Localizing Arithmetic Processes in the Brain - Johns Hopkins University

1 downloads 0 Views 1MB Size Report
brain tumor and associated seizures, underwent for clini- ..... At one inferior frontal site, stimulation produced audi- ... Uematsu, S., & Fisher, R. S. (1990).
Localizing Arithmetic Processes in the Brain: Evidence from a Transient Deficit During Cortical Stimulation John Whalen, Michael McCloskey, Ronald P. Lesser, and Barry Gordon The Johns Hopkins University

Abstract Although substantial progress has been made in characterizing the cognitive processes involved in simple arithmetic, the localization of these processes in the brain is not yet well understood. In this article we consider the localization of a specific arithmetic process, the retrieval of arithmetic table facts from memory. We report a single-patient study in which cortical stimulation was used to create transient disruption of

brain activity in localized regions of the cortex. We show that stimulation at a left parietal site impaired performance on simple multiplication problems and further that the impairment reflected stimulation-induced disruption of arithmetic fact retrieval. Our findings support the hypothesis (e.g.,Warrington, 1982) that the left parietal lobe is implicated in the arithmetic fact retrieval process.

INTRODUCTION

sought to relate brain areas to arithmetic tusks and not to specific cognitive processes such as arithmetic fact retrieval. For example, studies exploring lesiondeficit correlations have typically focused on identrfying the lesion loci associated with impaired performance on some calculation task or tasks (e.g., Benson & Weir, 1972; Cohn, 1961; Corbett et al., 1986; Grafman, Kampen, Rosenberg, Salazar, & Boller, 1989; Luccelli & De Renzi, 1993; Takayama, Sugishita, Akiguchi, & Kimura, 1994). However, an association between a lesion site and impairment on a calculation task does not in itself constitute strong evidence that the damaged brain area is implicated in arithmetic processing, because the impaired performance could have resulted from disruption to nonarithmetic processes required by the task (e.g., attention,working memory, numeral comprehension,numeral production). Although some researchers have attempted to control for this possibility, the controls have usually not been sufficient to rule out plausible nonarithmetic interpretations for the calculation task impairment. These points also apply to research involving electrophysiological recording and cortical stimulation methods. In several recent studies subjects performed a serial subtractions task (e.g., count backward by 7s from 100) while brain activity was recorded (Roland & Friberg, 1985; Inouye et al., 1993) or cortical stimulation was applied (Morris, Luders, Lesser, Dinner, & Hahn, 1984; Ojemann, 1974).A number of brain areas were found to be associated with performance on the task, including the left parietal lobe (Roland & Friberg, 1985; Inouye

Solving arithmetic problems (e.g., 47 x 83) requires a variety of cognitive processes, including comprehension and production of digits (e.g., 3,7),retrieval of arithmetic “table”facts (e.g., 3 x 7 = 21), and execution of procedures specrfying the sequence of steps to be carried out (e.g., multiply the digits in the rightmost column, write the 1s digit of the product, and so forth). Recent research with normal subjects and braindamaged patients has contributed substantially to our understanding of these processes at a functional level (e.g., Ashcraft, 1992;Dehaene & Cohen, 1995;McCloskey, 1992;McCloskey, Harley, & Sokol, 1991). However, less progress has been made in discovering how (or whether) arithmetic processes are localized in the brain (see Kahn & Whitaker, 1991, for a recent review). A number of findings suggest that posterior cortical regions--and especially parietal regions-may play a role in arithmetic abilities (e.g., Benson & Weir, 1972; Grafman, Passafiume, Faglioni, & Boller, 1982; Hecaen, Angelergues & Houillier, 1961 ;Inouye, Shinosaki, Iyama, & Matsumoto, 1993;Jackson & Warrington, 1986;Rosselli & Ardila, 1989;Warrington, 1982). However, many other brain areas have also been implicated, including the frontal lobes (Lucchelli & De Renzi, 1993; Roland & Friberg, 1985), thalamus (Ojemann, 1974),and basal ganglia (Corbett, McCusker, & Davidson, 1986; HittmairDelazer, Semenza, & Denes, 1994). Interpreting the available evidence is not entirely straightforward, in part because most studies have 0 1997 Massachusetts Institute of Technology

et al., 1993;Morris et al., 1984), the frontal lobes (Roland & Friberg, 1985),and thalamic regions (Ojemann, 1974). Although intriguing, these findings do not necessarily imply that the identified brain areas are implicated in arithmetic processing;the reported data do not exclude the possibility that some or all of the brain areas are instead involved in nonarithmetic processes required by the serial subtractions task ( e g , comprehending the initial stimulus number, producing responses).

