Princeton University
Media and Young Children's Learning Author(s): Heather L. Kirkorian, Ellen A. Wartella, Daniel R. Anderson Source: The Future of Children, Vol. 18, No. 1, Children and Electronic Media (Spring, 2008), pp. 39-61 Published by: Princeton University Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/20053119 Accessed: 12/09/2009 13:34 Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use, available at http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp. JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use provides, in part, that unless you have obtained prior permission, you may not download an entire issue of a journal or multiple copies of articles, and you may use content in the JSTOR archive only for your personal, non-commercial use. Please contact the publisher regarding any further use of this work. Publisher contact information may be obtained at http://www.jstor.org/action/showPublisher?publisherCode=princetonu. Each copy of any part of a JSTOR transmission must contain the same copyright notice that appears on the screen or printed page of such transmission. JSTOR is a not-for-profit organization founded in 1995 to build trusted digital archives for scholarship. We work with the scholarly community to preserve their work and the materials they rely upon, and to build a common research platform that promotes the discovery and use of these resources. For more information about JSTOR, please contact
[email protected].
Princeton University is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to The Future of Children.
http://www.jstor.org
Media
and Young L. Kirkorian,
Heather
Children's
Ellen A. Wartella,
Learning R. Anderson
and Daniel
Summary on media, particularly television, have long been criticized for their potential impact One area for concern is how media influences cognitive development exposure early summa and academic achievement. Heather Kirkorian, Ellen Wartella, and Daniel Anderson
Electronic children.
rize the relevant and minimizing
research
and provide the negative effects.
suggestions
for maximizing
the positive
effects
of media
is the seemingly unique effect of television on children under age two. that well-designed, televi educational Although clearly demonstrates age-appropriate, sion can be beneficial to children of age, studies on infants and toddlers suggest that preschool
One
focus of the authors research
these young children may better understand and learn from real-life experiences some research from video. Moreover, suggests that exposure to television during years of life may be associated
with
poorer
cognitive
than they do the first few
development.
the importance of content in mediat respect to children over two, the authors emphasize on to age ing the effect of television cognitive skills and academic achievement. Early exposure an is around educational curriculum associated with appropriate programs designed cognitive
With
and academic particular,
enhancement,
is associated
with
whereas poorer
exposure cognitive
to pure entertainment, and violent content in lower and academic achievement. development
authors point out that producers and parents can take steps to maximize the positive effects and minimize the negative effects. They note that research on children's television can inform for producers of children's media to enhance viewing guidelines learning. Parents
The
of media
can select well-designed, to maximize the positive
programs and view age-appropriate effects of educational media.
the programs
with
their children
aim is to inform educators, parents, and others who work with policymakers, children the about of media, particularly children, and television, on preschool young impact can to costs. maximize what society do the benefits and minimize the The
authors'
www.futureofchildren.org research associate at the University of Massachusetts-Amherst. Ellen Wartella is a professor, Heather Kirkorian is a postdoctoral Daniel Anderson is a professor at the University of executive vice chancellor, and provost at the University of California-Riverside. Massachusetts-Amherst. VOL. 18
/ NO. 1 / SPRING
2008
39
Heather L. Kirkorian, Ellen A. WarteUa, and Daniel R Anderson
in first appeared rooms in the the nation's living middle of the twentieth century,
Since
television
recurrent
have voiced
observers
its impact on view children. In recent years, ers, particularly to other electronic concern has extended this concern
over
including computers researchers game consoles. Although still have much to learn, they have provided electronic information on the links between
video
television, and children's especially is skills. The message learning and cognitive clear: most (if not all) media effects must be
media,
in media content. With light of to what children watch respect development, is at least as important as, and probably more watch. important than, how much they
considered
Until the 1980s, social
close academic subsequent for both media with some suggestions and and parents for enhancing producers effects of beneficial the potentially extending electronic
watched
In this article we
television.
research with
review media
on
skills and academic
cognitive in young children. We
over whether
children
younger to electronic
than two should be exposed the apparent media, emphasizing of infants and toddlers
deficit
learn better
video
inwhich
from real-life
they than
experiences Next we look at research from video. do they on media effects in three areas: associations between
media
use and cognitive
attention; experimental particularly for direct learning from educational 40
particularly
age.
THE FUTURE OF CHILDREN
as Active
Children Until
the 1980s,
Users
Media
social science
researchers
of how viewers had only implicit theory television. Analysts regarded televi watched sion viewing, particularly by young children, an
and under the being cognitively passive features control of salient attention-eliciting
as
such as fast movement
of the medium sound effects.
Jerome
and
Singer formalized that the "busyness"
of this theory, proposing leads to a sensory bombardment television a series of orienting responses that produces that interferes with cognition and reflection. cannot process television and therefore cannot learn from it.1
content Others
begin by children are active arguing that by age three, media users. We then discuss important that highlight aspects of child development the debate
are of preschool
those who
Aletha
emphasis achievement
in children,
similar views, arguing that proposed as Sesame Street such provided programs educational.2 be could that truly nothing
an implicit theory of how
an
use
media
As a result, children
had only
researchers
viewers
use and
and
screen media,
science
early media achievement. We
between
and associations
Huston
a somewhat
and John Wright proposed different theory of attention
positing that the features of television that drive children's attention may as a child ages. they Specifically, change salient claimed that in infancy, perceptually and features of television such as movement to television,
sound effects
age and are less influ
drive attention. With
however, experience, enced by perceptual pay greater attention such as dialogue
children salience
and are able to
to informative
features
and narrative.3
evidence
and the same time, Daniel Anderson a Elizabeth Lorch created complementary model of children's attention to television,
media;
drawing
skills,
Around
on evidence
that television
viewing
is
Media and Young Children's Learning
Table 1. Selected
Popular Television
Programs
for Young Children
and DVD Series
Network
TV programs
Description
Barney & Friends
Evoking a preschool setting, Barney the dinosaur teaches songs and dances to young children. The show focuses heavily on pro-social themes of sharing, empathizing, helping others, and cooperating.
PBS
Blue's Clues
viewers at home to help solve a mystery with his dog friend, A human host encourages Blue. The show is often repetitive and encourages interactivity by asking viewers to find clues and solve puzzles.
Nickelodeon
Bob the Builder
Bob the Builder and his construction crew face building, renovation, and repair chailenges. The series often focuses on identifying a problem and making a plan to solve the problem.
PBS
Dora the Explorer
Featuring a bilingual Latina girl as the lead, Dora and her friends go on quests and help others, encouraging viewers to help out through their own actions or by telling her what she needs to know. Inaddition to highlighting traditional educational content such as in English and color and shapes, Dora teaches language by repeating words and phrases Spanish.
PBS
Sesame
Combining puppetry, wide range of topics lution, music, dance,
Street
live action, and animation, this long-running series focuses on a conflict reso including the alphabet, numbers, emotion management, and healthy lifestyles.
Teletubbies
Centering on four colorful characters, the Teletubbies speak in a baby-like language and in their stomachs that show clips of learn through play. The Teletubbies have televisions real children from around the world. This program is targeted at toddlers.
Thomas & Friends
Based on a book series, Thomas the Tank Engine and his engine hard and be cooperative with each other.
The Wiggles
include songs Featuring a four-man singing group for children, episodes of The Wiggles children to sing songs and skits focused on solving a problem. The Wiggles encourages and move their bodies to music.
DVD series
Description
Baby Einstein
Series content covers wide range of topics including music, science. Targeted at children starting at one month.
Brainy Baby
Educational
Beginnings
series
PBS
Disney
art, language, poetry, and
Disney
highlighting range of subjects including alphabet, art, music, shapes, and right and left brain development. Targeted at children starting at
Features baby versions of the Muppets from Sesame Street. The focus is on encouraging interactions between child and caregivers. Targeted at children starting at six months.
on active cognition. They argued that in children at least as young as two attention content. in is large part by program guided children pay more For example, preschool based
attention
learn to work
PBS
Producer
foreign languages, nine months. Sesame
friends
PBS
to those clips than to make them incom
Brainy Baby Company
Sesame
Workshop
tion.6 Finally, to understand typical programs such as cuts, that use standard video montage in a variety pans, and zooms, children engage activities while viewing.7 of inferential
to normal video
that have been
edited
for example by using foreign prehensible, the dubs of the video clips or randomizing order of shots within the clips.4 Moreover, children pay more attention to preschool-age even than to commercials children's programs are more densely packed though commercials learn strate with formal features.5 Children television by using their gies for watching of formal features to guide atten knowledge
Considerations
Developmental Although television
children
are active viewers
of
age, research suggests by preschool that this may not be true of infants and tod research dlers. In this section we summarize to, comprehension of, and learn two. ing from video by children under
on attention
Attention Until
to Electronic
recently,
research VOL.
