia Cancer Research Centre, Vancouver, Canada) at the University of British Columbia’s Centre for Comparative Medicine, Vancouver, Canada. Mice were five months old during testing and weighed 21.6–28.4 g. Mice were group-housed in an OptiMICEÕ (Animal Care Systems, Centennial, CO, USA) cage system with autoclaved clean polycarbonate cages (MakrolonÕ , Animal Care Systems) with dimensions 31.8 cm long  27.9 cm wide front  8.9 cm wide rear  12.9 cm height. Each cage contained autoclaved ECOfreshTM (Absorption Corporation, Ferndale, WA, USA) bedding, a nest box, a cotton nesting square (Ancare, Bellmore, NY, USA), brown crinkle paper (Enviro-driÕ ; Shepherd Specialty Papers Inc, Richland, MI, USA) and free access to food (5001 PMI Lab Diet; Harlan Laboratories Inc, Indianapolis, IN, USA) and filtered water. The average humidity and temperature during testing were 48% and 23 C, respectively. Mice were kept under a 12 h light:12 h dark cycle; testing took place during the light phase (between 08:00 and 10:00 h) on two consecutive days. The University of British Columbia’s Animal Care Committee approved all the procedures used in this study.

Experimental apparatus An InnocageÕ mouse disposable individual ventilated cage (IVC) transparent mouse cage (InnocageÕ , Universal Euro Type II Long, Innovive Inc, San Diego, CA, USA; 37.3 cm L  23.4 cm W  14.0 cm H, with 205 cm2 floor space) was used as the test

Downloaded from lan.sagepub.com by guest on September 15, 2014

300

Laboratory Animals 48(4)

cage (Figure 1). On one side of the cage a hole was cut to project a powdered surgical latex glove (PerryÕ Style 42Õ , Ansell, size 7, Ansell, New Jersey, USA) sealed with tape. This allowed one hand to be placed into the cage during testing. A non-slip pad made from wood flooring underlayment (Shaw Floors; Shaw Industries, Dalton, GA, USA) was cut to fit the bottom of the test cage to minimize slipping. A clear Plexiglas lid with a small hole in the middle was placed on top of the cage to allow insertion of a tube to deliver the CO2. Before each trial, 500 mL of aspen-chip bedding was added to the test cage and removed after each euthanasia procedure. The tube delivering gas to the test cage was connected to a CO2 tank (Praxair, Delta, BC, Canada) and the flow was measured with a CO2 flow meter (Western Medica, Westlake, OH, USA). A small hole near the base of the cage in the center of the anterior wall of the test cage allowed insertion of the sampling tube connected to an O2 analyzer (Series 2000, Percent Oxygen Analyzer; Alpha Omega Instrument Corporation, Cumberland, RI, USA). The sampling tube was placed at the base of the cage such that gas samples within the test cage were taken at a similar height to that of the mouse exposure.

Oxygen analyzer testing Prior to experimental testing, the lag time of the O2 analyzer was measured as the time from insertion of an anoxic sample until the time the reading on the analyzer began to decline. Repeat testing showed this delay to be 10 s. When euthanizing mice with gradual-fill CO2, an uneven distribution of CO2 occurs within the euthanasia chamber due to flow rate turbulence and CO2 heaviness in comparison to air. To assess variability in CO2 concentration within the test cage, the sampling tube independently measured seven different areas (all four corners, the center, and the two ends of the test

cage with placement in the middle at each end), one area at a time, with a CO2 flow rate of 20% cage vol/ min. O2 concentrations were recorded every 15 s for 5 min. CO2 concentration in the test cage was calculated: [CO2(t¼x)] ¼ 100 – (100  ([O2(t¼x)]/[O2(t¼0)])). Values for the anterior left corner were found to be most similar to the average of the readings for the test cage (see Figure 1 for placement of the O2 analyzer tube).