Arithmetic Fact Retrieval In this article we consider the localization of a particular arithmetic process, the retrieval of arithmetic table facts (eg., 6 x 7 = 42). Evidence bearing specifically on this issue is sparse. Warrington (1982) described a patient (DRC) with a calculation deficit, demonstrating through careful cognitive testing that he was selectively impaired in retrieval of arithmetic facts. CT revealed a focal lesion in the left posterior parieto-occipital region, suggesting that this area may play a role in arithmetic fact retrieval. More recently,Hittmair-Delazeret al. (1994) documented an arithmetic fact retrieva! impairment in a patient (BE) with a lesion confined to the left basal ganglia and concluded that this region may also be implicated in the fact retrieval process. Although several other cases of selectively impaired arithmetic fact retrieval have been reported (e.g., Dagenbach & McCloskey, 1992;Dehaene & Cohen, 1991; McCloskey, Aliminosa, & Sokol, 1991; McCloskey, Caramazza, & Basili, 1985;Sokol, McCloskey, Cohen, & Aliminosa, 1991), the brain lesions in these cases were very extensive, or lesion localization data were not available. In the present study we used cortical stimulation to explore the localization of arithmetic fact retrieval. Patient AA, a young man suffering from a left-hemisphere brain tumor and associated seizures,underwent for clinical purposes a procedure in which arrays of electrodes were surgically placed on the surface of the left hemisphere. The arrays, which remained in place until resec-

right arm and periods of unresponsiveness.Rarely, these progressed to general tonic-clonic seizures. Neurological evaluation revealed a tumor in the left parasagittal parietal region, identified upon resection as a grade C oligodendroglioma. WADA testing prior to surgery revealed that the left hemisphere was dominant for language. In 1992, at age 16, AA again began to experience seizures.When these proved refractory to medication,he was admitted to the Johns Hopkins Hospital for evaluation. MRI with and without gadolinium contrast suggested recurrence of the tumor at the same location (Figure l), and this diagnosis was subsequently confirmed at surgery.

Neuropsychological Evaluation Neuropsychological assessment carried out prior to the cortical stimulation study revealed that despite the tumor and seizure disorder, AA’s performance was normal on tests of general intellectual and language abilities. On the WISC-R (Wechsler, 1974) he obtained a full-scale IQ of 97 (Verbal = 103,Performance = 91). He scored 89/90 (99%)for auditory comprehension on the Token Test (De Renzi & Vignolo, 1962), and 88/90 (98%) for reading comprehension.Performance was normal (54/60) on the Boston Naming Test (Goodglass & Kaplan, 1983), and oral word reading was performed without error (1 17/117). Spontaneous speech was fluent and meaningful,without obvious articulatory difficulties,word-finding problems, or paraphasias. On the Wide Range Achievement Test (WRAT;Jastak & Wilkinson, 1984) AA scored at the eleventh grade level in reading, and the tenth grade level in spelling.Because of school missed as a result of his seizures and the ensuing surgery in 1986, AA was completing grade 9 at the time of testing, one grade behind that expected for his age. His reading and spelling scores are therefore above average for someone at his grade level and at or above the norm for students of his age.

tion of the tumor 16 days later, were used for recording aimed at localizing the seizure foci and for stimulation carried out to map the functions of cortical areas that might be affected in the resection. In the functional mapping mild stimulation was applied briefly across adjacent electrodes, creating transient disruption of brain activity in localized regions. Sensory, motor, and cognitive tasks were presented during stimulation to determine what functions (if any) were transiently impaired. The cortical stimulation results reported in this article come from the clinical stimulation testing, supplemented by additional testing carried out for research purposes.