18
Media on media
/ NO.
effects
1 / SPRING
2008
41
Heather L. Kirkorian, Ellen A. WarteUa, and Daniel R. Anderson
did not focus on infants and toddlers. studies two
that children
reported paid little attention
Early younger than
to television, perhaps little television was produced for them.8 The early 1990s, however, saw a virtual in the of television explosion production
because
Research
that
suggests
children do not comprehend
media
over whether
years.
pay close attention increase in infant
products has led to debate infants and toddlers should be
(See table 1 for exposed to electronic media. a some of description popular media products for young children.) the underlying mechanisms driving to video appear to be the same in adults and infants as young as three months, some research has found differences in the
Although attention
ways in which younger and older viewers watch video.10 For produced professionally one observed example, patterns of eye study movements
in
one-year-olds,
four-year-olds,
and adults while and found
Sesame Street they watched between systematic differences
infants and older viewers.
Infants' visual
and twenty-four months watched eighteen, normal and distorted segments o? Teletubbies, a viewers in this age for program designed THE FUTURE OF CHILDREN
speech. older
and twenty-four months) looked for longer periods at the normal video segment than at the distorted segments, children and twelve did (six months) younger not appear to discriminate between the two. These findings suggest that children under and eighteen months may not understand, in the same way
television
children.
as
In
to
of Video Perception One area of cognitive influenc development to learn children's from television ing ability
is
the perception of video itself. Some research suggests that children do not begin to discrim inate between
television
and real-life
events
until
the early preschool years. For example, Jaglom and Howard Gardner reported of three children from qualitative observations
Leona
age two to five. They noted that at age two, the children that the television recognized world was
contained
within
the television
set
but not until
or four they reached age three did they realize that the television world could not affect them?that, for example, television characters The
authors
could not enter concluded
their bedrooms.
that sometime
be
tween ages two and three, children develop of the representational understanding
an
nature
of video.13
fixa
tions, for example, were more variable and less sensitive to in content.11 In an changes other experiment, children aged six, twelve,
42
experiment children (eighteen
produce backwards found that although
particular, they may be dialogue and may fail to inte across successive shots grate comprehension in filmic montage.
toddlers, that infants and toddlers
directed
to
The
inattentive
for infants and designed and some research now suggests
to these videos.9 The
reversed
do older
and videos
programs
were
thus learn from,
the symbolic nature of television until they reach the preschool
one distorted video, shots were range.12 In in the other, utterances randomly ordered;
In a similar vein,
John Flavell and several a series of conducted colleagues experiments with children to investigate the preschool-age distinction real objects they made between on video. and those represented Younger children were less likely to answer correctly
Media and Young Children's Learning
uses of on regarding the objects For example, three- and four-year old children saw a video image of a bowl of
learn from commercially years and younger television programs. Experiments produced on found learning from video have repeatedly
popcorn and were asked fall out of the bowl when
that infants and toddlers
questions television.
if the popcorn the television
would set
was
turned upside down. The four-year-olds that televised recognized images represent real objects while three-year-olds failed to
learn better
from
real-life
than from video. This experiences video deficit disappears by about age three, when learning from video becomes so-called
robust.18
discriminate
between televised images and real objects, claiming that the popcorn would fall out of the bowl if the television was down.14
turned upside Other
research
to discriminate
focusing between
Support for the video deficit hypothesis comes from several lines of research. Studies of language learning have demonstrated that children aged two and older can learn vocab
on children's
ability
televised
programs has generally demonstrated younger than five cannot con
ulary from television.19 Unlike infants and toddlers however,
and commercials
learn from video. One
that children
children
sistently make that distinction.15 Even when young children correctly label programs and commercials, they may still think that the is part of or connected
commercial
to the
program.16 Moreover, although children may be able to identify commercials based on cues perceptual by age five, their ability to intent and inherent recognize the persuasive bias in advertising does not appear to develop until age seven or eight.17 this research
suggests that children nature the comprehend symbolic of television until they reach the preschool
Together do not
and learn years; evidence of comprehending at from television younger ages than ing ismeager. And itmay about two-and-a-half take several more
years before children are able to make more specific discriminations with
respect
to program
from Electronic Media Learning infant-directed media Many products explicit claims about their educational
make
value; titles such as Baby Einstein, keep their claims implicit. But analysts know little about the extent to which children two others, with
younger than two learned vocabu than lary better from real-life experiences from equivalent video presentations.20 Other
research demonstrates experimental are less effective television models ones
in preserving
phonemes Additional
that than live
of foreign in infants.21
discrimination sounds)
(speech
support for the video deficit comes from studies examining
hypothesis infants' and toddlers'
to imitate ability specific as an such adult actions actions, demonstrating with a puppet. In an experiment comparing toddlers' imitation of live and mediated (that is, videotaped)
models,
Rachel
Barr and
Harlene
Hayne reported that twelve-, fifteen-, were more to and eighteen-month-olds likely a behavior after viewing unmediated, perform live models than after viewing either the video
model
or no model.
the oldest
age group likely perform the behavior after seeing the video model than the control group after seeing no modeled behavior.22 A more
was more
content.
older children, are less to likely found that experiment
Only
to
recent experiment made similar findings for at children twenty-four and thirty months.23 It is clear that, unlike preschool-age behaviors
seen
infants and toddlers, children can readily imitate on
video.24
VOL. 18
/ NO.
1 / SPRING
2008
43
Heather L. Kirkorian, Ellen A. Wartella, and Daniel R. Anderson
Another
line of research
and toddlers'
ability
relevant
to infants'
to transfer from video
it can be overcome though an interactive relationship. Researchers by have not yet demonstrated any learning, or even
toddlers
to
involves object-retrieval real-world problems the child either tasks. In these experiments, room sees a toy hidden in an through adjacent or watches awindow the toy being hidden on television. In a study of children aged two Georgine Troseth and reported that both age groups Judy DeLoache were able to find the toy on every trial when and two-and-a-half,
One baby videos. a series effect of the evaluated study to foster of baby videos designed parent-child interactions. Compared with parents who a series (Baby Einstein), watched comparison lack of it, from commercial
recent
videos from the Sesame parents who watched more series showed engaged Beginnings interactions with their twelve- to twenty-one
the hiding event was seen through a window but less often when the event was watched
month-old
on television,
videos
the younger particularly for An Kelly Schmitt and Daniel
participants.25 derson reported
similar findings with overall at chance levels (25 percent) for
performance children aged two and about 50 percent for in the television children aged two-and-a-half
task but nearly perfect at both ages for the did well on both window task. Three-year-olds tasks.26 Marie
Schmidt, Alisha Crawley-Davis, to minimize and Daniel Anderson attempted cues and simplify the influence of perceptual In the first, a the task in two experiments. a cutout on was sticker hidden underneath a felt-board
that had the same dimensions
as the television
screen.