Experimental procedure One researcher retrieved a mouse from the housing room while another cleaned the apparatus, added 500 mL of bedding, and a stainless steel 3.5 inch straight mosquito hemostat (Lawton, Fridingen, Germany), and placed the O2 sampling tube through the cage hole into the left anterior corner of the test box. Higher flow rates result in a faster accumulation of CO2 within the test box. During each trial the O2 analyzer readings were monitored and the gas flow was altered via manual adjustment of the CO2 flow meter such that CO2 cage concentration was held just below 40%. In this way conscious mice were never subjected to CO2 concentrations associated with pain. At the beginning of each trial a mouse was placed into the test cage. The Plexiglas lid was placed on top of the cage and the CO2 tube was placed into the centrally placed lid hole. A researcher then placed one hand into the test cage glove and kept the hand motionless on the floor of the cage. Trials began with an onset of CO2 into the cage; mice were randomly assigned to one of four flow rates: 20 (n ¼ 6), 30 (n ¼ 6), 40 (n ¼ 6), or 50 (n ¼ 6) % cage vol/min. Once the mouse was recumbent the experimenter (blind to treatment) tested for loss of the righting reflex by placing the mouse on its back. Three mice attempted to escape the approaching hand and in these cases the researcher waited until the animal was recumbent again for 3 s, before re-testing. Immediately after failure to self-right, loss of the pedal withdrawal reflex was tested, and then re-tested every 10 s using the hemostat (first notch) to pinch alternating hind paw interdigital webbing. Loss of the pedal withdrawal reflex was signified by the absence of a response to three consecutive pinches. After loss of this reflex, gas flow was increased to 60% cage vol/min until the mouse was no longer breathing. The gas was then turned off and cervical dislocation was used to ensure death.

Data collection

Figure 1. Diagram of experimental set-up.

Each trial was recorded using a high definition camera (Model TM41P, Panasonic Corporation, Osaka, Japan). Videos were scored for: onset of labored

Downloaded from lan.sagepub.com by guest on September 15, 2014

Moody et al.

301

breathing, onset of recumbency, loss of the righting reflex, and loss of the pedal withdrawal reflex (Table 1), with observers blind to treatment. We calculated the interval between onset of labored breathing– onset of recumbency, onset of labored breathing–loss of the righting reflex, and onset of labored breathing– loss of the pedal withdrawal reflex. Before the experiment, all behavioral scoring was practiced to establish inter-observer reliability. Video recordings of previous mouse euthanasia procedures were viewed and scored independently by two observers; all score times were consistent (2 s) between observers. One observation from the 30% treatment group was identified as an extreme outlier (more than three standard deviations above the mean) for loss of the pedal withdrawal reflex and was removed from the analysis, resulting in five subjects for this treatment.

affected by gas flow (F3,22 ¼ 0.57, P ¼ 0.64). Less conservative estimates of insensibility (recumbency and loss of the righting reflex) did vary with gas flow (F3,22 ¼ 7.12, P ¼ 0.0021 and F3,22 ¼ 11.68, P ¼ 0.0001, respectively). Mice became recumbent approximately 22 s sooner when exposed to a 50% versus 20% flow rate of CO2. Similarly, loss of the righting reflex occurred at approximately 51 s at the highest flow rate versus 75 s at the lowest flow rate. The interval between onset of labored breathing and onset of recumbency was reduced with increasing flow rate (F3,22 ¼ 7.83, P ¼ 0.0013, Figure 2a), as was the interval between onset of labored breathing and loss of the righting reflex (F3,22 ¼ 13.62, P < 0.0001, Figure 2b).

(a) 60

The effect of flow rate (three degrees of freedom) on behavioral responses was tested with a general linear model (Proc GLM in SAS v. 9.2; SAS Institute Inc, Cary, NC, USA) that included the home cage as a block (with eight degrees of freedom) and mouse body weight as a covariate (one degree of freedom). Least-square means  one standard error are reported below.

45

Time (s)

Statistical analysis

15 0 20

30

40

50

CO2 Flow Rate (%)

(b) Time (s)

60

Results The onset of dyspnea (i.e. the onset of labored breathing) occurred approximately 14 s after the start of gas flow; onset and dyspnea did not vary with gas flow (F3,22 ¼ 0.70, P ¼ 0.56; Table 2). Our most conservative estimate of insensibility (loss of the pedal withdrawal reflex) occurred approximately 109 s after CO2 began to flow into the cage, again with no effect of flow rate (F3,22 ¼ 0.66, P ¼ 0.59), but with considerable between-subject variation. The interval between the onset of labored breathing and the pedal withdrawal reflex averaged approximately 95 s, and was also not

30

45 30 15 0 20

30

40

50

CO2 Flow Rate (%)

Figure 2. Mean (SE) interval in which mice exposed to a 20, 30, 40 or 50% flow rate of gradual-fill CO2 may have experienced dyspnea. Two periods are shown: (a) onset of labored breathing until recumbency, and (b) onset of labored breathing until loss of the righting reflex.