CASE HISTORY Patient AA is a left-handed male. In 1986, at age 9, he began to experience seizures involving shaking of the 41 0

Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience

Assessment of Numerical Processing In the cortical stimulation study singledigit multiplication and addition problems were presented visually (e.g., 8 x 7),and AA said the answer aloud (e.g., “fifty-six”). These tasks require comprehension of numerals in arabic form (eg., 8, 7), retrieval of arithmetic table facts (e.g., 8 x 7 = 56), and production of numerals in the form of spoken words. Accordingly, several tasks designed to probe these processes were administered in the absence of stimulation. AA was 99% correct (1 19/120) across six tasks in which he translated numerals from one form to another (e.g., written stimulus 2041, spoken response ”two thousand forty-one”),suggesting intact numeral comprehension and production abilities. For tasks requiring arabic Volume 9, Number 3

Figure 1. Horizontal and coronal MRI sections from scan conducted at the time of the cortical stimulation study, prior to AA's second resection surgery.

numeral comprehension or spoken numeral production he was 100% correct (80/80). On speeded tests of singledigit multiplication and addition (e.g., 9 x 4 , 8 + 6 ) AA was 98%correct (246/250) for multiplication and 100% correct (45/45) for addition. Mean correct reaction time was 1644 msec for multiplication and 1562 msec for addition. These results demonstrate that AA was able to solve the problems by retrieving stored arithmetic table facts, since the re-

sponse times are considerably faster than would be expected (see, eg., Ashcraft, 1982;Siegler, 1988) if he were using some nonretrieval strategy (e.g., adding seven 8s to solve 8 x 7). On pencil-and-paper tests of addition,subtraction,and multiplication AA was 98% correct (59/60) for singledigit problems and 83% correct (25/30) for multidigit problems (eg., 413 x 59),within normal limits for adults (McCloskey,Aliminosa, & Macaruso, 1991).The errors on Whalen et al.

41 I

Inferior Fronta1

Posterior Frontal

Anterior Parietal

Medial Parietal

Figure 2. Diagram of location of specific neuropsychological deficits produced by electrical stimulation.

multidigit problems reflected occasional slips in executing calculation procedures (e.g., forgetting a carry digit); retrieval of arithmetic table facts on these problems was uniformly accurate. Further evidence of intact fact retrieval comes from the arithmetic subtest of the WRAT. Although AA scored at the seventh grade level,his errors stemmed from difficulty in dealing with algebra, exponents, and quadratics; performance on arithmetic facts was excellent. The numerical processing results suggest that in spite of the tumor and associated seizures, AA’s numerical abilities are relatively normal. In particular, the numeral comprehension,numeral production, and arithmetic fact retrieval processes required for the cortical stimulation study appear to be intact. 412

Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience

RESULTS Transient Impairment of Multiplication Performance Singledigit multiplication was tested during cortical stimulation at each pair of adjacent electrodes in the inferior frontal,anterior parietal,and medial parietal electrode arrays (Figure 2), except for sites where stimulation was found to affect motor or sensory functions. Problems were presented in digit form (e.g., 9 x 4), and AA was allowed up to 7 sec to say the answer aloud. Stimulation appeared to affect multiplication performance for only one electrode pair: AP18-26, located in the anterior parietal region. The AP18-26 site is distant from both the tumor and the seizure foci (which were Volume 3,Number 3

located on the medial aspect of the hemisphere); AP1826 stimulation did not induce seizures. Under AP18-26 stimulation AA responded correctly to only 27% (17/63) of the multiplication problems administered over several days. In contrast, he was 98% correct (246/250) on intermixed control trials without stimulation, x2 ( 1 , N = 313) = 191.2,p c .0001. In 27 of the 46 errors on stimulated trials AA produced an incorrect answer (eg., 8 x 4 = “forty”;6 x 5 = “twenty five”;7 x 9 = “eighty two”);on the remaining 19 trials he produced no response within the 7-sec stimulation period. On many of the trials in which he failed to respond correctly during stimulation, AA gave the correct response immediately after stimulation was terminated. A more carefully controlled comparison confirmed that performance on the multiplication task was impaired by stimulation at the AP18-26 site. For each of the 57 problems tested with and without stimulation (e.g., 7 x S),’ a mean accuracy was computed for stimulated trials and for nonstimulated control trials. AA’s accuracy was significantly lower for the stimulated trials (28% correct) than for the matched control trials (99% correct), t(56) = 11.67,p c .0001. On trials involving stimulation at sites other than AP18-26, AA was 94% correct (138/147).Even for electrode pairs adjacent to AP18-26, stimulation had no effect on multiplication performance: AA’s accuracy under stimulation at these sites was 98% (43/44). Results for stimulated and nonstimulated trials are summarized in Table 1 .