In the second,
an
experimenter simply told the child, either live or on closed-circuit television, where the was hidden. Performance of two-year object olds in both tasks was still at chance levels in the television conditions.27 Georgine Troseth attributed this deficit to and Judy DeLoache a poor of symbolic representa understanding tions or to prior expectations about television as "unreal." Recent
work by Troseth shows that if toddlers have interactive experiences
for example, they con television?if, verse with an via closed-circuit experimenter video?the video deficit in the object-retrieval can task be overcome.28
the if they had coviewed on occasions.29 Al multiple
children
at home
is as yet no evidence that babies though there learn anything from baby videos, apparently can.
parents
coviewing
no research To our knowledge ined computer and interactive
has yet exam game use in
although these products for children as developed
infants and toddlers, are now young
being as six months
of age and some parents and toddlers use infants that their
report these media
on a recent sur regularly. Based Kaiser vey of parents, the Family Foundation that 61 percent of children under estimated (television, videos, age two use screen media on a 43 percent of and DVDs) typical day infants and toddlers watch television every re a relative dearth of empirical day.30 Given search on infants and toddlers and a dispute screen even over whether they comprehend media, will
for the remainder
focus on educational
preschoolers
of this article we media
and older children.
urgently needed, influence media
however,
for designed is Research
to determine
how
infants and toddlers.
with
the bulk of the research supports Overall, a video deficit for learning by infants and 44
THE FUTURE OF CHILDREN
Media
Effects
on Attention
and
Other Cognitive Skills their other charges, critics have Among often accused television of being a negative influence on the development of children's cognitive
skills. Much
of the debate
about
the
Media and Young Children's Learning
effect
concerns the cognition of attention. The most common
of television
development
on
has been that frequent changes in hypothesis scenes and content disrupt young children's to sustain attention.31 One ability reanalysis of 1980s collected data longitudinal during the found a small correlation between early at ages one and three years and subsequent symptoms of attention at age seven.32 from stud problems Findings ies since then have been mixed.33 television
exposure
sures of whereas those who self-regulation, showed higher viewed the prosocial programs levels of task persistence, rule obedience, and of delay relative to baseline mea sures and to children in the neutral viewing
tolerance
It is important to note that the three categories of programs likely differed not in content but with respect to formal only condition.
factor in the link be possible mediating tween early television viewing and attention skills is program content. Most correlational studies do not measure to which impossible
content
the types of programs it exposed, making to draw any conclusions regarding
children
effects.
are
a recent
However,
(animation
versus
live-action) and pace. It is difficult within the context of this to isolate the links be study tween
One
such as format
features
content
and self-regulatory skills, but as clearly indicate that television
the findings a medium does not have an indiscriminate
on attentional skills. In fact, negative effect have found that televi several experiments sion can teach specific attention skills and strategies.36
corre
that content is an study suggested of the relation between important mediator lational
exposure subsequent
to television attentional
early exposure entertainment associated
before
to violent
age three and
problems. Specifically, and non-educational
was programming positively with later symptoms of attention
television but exposure to educational was not related to attentional problems.34 deficit
television
Educational
those designed programs, around a curriculum with to communicate specific goal academic or social skills, teach their intended
early study of the effects of television on behavior in experimentally preschoolers varied the type of content children viewed.
a
lessons.
One
who were
The
study compared preschoolers to (Mister Rog exposed prosocial programs ers' Neighborhood), neutral films, and violent cartoons
Superman).35 Children were observed first for a baseline period of then for a four-week television three weeks, two weeks viewing period, and finally for from this after the viewing period. Findings television study suggest that the link between (Batman,
skills isme viewing and children's attentional diated by content. Children who viewed the in mea violent cartoons showed decreases
Many allegations regarding the effect of on children's attention skills focus television on the fast pace of programs such as Sesame Street.37 The only study to experimentally vary the pace of a television program ob served preschoolers during tasks of perse verance after the children either viewed an edited
version
of Sesame
of either particularly particularly books with differences
Street,
composed
segments or read segments,
fast-paced
slow-paced
or
found no group or distractibility either reading or
parents. Analysts in measures of
impulsiveness
following VOL. 18
/ NO.
1 / SPRING
2008
45
Heather L. Kirkorian, Ellen A. Wartella, and Daniel R Anderson
television
viewing.38 This finding suggests that is no immediate link between program
there
and attentional
pacing
skills. Nonetheless,
from Educational
Learning Educational
around
most
tion in
educational
preschoolers.
of computer use and video games have been more optimistic, with the relevant research seeming to support a link between both and cognition. The research generally
Discussions
focuses
on
tion. One
skills other
cognitive study, for instance,
experiment the effects
conducted
no on differences study found between-group of spatial skill, it found pre-test measures scores for the significantly higher post-test spatial video game group than for the control
media
results have been
reported
the research might
suggests that electronic have an effect on attention skills.
especially when viewed by children than younger age two, may have a negative on attention effect development, though the is over evidence weak. Concern relatively television exposure before age two has been
Television,
echoed
in research
on
cognitive development more Content appears to be an generally.41 and specific television important mediator, content
has been
Studies
of interactive media
linked to attention
skills.
have found
that
video game play may enhance spatial cogni tion, but research is lacking on other cognitive skills, particularly attention development. 46
THE FUTURE OF CHILDREN
or social skills,
lessons. But because
research
of assessing the effectiveness curricula is proprietary or not
in archival
published evaluations
sources, most
program the by general public. of this research
go unseen reviews Nevertheless, demonstrate
the effectiveness,
term and long-term,
both
short
of curriculum-based
an
fifth graders to investigate on of video game experience
were spatial skills in children. Subjects an group randomly assigned to experimental that played a spatial game, such as navigating a marble or a along tracks through space, a control group that played computerized word game that was not the spatial. Although
Overall,
academic
than atten
with
group.39 Similar by others.40
those programs, a curriculum with a specific
designed to communicate goal teach their intended
research manipulating program longitudinal content is needed to investi experimentally on atten the causal effect of television gate
Media
television
Preschoolers
view
who
Sesame
Street have higher levels of school readiness than those who do not.
for children in areas as diverse programming as science, and social literacy, mathematics, skills.42 Academics have also published research
evaluating
the effectiveness
of
educational programs. We present examples of both correlational and experimental evaluative studies.
is a television program focusing on social and skills cognitive problem-solving a in In two-year program preschoolers. evaluation, Jennings Bryant and others
Blue's Clues
who were regular preschoolers of the show and preschoolers who not because the program did not air in
followed viewers were
their town of residence. children
The
two groups of of prob
did not differ on measures
lem solving and flexible at the start of thinking the study. At the end of the two-year observa tion period, however, regular viewers of Blue's Clues outperformed their non-viewing peers in
many
measures
and systematic
and were
more
in their problem
successful
solutions.
Media and Young Children's Learning
the problems required careful a trait and planning, frequently modeled in the an described In program.43 experimen tal study, were ran children preschool-age to watch one domly assigned episode o? Blue's Solving
or the same
Clues,
showed better
of the
comprehension in the show, specific information presented and children who watched the program five times showed better than comprehension saw it
those who Clues on
once. Moreover, Blue's only scored higher than non-viewers
viewers
tasks different
problem-solving
tions, such as increases
from those
in the program, particularly the program repeatedly44 these studies demonstrate immedi
directly presented when they viewed
ment,
also are plausible.47
Other
forms of electronic
been
used
ated with
Street or Dora domain
research
these websites.
entering school. One such program is Sesame Street, which has been by far the most studied children's program, probably because of s commitment to research, Workshop the program's longevity and popularity, and its long history of both criticism and praise.
Sesame
research
demonstrates
between
early exposure Street and school readiness.45 That control
alysts statistically factors known to affect
a
positive to Sesame
is, after an for a range of other school readiness, they
find that preschoolers who view Sesame Street have higher levels of school readiness than those who evidence
do not. Nationally, there is some for an increase in school readiness
among preschoolers
in recent years.46 One for this trend may be
plausible explanation increased early exposure to television, particu larly educational programs for young children,
are associ
shows such as Sesame
television
the Explorer, is available
no though public on the effect of
Researchers
have conducted
on the use of educational
studies
For
home.
software
one experiment example, in the effectiveness gains
significant cational software when
To summarize,
programs designed for young focus on a variety of academic and social skills to help prepare children for
children
some
instance,
produced, professionally Internet websites for preschoolers
Some
television
For
also have
curriculum-based
by other
association
enroll
preschool
media
for education.
Together ate and potentially long-lasting effects of Blue's Clues on skills, problem-solving for viewers the of especially program. regular
Correlational
in
or one
five times,
episode a different of episode program. Not surpris children who viewed the Blue's Clues ingly,
program
as yet evidence is insufficient to draw though solid conclusions. Although media may have to the trend, many other contributed explana
to use the software
reported of edu
children were as well
at home
school.48 Similar benefits
at
allowed
as in
have been
reported
researchers.49
it is clear that children
learn from educational
media.
can
Television
pro grams designed with a specific goal to teach academic or social skills can be effective with potentially long-lasting effects. Although scarce, research on interactive media soft ware
suggests similar results. We turn now to a discussion of associations between overall
media
use
measures
in
and subsequent early childhood of overall achievement.
Early Media Use and Academic Achievement common criticisms of chil Among the most use ac is that it dren's media displaces other tivities believed to be more beneficial such as outdoor
and leisure play, homework, reading. television however, viewing has Historically, media largely displaced other entertainment such as comic books, radio, and cinema.50 For the most appear
part, television more to displace VOL. 18
viewing
does not
educationally
/ NO.