Table 2. Mean (SE) time of first sign of dyspnea and three measures of insensibility in mice euthanized using gradual-fill CO2 at flow rates of 20, 30, 40, and 50% chamber vol/min. CO2 flow rates (%) Behavioral parameter

20

30

40

50

Dyspnea onset Recumbency onset Loss of righting reflex Loss of pedal reflex

15.5  1.5 65.2  3.4 74.7  2.9 119.2  10.0

13.8  1.6 54.8  3.8 63.4  3.2 98.8  11.0

12.8  1.5 50.3  3.4 57.0  2.9 110.3  10.0

13.0  1.5 43.3  3.4 51.2  2.9 106.2  10.0

Downloaded from lan.sagepub.com by guest on September 15, 2014

302

Laboratory Animals 48(4)

Discussion The onset of labored breathing occurred at a similar time for all the flow rates tested, suggesting that dyspnea may begin soon after exposure to even low concentrations of CO2. The gas flow rate had a stronger effect on the time until mice became recumbent and lost their righting reflex (and also on the intervals between the onset of labored breathing and these measures). These results are consistent with the idea that loss of sensibility will occur more quickly when animals are exposed to a higher cumulative dose of anesthetic.29 Loss of the pedal withdrawal reflex did not vary with flow rate, and this measure showed considerable variability among mice. It is possible that some variation may be due to mice spending more or less time in areas of the cage with higher CO2 concentrations. Past studies30,31 suggest that use of a hemostat to assess loss of the pedal withdrawal reflex is the best indicator of a surgical depth of anesthesia in mice. However, these previous studies used a locking pin on the hemostat whereas we used the first notch on the hemostat. The variation in this measure may indicate difficulty in applying the hemostat in a consistent manner or differences among animals in their response to the pinch. We suggest that further work is needed to assess the repeatability of methods used to assess loss of the pedal withdrawal reflex. A study by Coenen et al.32 suggested that when euthanizing rats with CO2, loss of posture and muscle tone (i.e. recumbency) was correlated with the onset of an abnormal electroencephalogram pattern, and the authors suggest this indicates loss of consciousness. However, in many animal species, nociceptive reflexes such as loss of righting and loss of pedal withdrawal are commonly used to assess insensibility.25,33 It has been suggested that failure to respond to a verbal command in humans is correlated with loss of the righting reflex in rodents, both signifying loss of consciousness.33 The correlation between loss of consciousness and lack of ability to respond to a verbal command in humans has been recognized since the introduction of anesthesia.34 However, this definition may be problematic; awareness (appropriate response to a command) and memory may be lost at anesthetic concentrations below 50% of those needed to abolish movement.25 Given the lack of knowledge in this area, several indicators should be used when assessing insensibility. It is possible that indicators used to assess insensibility may vary with strain. Minimum alveolar concentrations (that prevent purposeful movement) differ across strains, and depend upon the type of insensibility indicator and the type of anesthetic used.35 We used the interval from onset of labored breathing to: (1) recumbency, (2) loss of the righting reflex, and (3) loss of the pedal withdrawal reflex, to assess the time

when mice may experience unpleasant sensations associated with dyspnea. The most relevant of the three intervals assessed in this study depends upon when mice undergoing gradual-fill CO2 euthanasia are no longer able to experience a negative affect. From a welfare perspective, the best case would be that mice are unresponsive after the onset of recumbency, suggesting that they consciously experience between 30 and 50 s of dyspnea, depending on the flow rate. The worst case is that they are able to experience a negative affect associated with dyspnea up until loss of the pedal withdrawal reflex (i.e. 90 s or more) with no benefit from a faster flow rate. Severity of any negative affect should be considered in addition to the duration of the experience to assess the welfare effects of different procedures. In humans, CO2-induced air hunger results in a conscious awareness of the urge to breath, evoked by hypercapnia.13 CO2 causes hypercapnia and a reduction in blood pH;36 increased respiration is the body’s attempt to eliminate excess CO2. The inability of the body to overcome this excess results in secondary effects including the physical and emotional components of dyspnea. In a future study, it would be interesting to examine when each of the three insensibility parameters occurred in relation to partial pressure of CO2 in the blood of mice being euthanized with CO2. A study by Ziemann et al.24 examined four paradigms to assess CO2 as a fear-inducing stimulus in mice, by examining CO2 and: (1) freezing behavior, (2) open-field test, (3) aversion, and (4) fear conditioning. A 10% concentration of CO2 was found to cause more freezing behavior and reduced time in an openfield test. Also mice with the choice between a chamber with 90% of their time in the chamber with the lower CO2 concentration. In the fear conditioning test, mice subjected to 10% CO2 before and while receiving foot shocks showed more freezing behavior than those not subjected to CO2 while receiving the foot shocks. When re-tested the following day without CO2, mice again showed more freezing behavior if they had previously been subjected to the CO2. These results indicate that CO2 gas is both fear inducing and aversive in mice, even at the relatively low concentration of 10%. In addition, a series of studies have now shown that rodents do not willingly tolerate exposure to similar CO2 concentrations.15–21 Therefore, if CO2 is used to kill rodents, refinements to minimize distress during this procedure are important. Our study results indicate that a gradual-fill CO2 flow rate of 50% cage vol/min reduced the period from onset of labored breathing until onset of recumbency and loss of the righting reflex. When using this flow rate, a gas holding technique should be used to ensure that painful CO2 concentrations (>40%) are

Downloaded from lan.sagepub.com by guest on September 15, 2014

Moody et al.