Identifying the Cognitive Process(es) Disrupted

by Stimulation Having established that stimulation at AP18-26 impaired performance on the multiplication task, we may next consider which of the cognitive processes required by the task were disrupted by stimulation. Several findings bear on this issue.

Figure 3. Comparison of the cognitive processes required by singledigit multiplication and addition tasks with arabic stimuli and spoken responses.

8x2

+

Arabic Numeral Comprehension

Table 1. AA’s Performance on Multiplication Problems with Spoken Responses

Stimulation Site

AP 18-26 All Other Sites

Adjacent to AP 18-26 None

Number of Problems

Number Correct

Percent Correct

63

17

27

147

138

94

44

43

98

250

246

98

Under AP18-26 stimulation AA performed without error on screening tests of spontaneous language production (6/6),object naming (8/8), reading aloud (8/8), and responding to commands (4/4). These results suggest that the stimulation-induced deficit on the multiplication task did not result from disruption of speech production or from impairment of some general cognitive function such as attention. Given that tasks such as object naming, reading aloud, and spontaneous speech (instruction: describe your high school and home state) required AA to recall from memory several different types of information, it also seems unlikely that AP18-26 stimulation produced some sort of general memory impairment. In addition to processes such as attention or speech production, the multiplication task implicates several forms of numerical processing: comprehension of arabic digits ( e g , 7 ,€9,retrieval of stored arithmetic facts (e.g, 7 x 8 = 56), and spoken production of numerals (e.g., “fifty-six”).To assess which of these processes were affected by stimulation, we compared AA’s performance on multiplication and addition tasks. In the addition task, as in the multiplication task, singledigit problems were presented in arabic form ( e g , 6 + 9), and AA said the answers aloud (e.g., ‘liftfifteen’’).Thus, both tasks required arabic numeral comprehension and spoken numeral production (Figure 3). However, the two tasks differed in arithmetic fact retrieval processes: the multiplication

Multiplication Fact Retrieval

Spoken Numeral Production

“sixteen“

Addition Numeral Comprehensio

Retrieval

Numeral Production

“sixteen”

Whalen et al.

413

task required retrieval of stored multiplication facts, whereas the addition task required retrieval of addition facts. If the stimulation-induced impairment of multiplication resulted solely from disruption of arabic numeral comprehension or spoken numeral production, we would expect performance to be equally impaired on the addition task. If, however, stimulation affected arithmetic fact retrieval, performance under stimulation might differ between the addition and multiplication tasks. Because addition facts may be better learned than, or stored separately from, multiplication facts (see, e.g., Dagenbach & McCloskey, 1992; Siegler & Engle, 1994), stimulation might well affect addition and multiplication fact retrieval to different extents. Under stimulation at the AP18-26 site AA’s performance was much more impaired for multiplication (17/63; 27%correct) than for addition (26/30; 87%correct), x2 (1, N = 313) = 29.12,p e .001. This finding suggests that the stimulation affected arithmetic fact retrieval and more specifically that the stimulation dis rupted retrieval of multiplication facts to a greater extent than retrieval of addition facts. However, before firm conclusions can be drawn on this point, we must ensure that the addition and multiplication tasks are adequately matched on their numeral comprehension and production requirements. Whereas saying the answer to a singledigit multiplication problem often requires spoken production of two number words (e.g., “fifty” and “six” for 7 x €9,all singledigit addition problems have one-word answers (e.g., ‘‘fifleen” for 7 + 8). The multiplication task may therefore place greater demands than the addition task on spoken numeral production processes and so might be more seriously affected by disruption of this process. The finding of greater multiplication than addition impairment under AP18-26 stimulation might, then, reflect disruption not of arithmetic fact retrieval, but rather of spoken numeral production. To evaluate this possibility we compared performance on singledigit multiplication and addition problems that have the same answer (e.g., 9 + 7 , 8 x 2) and therefore the same spoken numeral production requirements. For each of 13 answers shared between addition and multiplication problems, a mean accuracy was computed for multiplication and for addition. ’Ikenty-five addition trials and twenty-three multiplication trials contributed to the analysis. Under stimulation, AA’s performance was reliably worse for the multiplication problems (50%correct) than for the answer-matched addition problems (87%correct), t(12) = 2.7,p e .05.Therefore,even when the two tasks were equated on spoken numeral production requirements, stimulation at the AP18-26 site led to greater impairment for multiplication than for addition. This finding strongly suggests that the multiplication-addition dissociation under stimulation reflects an effect of stimulation on arithmetic fact retrieval. 41 4

Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience

However, we must also consider whether the dissociation could be due to differences between tasks in arabic numeral comprehension requirements. Here again, the answer-matched problems prove useful. For addition and multiplication problems with the same answer, the multiplication problems usually have the smaller operands (e.g., 3 x 6 versus 9 + 9);in our answer-matched problem sets the mean operand magnitude was 3.4 for multiplication and 4.3 for addition. Therefore, the multiplication problems were if anything less demanding than the addition problems in their arabic numeral comprehension requirements. Thus, relative to the addition problems the answermatched multiplication problems placed equal demands on spoken numeral production and equal or lesser demands on arabic numeral comprehension. The greater impairment of multiplication than addition under AP1826 stimulation therefore cannot be attributed to effects of stimulation on numeral comprehension or production processes. Hence, the result points strongly to the conclusion that stimulation disrupted arithmetic fact retrieval, with multiplication being affected to a greater degree than addition. Whether AP18-26 stimulation had any effect on retrieval of addition facts is uncertain. Addition performance was slightly worse on stimulated trials (87%)than on nonstimulated control trials (loo%),x2 (1, N = 75) = 3.97,p c .05. However, we cannot be certain that this result reflects stimulation-induced disruption of addition fact retrieval, because mild disruption of numeral comprehension or production could have been responsible instead. (Note that the addition-multiplication comparisons did not rule out the possibility that stimulation had some effect on numeral comprehension or production; rather, these comparisons established that the stimulation had effects on multiplication fact retrieval over and above any effects it might have had on numeral comprehension and production.) Why might AP18-26 stimulation have substantially dis rupted multiplication fact retrieval while affecting addition fact retrieval to a lesser extent or not at all? One possibility is that multiplication facts are typically not as well learned as addition facts and therefore are more susceptible to disruption. Another possibility is that addition and multiplication facts are localized to different brain areas, as some recent evidence from braindamaged patients suggests (e.g., Dagenbach & McCloskey, 1992; Siegler & Engle, 1994).

Stimulation-Induced Impairment on Other Multiplication Tasks Given the conclusion that AP18-26 stimulation impaired retrieval of multiplication table facts, we should expect to observe stimulation-induced impairment on any task requiring multiplication fact retrieval. In a verification task problems were presented with candidate answers Volume 9,Number 3

(e.g., 3 x 2 = 8), and AA indicated whether the answer was correct or incorrect by touching the appropriate word (‘Yes’’ or ‘‘no’’)on a card in front of him. He was uniformly correct on nonstimulated control trials (6/6) but performed at chance under stimulation at the AP1826 site (3/6). Although too few trials were administered to permit firm conclusions, the results are consistent with the conclusion that AP18-26 stimulation impaired multiplication fact retrieval. In another task singledigit multiplication problems were presented in arabic form,and AA typed the answers on a numerical keypad. He was 97%correct (68/70) on nonstimulated control trials but only 64% correct (14/22) on stimulated trials, x2 (1, N = 92) = 19.39,p c . 0 0 1 . Thus, even when AA was not required to produce spoken responses,M18-26 stimulation significantly impaired multiplication performance. Performance under stimulation was somewhat better in the keypad response task (64%correct) than in the task with spoken responses (27% correct). This result suggests that stimulation, in addition to disrupting multiplication fact retrieval, may have produced a mild impairment of spoken numeral production. This possibility, it should be emphasized, does not affect the arguments underpinning our conclusion that Stimulation disrupted multiplication fact retrieval. As discussed in the preceding section, the greater impairment for multiplication than for addition under stimulation cannot be attributed to effects of stimulation on numeral comprehension or production and points clearly to a stimulation-induced impairment of multiplication fact retrieval. Also, the results reported in the present section show that stimulation disrupted multiplication performance even in tasks not requiring spoken numeral production.