1 / SPRING
valu 2008
47
Heather L. Kirkorian, Ellen A. Wartella, and Daniel R. Anderson
case of except perhaps in the and youth with extraordinarily high
able activities, children television children
exposure
or of early school-age in first and to read, typically
learning grade.51 Potential
effects displacement interactive media are less
second
of relatively new, clear because users can access multiple media a computer, using simultaneously, platforms television.52 for example, while watching studies of the effect of television
Many on
achievement
academic
tions between and
exposure
examine
some measure some
of achievement.53
a
rates of television viewing were associ higher achievement.55 Other ated with decreasing a similar pattern.56 have found studies in the association important factor television viewing and academic between
measure
studies can findings of correlational be difficult to interpret. Itmay be that televi sion viewing lowers academic achievement,
Moreover,
but it is equally plausible that academically children are more drawn to televi challenged sion as a leisure-time
activity. Moreover, that has not been accounted
may be the age of the viewer. amount of television exposure
achievement The
correla
tions are often negative, indicating greater achievement with lower exposure to television, but the associations are also often quite small.
third variable
was positively day), television viewing but associated with academic achievement,
hours
One
of television
contemporaneous
(one to two
is, in moderation
That
curvilinear.
viewing
correla
In these studies,
total viewing time age correlation between a was and academic achievement only -.05, More accurately described, tiny association. the relation was what social scientists call
some
and explain both television exposure In the case of television view achievement.
optimal as a function of may vary with age, possibly the types of programs viewed at different Few studies have directly investigated ages.57 the association television
between
viewing
achievement
and
in infants and toddlers.
In one
study, however, viewing before age related to later academic three was negatively whereas achievement viewing at three years related to subse and beyond was positively quent
achievement.58
for may
ing, for example, children from lower-income homes tend to watch more television and also to score lower on measures
of academic
than do their higher-income In this example, both televi counterparts.54 sion exposure and academic achievement may
achievement
be the result of family income. In fact, when studies take into account other correlational to find important factors, they often fail signifi cant associations between television exposure in children. and academic achievement
Detailed
analyses
of the relation
between
exposure and academic achieve suggest that this relation is not straight
television ment
For example, a meta-analysis of aver the that studies twenty-three reported
forward.
48
THE FUTURE OF CHILDREN
It is also important studies mentioned
to note
that most
of the
thus far did not distin
the types of content viewed. guish between a association of The lack straightforward television exposure and academic between may be at least partially medi ated by the content of the programs viewed. For instance, although one study reported achievement
a
with high generally curvilinear relation one est achievement for children watching viewers to two hours a day, these moderate were also more to report watching likely heavier whereas educational programming viewers were more ment
likely
to report entertain several more recent
viewing.59 Indeed, studies have found that achievement to early exposure specifically television programming.
to
is linked
educational
Media and Young Children's Learning
One
of the most
studies of this kind
extensive
educational program reported that viewing was at associated five age ming positively with high school grades in English, math, and science. Early exposure to educational programming a host of other
opposed information
linked with also positively factors such as leisure time
on the and thirty months of age depended content viewed. For example, early exposure to programs such as Blue's Clues and Dora was
linked with sub positively language sequent vocabulary and expressive was Teletubbies whereas negatively viewing on these measures.61 linked with performance its face, these findings contradict results from video indicating little language learning in children under two. It is possible, however,
On
that the findings are attributable not to learn to self-selection ing from programs but rather
language
have well-developing to watch different
skills prefer than their more
slowly develop nature of Given the correlational peers. ing to know for certain this study, it is impossible these findings. what produced programs
entertainment
and viewing
to educational)
was
such that children who
studies report longitudinal associations between academic
negative achievement
in extracurricular reading and involvement activities.60 In another longitudinal study, six the effect of television exposure between
the Explorer
some
content,
media.63
(as
Specific from
learned
spontaneously does not appear to viewing entertainment same cumulative have the long-term benefit as viewing
curriculum-based
educational
programming.
Educational
programs are associated positively with overall measures of and with
achievement
potentially long-lasting effects, while purely entertainment
content, violent
particularly is negatively
respect
associated
with
achievement.
academic With
content,
to interactive media
such as
are games and the Internet, findings mixed, almost entirely correlational (allowing and no conclusive cause-effect associations), video
educational programs Although watching social benefits, watching and have academic other types of content can have drastically different results. For instance, longitudinal a research has demonstrated correlational negative association between sure to violent video content
can
early expo and academic
achievement.62
Not
all non-educational
television
programs such as have explicitly negative violence, but research on the links between and general entertain academic achievement children ment content is less clear. Although content
can learn spontaneously
from entertainment
young children. Al a associa negative study reported though tion between video game use and academic
seldom
with
conducted
one
achievement a
in adolescents, between
association
others
report achievement
positive and computer and Internet use at home.64 these few studies may suggest that Though linked with video games are negatively and Inter whereas achievement computers linked with net are positively additional research is needed cally investigate
this potential
achievement, to systemati in difference
outcome.
VOL.
18
/ NO. 1 / SPRING
2008
49
Heather L. Kirkorian, Ellen A. Wartella, and Daniel R. Anderson
To summarize,
when
portant confounding and parent education,
studies
control
for im
to children.
tial benefits
such as income
variables
they often fail to find linear relations between television
significant viewing and subsequent the association appears
In fact, achievement. to be curvilinear, with to a achievement at low levels increasing peak of television viewing (one to two hours a day), and then with heavier viewing. That declining
In this section we
a few important mediators briefly discuss the effectiveness of educational media.
of
Attention Children
cannot
messages
to which
Moreover,
viewers
learn from educational not pay attention. they do learn more from television
programs when they can pay sustained, un broken attention.65 Researchers have identi fied several means
of maximizing children's some to a program, to do having program content, others with formal fea
attention
To maximize resources
the cognitive available to children
with
tures such as camera
and sound techniques one way to increase atten is to maximize comprehen of the content, a topic we discuss in sibility
As noted, a tion to program
effects.
to process educational content, one study suggests that producers integrate narrative content
the
and educational as much as possible.
next
section.66
At least years, by the preschool formal features of media?those istics that can be described to content?to
reference said, the most
children
use
character
with minimal guide
attention.
be content
cuts between shots, camera example, are and sound effects to considered pans,
cational
be formal
with
appears important mediator of the programs viewed. Edu associated programs are positively
overall measures
of achievement
to
formal
and
with potentially long-lasting effects, while content, particularly purely entertainment is violent content, associated with negatively academic
content
systematically. Nonetheless, in the section, software with previous an educational can have a curriculum positive influence on learning.
noted
Production Techniques Maximize Educational of Electronic Media Producers
of children's
can do several 50
THE FUTURE
things
to Benefits
educational to maximize
OF CHILDREN
media the poten
differ
study found that in the extent to which
and suppress looks at the television. terminate,
are example, child voices likely to elicit looks from inattentive viewers whereas adult
male
voices
are
to suppress looks. likely The authors of the their study interpreted as learned associa findings demonstrating
to examine
and computers
features
One
For
achievement.
games effects
features.
they elicit, maintain, children's preschool
Age may also be an Too mediator. few studies have important been conducted with interactive media such as video
For
as
tions between of content.67 associate
formal Children,
features
and types for instance, often
child voices with
child-directed
and adult male voices with programming content for adults. The attention-directing effect of formal features may thus change with
consistent with age and experience, Huston and Wright's theory.68 Such theories some interest in generate understanding how infants and toddlers respond to formal
Media and Young Children's Learning
to date though the only such study that infants, toddlers, and older concluded to a few visual children responded similarly formal features.69 That is, the same features features,
to elicit and maintain
appear
in all
attention
young viewers from infancy at least through viewers of the preschool years. Although all ages respond to formal features, Huston that content and Wright's theory predicts becomes formal
important with age and increasingly less impor features consequently
tant except insofar as they are used to help the finding that process content. Moreover, attention
age two is driven does not necessar
in children
under
features partly by formal mean that video. It is they comprehend ily more children respond likely that such young to the saliency and unfamiliar automatically ity of formal
features.