303

not reached until after insensibility occurs. This can be achieved by manually controlling the flow meter or by using a programmable automated euthanasia machine. Once a mouse has become insensible, it is up to the user how to proceed. The animal may remain in the test cage at a concentration of just below 40% CO2 until death, or the flow rate of CO2 may be turned up to decrease time to death, or the animal may be taken out and subjected to a secondary method of euthanasia such as cervical dislocation.

Conclusion The results of this study indicate that the time until recumbency and loss of the righting reflex in mice can be reduced using higher flow rates of gradual-fill CO2 (e.g. 50% versus 20% chamber vol/min). When using higher flow rates, a gas holding technique should be used to ensure that painful CO2 concentrations (>40%) are not reached until after insensibility occurs. Even when using this refinement, mice likely experience more than 30 s of dyspnea before insensibility occurs. Acknowledgements We thank the British Columbia Cancer Research Centre for donating the surplus animals used in this study and the staff at the Centre for Comparative Medicine for taking care of the mice for the duration of the study. This study could not have been conducted without the support of Chris Harvey-Clark and technical help from Gordon Gray. We would also like to thank Joanna Makowska for reviewing this manuscript, as well as Andre Vits and Giovanni Mancini for many conversations regarding mouse euthanasia. We are grateful to Warren Riley (Innovive Inc) for donating the test cage used in this study. This work was supported by an NSERC Discovery grant to D M Weary.

References 1. AVMA (American Veterinary Medical Associated). Guidelines for the euthanasia of animals: 2013 edition, https://www.avma.org/KB/Policies/Documents/euthanasia. pdf (2013, accessed 20 August 2013). 2. CCAC (Canadian Council on Animal Care). CCAC guidelines on: euthanasia of animals used in science, http:// www.ccac.ca/Documents/Standards/Guidelines/ Euthanasia.pdf (2010, accessed 20 August 2013). 3. Ambrose N, Wadham J and Morton D. Refinement in euthanasia. In: Balls M, van Zeller AM and Halder ME (eds) Progress in the reduction, refinement and replacement of animal experimentation. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science, 2000, pp.1159–1169. 4. American Thoracic Society. American Thoracic Society ad hoc Committee. Dyspnea mechanisms, assessment and management: a consensus statement. Am J Respir Crit Care Med 1999; 159: 321–340.