DISCUSSION Our principal findings may be summarized succinctly: Cortical stimulation at a single site in the left parietal region impaired patient AA’s performance on singledigit multiplication problems, and results from other tasks indicated that the stimulation specifically disrupted retrieval of stored multiplication facts. Accordingly, our findings suggest that the left parietal area is implicated in arithmetic fact retrieval. This conclusion must be drawn with some caution. In the first place, it is possible that AA’s functional brain organization is atypical in some respects,due to his brain tumor. Against this possibility it may be noted that the tumor was distant from the AP18-26 stimulation site and affected a brain area that has not been linked with arithmetic processing. Also, extensive testing of AA’s numerical processing prior to the cortical stimulation study revealed no abnormalities; arithmetic fact retrieval in particular appeared normal. Hence, it seems unlikely that arithmetic fact retrieval was localized atypically in AA; however, the possibility cannot be ruled out entirely.

A more general reason for caution is that the number of studies speaking to the localization of arithmetic fact retrieval remains very small. In addition to our results from AA, the available evidence consists primarily of Warrington’s (1982) study of patient DRC and HittmairDelazer et al.’s (1994) study of patient BE. Many more studies will have to be conducted before strong conclusions can be drawn. A considerable amount of evidence is needed in part because relationships between cognitive processes and brain areas may be quite complex. Multiple brain regions may be involved in implementing a cognitive process, and multiple cognitive processes may draw upon a given area of the brain. Also, there may be significant individual variation in localization of cognitive functions. Given these potential complexities, we can hope to clarlfy localization issues only by assessing the brain regions associated with a cognitive process in many individual subjects and preferably through a variety of methods. In light of these points it is interesting to compare our results from patient AA with the previous findings from patients DRC (Warrington, 1982) and BE (HittmairDelazer et al., 1994).At first glance the AA results appear to converge with those from DRC-who showed impairment in arithmetic fact retrieval following left parietooccipital damage-to suggest involvement of some dominant-hemisphere parietal region in arithmetic fact retrieval. However, the AP18-26 stimulation site is somewhat anterior to the locus of DRC’s structural lesion, and it is far from obvious that the brain area affected by stimulation in AA overlapped with the area damaged in DRC.2 The picture is complicated further by HittmairDelazer et al.’s (1994) patient BE; this patient’s lesion was apparently confined to the left basal ganglia, a region unlikely to have been affected in either AA or DRC. The apparent lack of consistency across studies may reflect the involvement of multiple brain areas in arithmetic fact retrieval or individual differences in localization of this cognitive process, or some other factor(@ may be responsible. Additional evidence will be needed to clarrfy the situation. Arithmetic fact retrieval is not at all atypical with respect to the complexities of localization issues, or the limited nature of the conclusions warranted by the available evidence.Notwithstanding the insights gained from traditional methods such as lesiondeficit correlation,and the development of powerful new techniques like cortical stimulation and fMRI, we are still in the early stages of understanding the functional organization of the brain.

METHOD Cortical Stimulation

Informed Consent This study was reviewed and approved by the Johns Hopkins Joint Committee on Clinical Investigation. FolWhalen et al.

415

lowing a full explanation of the procedures, AA and his parents gave for all research testing. The study guidelines by Lesser et al. (1987) for the safe use of the cortical stimulation technique.The effects of cortical stimulation and issues of safety have been discussed elsewhere (Gordon et al., 1990; Lesser et al., 1987; Nathan et al., 1993).

Cognitive Neuroscience. The study would not have been POSsible without AA's consistent willingness to work through evenings and weekends, and the help of Barbara Cysvk, Dr. John Hart, Erica palmer, Sarah Reusing, and Pamela Schwerdt.

Procedure

Note

Subdural arrays of electrodes were surgically placed on the surface of the cortex. The electrodes were 2.3-mm disc-, placed 10 mm from center to center, in arrays externaUy connected through a multiminicable system. As FiguR illustrates, arrays were placed in the posterior frontal, anterior Parietal, and medial Parietal regions, oriented approximately parallel to the sylvian fissure. A 2 x 8 a m v was also Placed in the inferior frontal region orient& approxim~telyperpendicular to the previous three arrays. Four other 2 x 5 arrays (not shown in Figure 2) were placed in the anterior frontal, medial frontal, anterior intercerebral,and posterior intercerebral areas. Electrode positions were determined independently by direct visual inspection at the time of operative placement and removal, as well as by CT-scan and plain skull x-rays. Electrical stimulation was administered with a Grass Model 5 12 stimulator at an intensity of 15 mA. Stimulation was applied between adjacent electrodes, in 300msec square wave pulses of alternating polarity at a frequency of 50 Hz. For each electrode pair, AA was first examined during stimulation for signs of motor or sensory impairment. He then received screening tests probing object naming, word reading, responding to commands, auditory comprehension, spontaneous language production, and singledigit multiplication. Stimulation trials were intermixed with nonstimulated control trials. AA was not informed of which trials would involve stimulation and was typically unaware when stimulation was applied. On stimulated trials,the onset of stimulation preceded the presentation of the test item by approximately 1 sec; stimulation continued until AA responded or the end of the response period was reached. Stimulation at certain sites on the posterior and inferior frontal strips (Figure 2) elicited sensations and/or movement of the right leg, arm, hand, fingers,mouth, or tongue (see Urasaki, Uematsu, Gordon, & Lesser, 1994). At one inferior frontal site, stimulation produced auditory hallucinations and disrupted auditory comprehension. Stimulation was not found to impair language comprehension or production at any other site.