Comprehension as children Just
cannot understand
not pay they do content learn from
to which
cational message
cannot
attention,
an edu
they Shalom Fisch that they do not understand. what he called the capacity model proposed of television of children's comprehension on the limited
based
cognitive for process people have available at any given moment.70 Fisch ing information a a makes distinction between program's programs resources
narrative
content?its
tional content?its
story?and
informative
its educa
messages?
the potential competition and emphasizes the two types of content for the between cognitive
resources
needed
program. To maximize es available to children tional content,
Fisch
to process
the
the cognitive resourc to process educa
suggests that producers content and educational
integrate narrative as much as the educational possible, making a central part of the ongoing story. message For example, characters may have to solve a before advancing to the particular problem
next chapter in the story. In this way, narra content can capitalize on tive and educational resources same rather than compete for the of the presentation televi focused on educational Fisch's
them. Although capacity model
it can easily be applied particular, interactive media as well. educational,
to
sion in
Repetition can be such a power reason why media tool is that content can be ful educational
One
Literal repeti easily and cheaply repeated. tion of episodes can enhance comprehension and subsequent learning. We have already noted the experimental study comparing one who watched episode of preschoolers the Blue's Clues with those who watched on five consecutive once a day episode to the episode attention days. In that study, remained high and relatively constant over while com the course of five presentations same
for program prehension with repeated exposure
content
increased
to the episode. interacted with the also increasingly (in terms of audience participation)
Children content
was as the Similar repeated.71 episode benefits of literal repetition have been ed in other
studies.72 Moreover,
report the Blue's
found that transfer of experiment learning from the specific examples present ed in the program to different problems with
Clues
similar solutions program
increased
as a function
of
repetition.
Viewer
Characteristics
Some
studies of media
effects
suggest
that
a variety of viewer characteristics, including socioeconomic but not limited to intelligence, the effects of status, and gender, can mediate media on learning and academic achievement. To the extent that producers of children's me dia can take these characteristics
into account
and production, they during program design value. For example, educational enhance may VOL. 18
/ NO.
1 / SPRING
2008
51
Heather L Kirkorian, Ellen A. Wartella, and Daniel R. Anderson
several correlational
studies
suggest that the television viewing may be stronger
negative impact of heavy on academic achievement
for girls or for individuals with higher intel ligence.73 Other studies suggest that television effects on chil viewing may have differential dren from different
groups.
viewing is associated in children from achievement
Specifically, with higher
homes
lower-income in children
socioeconomic
television
and lower achievement
homes.74 A higher-income study that separately analyzed
association
types found
between
that the positive to educational
exposure programs at age five and later achievement was significantly stronger for boys while the negative association between violent content and later achievement The
was
stronger for girls. this finding in the
authors
context
interpreted of socialization. For
socialization emphasis educational
because
example,
of girls generally
places
more
on academics,
early exposure to may help boys become
programs more relatively prepared for school.75 Al though these studies are often correlational
for the express purpose rarely conducted of investigating individual differences such as race or
some gender, they highlight possible of the effects of media on children. mediators
Direct
of
Learning learning of specific
educational
information
is
media
from but a
certainly useful, most of not educational initiatives (if all) goal is to empower to children apply what they have learned to real-life problems. Thus children must
transfer
to the real world what
they learn from the media context (for a television program set in a fantasy example now know Researchers environment). relatively
little about
transfer of
young children, particularly with television and interactive media, evidence 52
suggests
learning respect
though that even preschoolers
THE FUTURE OF CHILDREN
transfer
from television
based
on transfer of
in children learning and analogical reasoning more can Fisch that transfer argues generally, be maximized not only by repeating the in the course of the educational messages but also by varying the contexts He suggests surrounding each presentation. that presenting the same lesson, such as a
specific problem-solving strategy, several times different using types of examples can a child's mental increase the flexibility of of that representation strategy, thus enhanc select and ing the child's ability to accurately in different it real-life situations.77 apply
Parent
Coviewing
in to some can
and Mediation
as media
can increase the producers so value of electronic media, parents and other caregivers can also play an important role in increasing the effectiveness Just educational
of educational example, educational
and
Transfer
to real-life prob of ideal conditions for
information
lems.76 In a discussion
episode
from
longitudinal different content
transfer video
media.
can enhance
adults, for Coviewing the effectiveness of
programming by drawing at tention to the most important aspects of the and lessons presented program by extending in the program. Some studies suggest that a coviewing with parent or other adult may increase a child's learning from educational television, particularly when the coviewer ac tively mediates to the program
by explicitly drawing attention and by asking and answering some studies fail to find Although
questions.78 a benefit of adult coviewing our no evidence knowledge tive link between
or mediation,
to
a nega suggests such parent involvement
and learning from television. With respect to interactive media, are mixed. findings from educational software Although learning may be enhanced when an adult provides feedback or extends the lessons, it seems that children still need to be free to control the interactive
experience
themselves
to maintain
Media and Young Children's Learning
in the activity.79 Taken together, adult are most to coviewing and mediation likely have a positive effect on learning from educa tional media. interest
on electronic
most research Although focuses on use at home,
some
evaluating the classroom. create
Efforts
media are
initiatives
the use of educational
in
media
have been made
to
that integrate educa a set programs, and massive
school curricula
tional television of evaluations way.80 Ready ing initiative
of such initiatives to Learn, to enhance
a
is now under
public broadcast school readiness
television programs through educational for and online resources, offered workshops to extend how educators and showing parents lessons from television programs through practice and repetition. tion of Ready to Learn
positively academic
A five-year evalua found a modest but and the
the workshops positive link between time adults spent coviewing PBS programs lessons in and reading books that extended no the programs.81 Although analysts found that children's language and cogni evidence
associated
linked media
use with
and academic skill development most with achievement, thorough studies cognitive
strongly
suggesting
that content
is the most
factor in that relation. important mediating true for is particularly the finding Although television, it is likely to be important for interactive media evidence
as well. There
that children
from educational ate evidence
older
media,
that exposure
is strong than two learn
and there ismoder to educational
regarding the exposure to high educational media
findings
with
an
media
can take steps to maximize the effects of media and minimize
parents positive
ones. Research
negative
should guide the that foster learning
of programs production evidence and transfer. Moderate can also maximize
suggests that the benefits of
parents media by selecting age-appropriate, tional programs and coviewing with
educa their
children.
Our
review of media
effects
research
is based
on studies of young children of largely less age and older. Substantially preschool research is available on media exposure in
under
studies have
Research
associated
to capitalize on the important opportunity time that children older than two spend using and In fact, both producers these media.
Conclusions Many
in
quality, age-appropriate, offer producers of child-directed
media
benefited
ten years later. that early content,
also suggests entertainment
is negatively particular, and skills academic cognitive
with
achievement. benefits
even
achievement evidence
Moderate
from the coviewing, hold some promise. the findings nevertheless The apparent benefits of adult mediation may a new area for extending the lessons provide of educational media. tive abilities
is during the preschool years of linked with various measures
exposure to purely and media violence
in Schools
Media
Educational
television
children
younger
than two, and what
little
is strongly suggests that learning from be differ by infants and toddlers may ent than it is for older children. Children there
two suffer from a video deficit
such that
less from video than they learn substantially real-life experiences. from comparable nonetheless weak but Moreover, worrying evidence
suggests
a
negative association to television younger
than exposure age two and later cognitive development. Given the dramatic increase in media now between
for infants and toddlers, it has being produced to understand become particularly important the effect of media during the first few years of life. VOL.
18
/ NO.
1 / SPRING
2008
53
Heather L. Kirkorian, Ellen A. Wartella, and Daniel R. Anderson
Taken
together, the research indicates that electronic media are powerful influences on the lives of children. With contemporary advances
in
that provide dimensional possibilities media will
54
such as larger screens technology in three images high definition, surround sound, and greater for interaction,
the power of for the foresee
likely only increase
THE FUTURE OF CHILDREN
able future. The
influences
good and for ill. Researchers to understand which aspects be reduced research
and which
is
can be both for are
beginning of media should
enhanced,
but further
however, the required. Ultimately, is whether has the question society ability and will to enhance the positive aspects of media and reduce
the negative.
Media and Young Children's Learning
Endnotes 1.
J.
L.
"The Power
Singer,
Function
and Limits edited
of Television,
A
of Television:
P. Tannenbaum
by
Cognitive-Affective
(Hillsdale,
N.J.:
in The Entertainment
Analysis,"
Lawrence
Erlbaum,
1980).