5. Banzett RB, Lansing RW, Brown R, et al. ‘Air hunger’ from increased PCO2 persists after complete neuromuscular block in humans. Resp Physiol 1990; 81: 1–17. 6. Lansing RW, Im BSH, Thwing JI, et al. The perception of respiratory work and effort can be independent of the perception of air hunger. Am J Resp Crit Care Med 2000; 162: 1690–1696. 7. O’Driscoll M, Corner J and Bailey C. The experience of breathlessness in lung cancer. Eur J Cancer Care (Engl) 1999; 8: 37–43. 8. Von Leupoldt A and Dahme B. Differentiation between the sensory and affective dimensions of dyspnea during resistive load breathing in normal subjects. Chest 2005; 128: 3345–3349. 9. Bailey JF, Argyropoulos SV, Kendrick AH, et al. Behavioral and cardiovascular effects of 7.5% CO2 in human volunteers. Depress Anxiety 2005; 21: 18–25. 10. Feinstein JS, Buzza C, Hurlemann R, et al. Fear and panic in humans with bilateral amygdala damage. Nat Neurosci 2013; 16: 270–272. 11. Gorman JM, Askanazi J, Liebowitz MR, et al. Response to hyperventilation in a group of patients with panic disorder. Am J Psychiatry 1984; 141: 857–861. 12. Pappens M, De Peuter S, Vansteenwegen D, Van den Bergh O and Van Diest I. Psychophysiological responses to CO2 inhalation. Int J Psychophysiol 2012; 84: 45–50. 13. Lansing RW, Graely RH and Banzett RB. The multiple dimensions of dyspnea: review and hypotheses. Resp Physiol Neurobiol 2009; 167: 53–60. 14. Steel B and Shaver J. The dyspnea experience: Nociceptive properties and a model for research and practice. Adv Nurs Sci 1992; 15: 64–76. 15. Kirkden RD, Niel L, Lee G, et al. The validity of using an approach-avoidance test to measure the strength of aversion to carbon dioxide in rats. Appl Anim Behav Sci 2008; 114: 216–234. 16. Krohn TC, Hansen AK and Dragsted N. The impact of low levels of carbon dioxide on rats. Lab Anim 2003; 37: 94–99. 17. Leach MC, Bowell VA, Allan TF, et al. Aversion to gaseous euthanasia agents in rats and mice. Comp Med 2002; 52: 249–257. 18. Makowska IJ, Vickers L, Mancell J, et al. Evaluating methods of gas euthanasia for laboratory mice. Appl Anim Behav Sci 2009; 121: 230–235. 19. Niel L, Stewart SA and Weary DM. Effect of flow rate on aversion to gradual-fill carbon dioxide exposure in rats. Appl Anim Behav Sci 2008; 109: 77–84. 20. Niel L and Weary DM. Rats avoid exposure to carbon dioxide and argon. Appl Anim Behav Sci 2007; 107: 100–109. 21. Wong D, Makowska IJ and Weary DM. Rat aversion to isoflurane versus carbon dioxide. Biol Lett 2013; 9: 20121000. 22. Concas A, Sanna E, Cuccheddu T, et al. Carbon dioxide inhalation, stress and anxiogenic drugs reduce the function of GABAA receptor complex in the rat brain. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 1993; 17: 651–661.

Downloaded from lan.sagepub.com by guest on September 15, 2014

304

Laboratory Animals 48(4)

23. Niel L and Weary DM. Behavioural responses of rats to gradual-fill carbon dioxide euthanasia and reduced oxygen concentrations. Appl Anim Behav Sci 2006; 100: 295–308. 24. Ziemann AE, Allen JE, Dahdaleh NS, et al. The amygdala is a chemosensor that detects carbon dioxide and acidosis to elicit fear behavior. Cell 2009; 135: 1012–1021. 25. Antognini JF, Barter L and Carstens E. Movement as an index of anesthetic depth in humans and experimental animals. Comp Med 2005; 55: 413–418. 26. Hewitt TA, Kovacs MS, Artwohl JE, et al. A comparison of euthanasia methods in rats, using carbon dioxide in prefilled and fixed flow rate filled chambers. Lab Anim Sci 1993; 43: 579–582. 27. Thomas AA, Flecknell PA and Golledge HDR. Combining nitrous oxide with carbon dioxide decrease the time to loss of consciousness during euthanasia in mice – refinement of animal welfare? PLoS One 2012; 7: e32290. 28. Whelan G and Flecknell P. The assessment of depth of anesthesia in animals and man. Lab Anim 1992; 26: 153–162. 29. Clark DL and Rosner BS. Neurophysiologic effects of general anesthetics: I. The electroencephalogram and

30.

31.

32.

33.

34. 35.

36.

sensory evoked responses in man. Anesthesiology 1973; 35: 564–582. Arras A, Autenried P, Rettich A, et al. Optimization of intraperitoneal injection anesthesia in mice: drugs, dosages, adverse effects, and anesthesia depth. Comp Med 2001; 51: 443–456. Buitrago S, Martin TE, Tetens-Woodring J, et al. Safety and efficacy of various combinations of injectable anesthetics in BALB/c mice. J Am Assoc Lab Anim Sci 2008; 47: 11–17. Coenen AML, Drinkenburg WHIM, Hoederken R, et al. Carbon dioxide euthanasia in rats: oxygen supplementation minimizes signs of agitation and asphyxia. Lab Anim 1995; 29: 262–268. Franks NP. General anesthesia: from molecular targets to neuronal pathways of sleep and arousal. Nat Rev Neurosci 2008; 9: 370–386. Alkire MT, Hudetz AG and Tononi G. Consciousness and anesthesia. Science 2008; 322: 786–880. Mogil JS, Smith SB, O’Reilly MK, et al. Influence of nociception and stress-induced antinociception on genetic variation in isoflurane anesthetic potency among mouse strains. Anesthesiology 2005; 103: 751–780. Guais A, Brand G, Jacquot L, et al. Toxicity of carbon dioxide: a review. Chem Res Toxicol 2011; 24: 2061–2070.

Downloaded from lan.sagepub.com by guest on September 15, 2014