1 . Six problems were tested twice during stimulation; 57 (63 - 6) unique problems were presented under stimulation. 2. Although the effects of electrical stimulation on brain activity are far from fully understood, the best available evidence

Acknowledgments

Reprint requests should be sent to Michael McCloskey,Department of Cognitive Science, The Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD 21218, or via e-mail to: michael.mccloskey@ jhu.edu.

suggests that at the intensity used in the present study the effects are localized to the area immediately surrounding the electrodes (Nathan, Sinha, Gordon, Lesser, & Thakor, 1993). Similarly, although the boundaries of vascular lesions may be difficult to establish with certainty, it does not appear likely that DRC's lesion extended as far forward as the anterior parietal region stimulated in AA.

REFERENCES Ashcraft, M. H. (1982). The development of mental arithmetic: A chronometric approach. Developmental Review, 2, 2 13-236.

Ashcraft, M. H. (1992). Cognitive arithmetic: A review of data and theory. Cognition, 44, 75-106. Benson, D. E , & Weir, W E (1972). Acalculia: Acquired anarithmetia. Cortex, 8, 465-472. Cohn, R. (1961). Dyscalculia. Archives of Neurolou, 4, 301-

307. Corbett, A. J., McCusker, E. A., & Davidson, 0.R. (1986). Acalculia following a dominant-hemisphere subcortical infarct. Archives of Neurolou, 43, 964-966. Dagenbach, D., & McCloskey, M. (1992). The organization of arithmetic facts in memory: Evidence from a braindamaged patient. Brain and Cognition, 20, 345-366. Dehaene, S., & Cohen, L. (1991). l k o mental calculation systems: A case study of severe acalculia with preserved approximation. Neuropsycbologia, 29, 1045- 1074. Dehaene, S., & Cohen, L. (1995). Towards a functional and anatomical theory of numerical processing. Mathematical Cognition, I, 83-120. De Renzi, E., & Vignolo, L. A. (1962). The Token Test: A sensitive test to detect receptive disturbances in aphasics. Brain, 85, 665-678. Goodglass, H., & Kaplan, E. (1983). Boston naming test. 2nd ed. Philadelphia: Lea and Febiger. Gordon, B., Lesser, R. I?, Rance, N. E., Hart, J., Jr., Webber, R., Uematsu, S., & Fisher, R. S. (1990). Parameters for direct cortical electrical stimulation in the human: Histopathologic confirmation. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neumpbysiolou, 75, 37 1-377. Grafman, J., Kampen, D., Rosenberg, J., Salazar, A,, & Boller, E (1989). Calculation abilities in a patient with a virtual left heispherectomy. Behavioural Neurolou, 2, 183- 194. Grafman, J., Passafiume, D., Faglioni, I?, & Boller, E (1982). Calculation disturbances in adults with focal hemispheric damage. Cortex, 18, 37-50. Hecaen. H.. Angelergues. R., & Houillier. S. (1961). Les varietes cliniques des acalculies au cours de lesions retro rolandiques: Approche statistique du probleme. Revue Neurologique, 105, 85-103. .

This study was supported by NIH grants NS2 1047 and NS26553 to the Johns Hopkins University and by the Seaver Foundation, the Whittier Foundation, and the McDonnel Pew Program in

416

Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience

.

-

I

.