2. J.Healy, Endangered Minds: Why Our Children Don't Think (New York: Simon & Schuster, 1990). 3. A. C. Huston
and
and D.
by J. Bryant R. Anderson
R. Anderson
York: Academic
Press,
D.
pp. 1-31;
edited
R. Anderson
and
1983),
in Children's
Understanding
R. Anderson of TV
"The Effects
others,
edited
Comprehension,
and D.
by J. Bryant
and
of Formal
35-68.
pp.
or Reaction?"
Action
and Comprehension,
1983),
Inc.,
Functions
Informative
(New
Program
on Preschool Children's Visual Attention to Television," Child Development
Comprehensibility 151-57;
Inc.,
Press,
at Television:
"Looking
The
on Attention
Research
York: Academic
(New
on Attention
Research
of Television:
Processing of Television:
Understanding
and E. P. Lorch,
of Television:
S.
"Children's
Wright,
in Children's
Features,"
4. D.
C.
J.
"Children's
Pingree,
and Television
Activity
Communication
Comprehensibility,"
52 (1981):
Research
12
(1986): 239-56. 5.
K. L.
6.
T. A.
K. D. Woolf,
Schmitt,
and Adults
J.
R. Anderson,
C.
and A. C. Huston,
Wright,
the Viewers:
"Viewing
and Commercials,"
Programs
during
Campbell,
and D.
Television
Journal
"Form
Cues
Behaviors
Viewing
and Content
by
53
of Communication
Children 265-81.
(2003):
as Determinants
Difficulty
of Children's Cognitive Processing of Televised Educational Messages," Journal of Experimental Child 43
Psychology
E. P. Lorch,
311-27;
(1987):
Attention to Children's Comprehension 7.
R.
D.
Smith,
R. Anderson,
D.
R. Anderson,
and
S. R. Levin,
58 (1979): 453-563.
of Television," Child Development
and C. Fischer,
Children's
"Young
of Visual
"The Relationship
Comprehension
of Montage,"
Child
Attention
and Time
Devel
opment 56 (1985): 962-71. 8. D.
R. Anderson
and others,
"Television
at Home:
Viewing
in Visual
Trends
Age
with
TV," Child Development 57 (1986): 1024-33; D. R. Anderson and S. R. Levin, "YoungChildren's Attention to Sesame Street," Child Development 47 (1976): 806-11. 9.
R. Barr
and
"Television
others,
Exposure
Patterns
Infancy:
during
of Viewing,
and
Attention,
Interaction,"
poster presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research inChild Development, Tampa, Fla., April 2003; T A. Pempek and others, "The Impact of Baby Videos," paper presented at the biannual
10.
For
a review
son,
"Attentional
of the
11.
H.
L. Kirkorian,
Age
by
in
Eye
Boston,
Development, driving
Extended
R. V. Kail
Differences
and others,
Pempek
in Children's
32, edited
Massachusetts-Amherst, 12. TA.
mechanisms
underlying
Inertia vol.
and Behavior,
in Child
for Research
of the Society
meeting
Looking
Academic
Press,
Video
during
J.
2007.
E. Richards
in Advances
at Television,"
(Amsterdam:
Movements
see
to video,
attention
March
Mass.,
Viewing,
2004),
pp.
and D.
in Child
R. Ander
Development
163-212.
Dissertation,
of
University
2007.
"Infant
Responses
to
Sequential
and Linguistic
Distortions
of Teletubbies,"
inChild Development, poster session presented at the biannual meeting of the Society for Research Boston, 13.
L.
March
Jaglom
2007.
and H. Gardner,
"The Preschool
Television
Viewer
through Television: New Directions for Child Development, Francisco:
Jossey-Bass,
1981),
pp.
as
Anthropologist,"
in
Viewing
Children
edited by H. Kelly and H. Gardner (San
9-30. VOL. 18
/ NO. 1 / SPRING
2008
55
Heather L. Kirkorian, Ellen A. Wartella, and Daniel R. Anderson
14. J.H. Flavell and others, "Do Young Children Think of Television Images as Pictures or Real Objects?" Journal of Broadcasting and Electronic Media 34 (1990): 399-419. 15.
J.
L. Blatt,
Television stein
L.
D.C.:
(Washington,
"A
in Television
Advertising,"
"Children's
S. Ward,
and
Spencer,
U.S.
Perceptions,
Cognitive
and
Social
Government
Study
vol.
Behavior, Office,
Printing and
Explanations,
Developmental
4, edited
1972);
and D.
A Further
in Television and Social Behavior, vol. 4, edited by G Comstock and E. Rubenstein Government
Television
Children's
16.
D.
Office,
Printing
Programming,"
"Children
Kunkel,
E. Palmer
1972);
and
and C. McDowell,
Television
Host-Selling
29, no.
3 (1979):
Exploration,"
in
Separators
197-201.
Communication
Commercials,"
Levinson,
(Washington: U.S.
"Program/Commercial
of Communication
Journal
and E. Ruben
G. Reale,
Advertising:
to
Reactions
G. Comstock
by
S.Ward,
of Television
Judgments
of Children's
15
Research
(1988):
71-92.
17.
B. Blosser News, 12
and D.
Educational
Commercials,
(1985):
Television
Commercials,"
and Commercial 13-20;
and T
42
of Marketing
(1978):
Theory
"Children's
4 (1974):
Service
Analysis,"
Television
S.Ward,
137-44;
"Black
and White T
Journal
and
of
"Children
J. Rossiter, Research
Testing
the Defenses,"
and E. Wartella,
Research
Perceptions
of Consumer
Commercials:
D. Wackman,
Children:
Robertson
to TV
Responses
Communication
Announcements,"
34-40;
Intent:
of Message
Perceptions
and L. Henke,
Meyer,
An Attribution
Robertson,
24, no.
Communication
T.
Journal
Persuasion:
J. Rossiter
and Public
Spots,
T. T Donahue,
455-84;
in Children's
"Age Differences
Roberts,
How
1 (1974): of
Journal
Children
Learn
to
Buy: The Development of Consumer Information Processing Skills (Beverly Hills, Calif: Sage, 1977). 18.
D.
R. Anderson
and T A.
"Television
Pempek,
and Very
American
Children,"
Young
Scientist
Behavioral
48 (2005): 505-22. 19.
L. R.
and E. T. Kako,
Naigles
64
Development
(1993):
in Verb
"First Contact M.
1665-87;
L. Rice
Acquisition:
and others,
a Role
Defining
"Words
from
for Syntax,"
Street:
Sesame
Learning
Child Vocabulary
While Viewing," Developmental Psychology 26 (1990): 421-28; M. L. Rice and L.Woodsmall, "Lessons from Television: Children's Word Learning When Viewing," Child Development 59 (1988): 420-29. 20.
M.
B. Grela,
Krcmar,
and K. Lin,
"Can Toddlers
Learn
from Television?
Vocabulary
An
Experimental
Approach," Media Psychology 10 (2007): 41-63. 21.
P. K. Kuhl,
F. Tsao,
and H.
Liu,
"Foreign
Language
in
Experiences
Infancy:
Effects
of Short-Term
Expo
sure and Interaction on Phonetic Learning," Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 100 (2003): 9096-101.
22.
R. Barr
and H.
Development 23.
H.
Hayne,
mental
24.
"Developmental
and G
Simcock,
6, no.
3 (2003):
254-61.
Ross,
and
J. Herbert,
Science
A. Bandura,
Hayne,
in Imitation
Changes
from Television
during
Infancy,"
Child
70 (1999): 1067-81.
D.
S. A. Ross,
"Imitation
"Imitation
from
Television
of Film-Mediated
by
24-
Aggressive
and
30-Month-Olds,"
Models,"
Journal
Develop
of Abnormal
and Social Psychology 66 (1963): 3-11. 25.
G
Troseth
and
J. DeLoache,
"The Medium
Can
Obscure
Video and Reality," Child Development 69 (1998): 950-65. 56
THE FUTURE OF CHILDREN
the
Message:
Understanding
the Relation
between
Media and Young Children's Learning 26.
K. L.
and D.
Schmitt
R. Anderson,
"Television
and
Use
of Visual
"Two-Year-Olds'
Object
Toddlers'
Reality:
Information
from
Video to Guide Behavior," Media Psychology 4 (2002): 51-76. 27.
M.
E.
A. M.
Schmidt,
and D.
Crawley-Davis,
R. Anderson,
Retrieval
on
Based
Television: Testing a Perceptual Account," Media Psychology 9 (2007): 389^09. 28.
G
"TV Guide:
Troseth,
to Use
Learn
Children
Two-Year-Old
as a Source
Video
of Information,"
Develop
mental Psychology 39 (2003): 140-50; G Troseth, M. M. Saylor, and A. H. Archer, "YoungChildren's Use of Video as a Source of Socially Relevant Information," Child Development 77 (2006): 786-99. 29. Pempek and others, "The Impact of Baby Videos" (see note 9). 30. V J. Rideout and E. Hamel, The Media Family: Electronic Media in the Lives of Infants, Toddlers, Preschoolers, and Their Parents (Menlo Park, Calif: The Henry J. Kaiser Family Foundation, 2006). 31.
32.
"The Power
Singer,
D. A. Christakis
and Limits
and others,
(see note
of Television"
"Early Television
1). and
Exposure
in Children,"
Problems
Attentional
Subsequent
Pediatrics 113 (2004): 708-13. 33. G Obel and others, "Does Children's Watching of Television Cause Attentional Problems? Retesting the Hypothesis in a Danish Cohort," Pediatrics 114 (2004): 1372-73; K. B. Mistry and others, "Children's Television
and Behavioral
Exposure
and
Social
Does
Outcomes:
of Exposure
Timing
Pediatrics
Matter?,"
120 (2007): 762-69; T. Stevens and M. Mulsow, "There Is No Meaningful Relationship between Television Exposure and Symptoms of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder," Pediatrics 117 (2006): 665-72. 34.
F
J.
and D.
Zimmerman
A. Christakis,
"Associations
between
Content
of
Types
Media
Early
Exposure
and Subsequent Attentional Problems," Pediatrics 120 (2007): 986-92. 35.
and A. H.
L. K. Friedrich
Stein,
and Prosocial
"Aggressive
Television
the Natural
and
Programs
36.
A. M.
and others,
Crawley on
Clues
Blue's
of
"Effects
the Viewing
Behaviors
and
to a
Exposures
Repeated
Single
Comprehension
of the Television
Episode
of Preschool
Children,"
Behavior
38 (1973): 4.
of Preschool Children," Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development
Journal
Program
of Educational
in Spatial Cognition: Psychology 91 (1999): 630-37; J. Rovet, "The Education of Spatial Transformations," edited Mental Structures and The Representation of Spatial Relations, by D. R. Olson and Development of E.
(Hillsdale,
Bialystok
Erlbaum,
N.J.:
1983),
G
164-81;
pp.
of Filmic
"Internalization
Salomon,
Schematic
66 (1974): Operations in Interaction with Learners' Aptitudes," Journal of Educational Psychology 499-511; G
G
Salomon
Interaction
Salomon,
and A. Cohen,
of Media,
"Television
Formats:
(San Francisco:
and Learning
Cognition,
of Mental
Mastery
Skills
and
Jossey-Bass,
the Acquisition
of
1979);
Knowledge,"
Journal of Educational Psychology 69 (1977): 612-19. 37.
38.
Singer, D.
and Limits
"The Power
R. Anderson,
S. R. Levin,
(see note
of Television"
and E. P. Lorch,
1).
"The Effects
of TV Program
Pacing
on
the Behavior
of
in Girls
and
Journal
of
Preschool Children," Educational Communication & Technology 25 (1977): 159-66. 39.
K.
and P. M.
Subrahmanyam
Boys,"
Special
Issue:
Effects
Greenfield, of Interactive
"Effect
of Video
Entertainment
Game
Practice
Technologies
on on
Spatial
Skills
Development,
in P. M. Greenfield and R. R. Cocking, Applied Developmental Psychology 15 (1994): 13-32. Reprinted eds.,
Interacting
with
Video
(Norwood,
N.J.:
Ablex,
1996),
pp.
115-40. VOL. 18
/ NO.
1 / SPRING
2008
57
Heather L Kirkorian, Ellen A. Wartella, and Daniel R Anderson
40.
P. A.
and C. Chaille,
McClurg
Games:
"Computer
Environments
for
Developing
Spatial
Cognition?"
Journal of Educational Computing Research 3 (1987): 95-111. 41.
F.
J. Zimmerman
and D.
A. Christakis,
"Children's
Television
tudinal Analysis of National Data," Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine 42.
A. F. Alexander,
J. Bryant,
and D.
Brown,
from
"Learning
Educational
Television
1983),
(Mahwah,
Beyond 43.
Communication 44. 45.
and
Crawley S. Ball
A.
1970); G
Service,
ation
(Princeton,
N.J.:
Project:
Sesame
Street
Children
and
Associates,
Sesame
2001),
National
of
A.
by
S. M.
Year
Educational
edited N.
97-114;
S. M.
in "G" Is and R. T.
Fisch
1971);
for
Ala.:
(Tuscaloosa,
J.
C.
Street
"Growing":
Institute
and
for
"Growing":
N.J.:
Erlbaum
Window
Early
Years
on
of Research
Lawrence
Erlbaum from
Evidence
on Children
of Research
Lawrence
"The
N.J.:
Evalu
Continuing
Readiness?:
School
Years
A
Thirty
(Mahwah,
Truglio
Educational
N.J.:
Street:
and others,
Wright
Enhance
Thirty
(Mahwah,
(Princeton,
of Sesame
in "G" Is for
Sesame
Truglio
Street
36).
Year
and R. T.
Fisch
"Does
Sesame
Television:
An Evaluation
Second
for School,"
by
Zill,
The
Service,
Testing
Clues"
(see note Street:
S. Ball,
Children
Prepares Street,
of Sesame
and
Learning
(London: Academic
1999).
Exposures"
Repeated
in
Programs,"
2004).
of "Blue's
Viewing
Educational
from
Associates,
of Alabama,
Bogatz
of Children,"
Survey edited
Street,
pp.
Years'
University
The First
Bogatz,
Learning
Erlbaum Two
of
"Effects
others,
and G
Testing
Effects
Research,
Children's
Fisch,
Lawrence
N.J.:
and others,
J. Bryant
S. M.
1-30;
pp.
Longi
159 (2005): 619-25.
from Television: Psychological and Educational Research, edited by M. J.A. Howe Press,
A
Outcomes:
and Cognitive
Viewing
and
Associates,
a
Sesame
2001),
pp.
115-130.
46. K. Chandler and others, Statistics in Brief: Home Literacy Activities and Signs of Children's Emerging 1993 1999 and D.C.: Center National for Education Statistics, 1999). Literacy, (Washington, 47. K. A. Magnuson and J.Waldfogel,
48.
Gaps
in School
R. D.
Hess
Readiness,"
and L.
"Early Childhood Care and Education: Effects on Ethnic and Racial
Future
15, no.
of Children "School-Relevant
J. McGarvey,
1: 169-88.
Effects
of Educational
Uses
of
in
Microcomputers
Kindergarten Classrooms and Homes," Journal of Educational Computing Research 3 (1987): 269-87. 49.
F. S. Din
and
J. Calao,
"The Effects
of
Playing
Educational
Video
on
Games
Achievement,"
Kindergarten
Child Study Journal 31, no. 2 (2001): 95-102; S.W Haugland, "The Effect of Computer Software on Pre school Children's Developmental Gains," Journal of Computing in Childhood Education 3 (1992): 15-30; R.
Shute
and
J. Miksad,
"Computer
Assisted
Instruction
and
Cognitive
in Preschoolers,"
Development
Child Study Journal 27 (1997): 237-53. 50. H. Himmelweit, A. Oppenheim, and P. Vince, Television and the Child (London: Oxford, 1958); J.Murray and S. Kippax, "Children's Social Behavior inThree Towns with Differing Television Experience," Journal of Communication the
Displacement
28
(1978):
Hypothesis:
19-29;
D.
Television's
C. Mutz,
D.
Influence
F. Roberts,
and D.
on Children's
Time
P. van Vuuren,
"Reconsidering
Communication
Use,"
Research
20 (1993): 51-75; S. B. Neuman, Literacy in the Television Age: The Myth of the TV Effect (Norwood, W Schramm, J. Lyle, and E. Parker, Television in the Lives of Our Children (Stanford N.J.: Ablex, 1991); University 51.
E. A. Vandewater Children's
58
Press,
1961). and
others,
Development,"
THE FUTURE OF CHILDREN
"When
American
the Television Behavioral
Is
Always
Scientist
48
On: (2005):
Heavy
Television
562-77;
R.