Volume 9,Number 3

Hittmair-Delazer,M., Semenza, C., & Denes, G. (1994). Concepts and facts in calculation. Brain, I 17, 715-728. Inouye, T., Shinosaki,K., Iyama, A., & Matsumoto, Y. (1993). Localization of activated areas and directional EEG patterns during mental arithmetic.Electmencepbalograpby and Clinical Neumpbysiology, 86, 224-230. Jackson, M., & Warrington,E. K. (1986). Arithmetic skills in patients with unilateral cerebral lesions. Cortex, 22, 61 1-620. Jastak, S., & Wilkinson, G. S. (1984). Wide Range Acbievement Test-Revised. Wilmington, DE:Jastak Associates. Kahn, H. J., & Whitaker, H. A. (1991). Acalculia: An historical review of localization.Brain and Cognition, 17, 102-115. Lesser, R. E, Luders, H., Klem, G., Dinner, D. S., Morris, H. H., Hahn,J. E,& Wyllie, E. (1987). Extraoperative cortical functional localization in patients with epilepsy.Journal of Clinical Neumpbysiology, 4, 27-53. Luchelli, E,& De Renzi, E. (1993). Primary dyscalculia after a medial frontal lesion of the left hemisphere.Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgq, and Psycbiatly, 56, 304-307. McCloskey, M.(1992). Cognitive mechanisms in numerical processing: Evidence from acquired dyscalculia. Cognition, 44, 107-157. McCloskey, M., Aliminosa, D., & Macaruso, I? (1991). Theorybased assessment of acquired dyscalculia. Brain and Cognition, 17, 285-308. McCloskey, M., Aliminosa, D., & Sokol, S. M. (1991). Facts, rules, and procedures in normal calculation: Evidence from multiple single-patient studies of impaired arithmetic fact retrieval.Brain and Cognition, 17, 154-203. McCloskey, M., Caramazza, A,, & Basili, A. (1985). Cognitive mechanisms in number processing and calculation: Evidence from dyscalculia. Brain and Cognition, 4, 171-196. McCloskey, M., Harley, W., & Sokol, S. M. (1991). Models of arithmetic fact retrieval: An evaluation in light of findings from normal and braindamaged subjects.Journal of Experimental Psychology:Learning, Memoly, €7 Cognition, 17, 377-397. Morris, H. H., Luders, H., Lesser, R., Dinner, D., & Hahn,J. (1984). Transient neuropsychological abnormalities (includ-

ing Gerstmann's syndrome) during cortical stimulation. Neurology, 34, 877-883. Nathan, S. S., Sinha, S. R., Gordon, B., Lesser, R. E, & Thakor, N. V. (1993). Determination of current density distributions generated by electrical stimulation of the human cerebral cortex. Electmencepbalograpby and Clinical Neuropbysiology, 86, 183-192. Ojemann, G. (1974). Mental arithmetic during human thalamic stimulation.Neuropsychologia, 12, 1- 10. Roland, E E., & Friberg, L. (1985). Localization of cortical areas activated by thinking.Journal of Neumpkysiology, 5, 1219- 1243. Rosselli, M., & Ardila, A. (1989). Calculation deficits in patients with right and left hemisphere damage. Neumpsycbologia, 27, 607-617. Siegler, R. S. (1988). Strategy choice procedures and the development of multiplication skill.Journal of Experimental Psychology:General, 11 7, 258-275. Siegler, R. S., & Engle, R. A. (1994). Studying change in developmental and neuropsychological contexts. Cabiers de Psychologie Cognitive, 13, 321-349. Sokol, S. M.,McCloskey, M., Cohen, N. J., & Aliminosa, D. (199 1). Cognitive representations and processes in arithmetic: Inferences from the performance of braindamaged s u b jects.Journal of Expm'mental Pycbology:Learning, Memory and Cognition, 17, 355-376. Takayama, Y., Sugishita,M., Akiguchi,I., & Kimura,J. (1994). Isolated acalculia due to left parietal lesion. Archives of Neurology, 51, 286-291. Urasaki, E., Uematsu, S., Gordon, B., & Lesser, R. (1994). Cortical tongue area studied by chronically implanted subdural electrodes - with special reference to parietal motor and frontal sensory responses. Brain, I 1 7, 117-132. Warrington, E. K. (1982). The fractionation of arithmetical skills: A single case study. Quarterly Journal of Ex-rimental Psycbology, 3 4 , 3 1-5 1. Wechsler, D. (1974). Wecbsler Intelligence Scale for Cbildren-Revised. San Antonio,TX:The Psychological Corporation.

Whalen et al.

41 7