Exposure
S. Corteen
and
Young
and T. M.
Media and Young Children's Learning
"Television and Reading Skills," in The Impact of Television: A Natural Experiment in Three
Williams,
edited
Communities, van
der Voort,
by
Human
Fla.:
(Orlando,
Communication
Academic
Press,
on Children's
of Television
Effects
"Longitudinal Models,"
Explanatory
T. M. Williams
23
Research
C. M.
1986);
Leisure-Time
and T. H. A.
Koolstra A Test
Reading:
of Three
4-35.
(1996):
52. S. Coffey and H. Stipp, "The Interactions between Computer and Television Usage," Journal of Advertising Research 37 (1997): 61-67; U. G Foehr, Media Multitasking among American Youth: Prevalence, Predic tors, and Pairings (Menlo Park, Calif: The Henry J. Kaiser Family Foundation, 2006). 53.
P. A. Williams
and others,
"The
Impact
of Leisure-Time
on School
Television
A Research
Learning:
Synthesis," American Educational Research Journal 19 (1982): 19-50. 54. G. Comstock and H. Paik, Television and the American Child (Orlando, Fla.: Academic Press, 1991). 55. Williams 56.
M.
and others,
"The
"Television
Fetler,
of Leisure-Time
Impact and
Viewing
School
(see note
Television"
Achievement,"
53). 34, no.
of Communication
Journal
2 (1984):
104-18; S. B. Neuman, Literacy in the Television Age: The Myth of the TV Effect (Norwood, N.J.: Ablex, M.
1991);
"The
Razel,
Model
Complex
of Television
Viewing
and Educational
Achievement,"
Journal
of
Educational Research 94 (2001): 371-79. 57. Razel, "The Complex Model of Television Viewing" (see note 56). 58.
Zimmerman
59.
Fetler,
60.
D.
and Christakis,
"Television
R. Anderson
"Children (see note
Viewing" and
and Television
others,
Television
of the Society for Research in Child Development, 61.
D.
L.
and D. Walker,
Linebarger
41).
56).
Childhood
"Early
(see note
Viewing"
"Infants'
and Adolescent
Viewing
Behavior,"
Monographs
68, Serial No. 264 (2001), 1-143.
and Toddlers'
Television
and Language
Viewing
Outcomes,"
American Behavioral Scientist 48 (2005): 624-25. 62.
Anderson
and others,
Television
and
Television
Childhood
"Early
the Aggressive
Viewing"
A Cross-National
Child:
(see note
60); L. R. Huesmann
(Hillsdale,
Comparison
and L. Eron,
Lawrence
N.J.:
Erlbaum,
1986). 63.
For
studies
son,
"Instruction
how
showing and
can
children
Modality
learn
from
entertainment
on Children's
Effects
Television
see D.E.
content, Attention
and
Field
and D.
R. Ander Journal
Comprehension,"
of Educational Psychology 11 (1985): 91-100; G Noble, "Social Learning from Everyday Television," inLearning from Television: Psychological and Educational Research ,edited by M. J.Howe (London: Academic Press, 1983), pp. 1-30. One such longitudinal study isAnderson and others, "EarlyChildhood Television 64.
M.
B. Harris
P. Attewell (1999):
(see note
Viewing"
60).
and R. Williams, and
1-10;
"Home
J. Battle,
L. A.
"Video
Jackson
and
Games
and
Computers others,
and
"Does
School School
Home
Performance," Performance,"
Internet
Use
105
Education The
Information
Influence
(1985): Society
the Academic
306-09; 15
Performance
of Low-Income Children?" Developmental Psychology 42 (2006): 429-35. 65.
J. J. Burns,
and D.
R. Anderson,
"Attentional
Inertia
and Recognition
Memory
in Adult
Television
Viewing,"
Communication Research 20 (1993): 777-99. 66.
Anderson
and others,
"The Effects
of TV
Program
Comprehensibility"
(see note VOL. 18
4).
/ NO. 1 / SPRING
2008
59
Heather L Kirkorian, Ellen A. Wartella, and Daniel R Anderson
67.
D.
R. Anderson
and
others,
Children
"Watching
Watch
Attention
Television,"
and
Cognitive
Development,
edited by G Hale and M. Lewis (New York: Plenum, 1979), pp. 331-61. 68.
Huston
69.
K. L.
and
"Children's
Wright,
"Infants,
Schmitt,
and Television:
Toddlers,
(see note
of Television"
Processing
The
3). to Three
of the Home,"
Zero
of Educational
Content
Ecology
22
(2001):
17-23.
70.
S. M.
"A
Fisch,
Model
Capacity
of Children's
Comprehension
on Television,"
Media Psychology 2 (2000): 63-91; Fisch, Children's Learning from Educational Television (see note 42); A. D. Baddeley, Working Memory (Oxford: Clarendon Press, 1986). 71.
72.
and
Crawley H.
and L.
Skouteris
Factors
"Effects
others,
that
Kelly, on
Impact
of
36). of an Animated
and Co-Viewing
"Repeated-Viewing Children's
Young
(see note
Exposures"
Repeated
of Video
Comprehension
An Examination
Video: Australian
Content,"
of
of Early
Journal
Childhood 31 (2006): 22-30. 73. Williams
and
"Parental
"The
others,
Involvement,
of Leisure-Time
Impact
and TV Time:
Homework,
(see note
Television" Direct
and
Indirect
53); T. Z. Keith
and
on
School
Effects
High
others, Achieve
ment," Journal of Educational Psychology 78 (1986): 373-80. 74.
Comstock School
75.
76.
and Paik,
Anderson
and others,
Educational
"Early
of
(see note
Child
Television
and Far
(see note
Viewing"
(see note
Exposures"
Repeated
Near
Transfer
from
"Children's
Ability
and
Viewing
Problem-Solving
60). S. M.
36);
Fisch,
poster
Cyberchase,
meeting of the Society for Research inChild Development, to Learn
"Television
54); Fetler,
56).
Childhood
"Effects Television:
the American
and
(see note
and others,
Crawley from
Television
Achievement"
Transfer
of Learning at the biennial
presented
Atlanta, Ga., April, 2005; T V Hodapp,
Strategies
from
Television
(see note
Alberta
Television,"
of Educational
Journal
Research 23 (1977): 171-77. 77.
Fisch, and
Children's relevant
Anderson, from
Learning
research "Transfer
a Modern
on
Educational
from transfer
of
of Learning
Multidisciplinary
more
learning
Education:
Perspective,
edited
The by
for a
see S. Fisch,
generally,
in Informal
42);
Case
J. Mestre
review
complete
H.
L. Kirkorian,
of Television,"
in
and D.
Transfer
Conn.:
(Greenwich,
of this model R.
of Learning
Information
Age
Publishing, 2005), pp. 371-93. 78. L. K. Friedrich and A. H. Stein, "Prosocial Television and Young Children: The Effects of Verbal Labeling and Role Playing on Learning and Behavior," Child Development 46 (1975): 27-38; P. M. Valkenburg, M.
Krcmar,
and
S. de Roos,
"The
Impact
of a Cultural
Children's
Program
and Adult
on
Mediation
Children's Knowledge of and Attitudes towards Oper a,"Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media 42 (1998): 315-26. 79.
S.W
"The Effect
Haugland,
Journal of Computing Instruction" for
Young
(see note Children's
Software
on Preschool
Children's
Developmental
Gains,"
in Childhood Education 3 (1992): 15-30; Shute and Miksad, "Computer Assisted 49);
S. L. Calvert,
Attention
(2005): 578-89. 60
of Computer
THE FUTURE OF CHILDREN
to and
B.
Strong,
Learning
of
and L. Computer
Gallagher, Content,"
"Control American
as an
Engagement
Behavioral
Feature Scientist
48
Media and Young Children's Learning 80.
S. Ball N.J.:
81.
and G. A.
Educational
K. Boiler Parents,
Bogatz,
Using and
with
Service,
Testing
and others, Educators,
Reading
Television
the Children
Television:
An Evaluation
of "The Electric
Company"
(Princeton,
1973). as a
Teaching
in Their
Care
Tool:
The
(Princeton,
Impacts N.J.:
of Ready Mathematica
to Learn Policy
on
Workshops
Inc.,
Research,
2004).
VOL. 18
/ NO. 1 / SPRING
2008
61