Parallel Information Processing in the Dorsal

3 downloads 0 Views 355KB Size Report
and to behavior based on cognitive–spatial information in the water maze. Rats were trained concurrently on the cue (visible platform) and spatial (submerged ...
The Journal of Neuroscience, April 1, 1999, 19(7):2789–2798

Parallel Information Processing in the Dorsal Striatum: Relation to Hippocampal Function Bryan D. Devan and Norman M. White Department of Psychology, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, Canada H3A 1B1

We investigated the effects of localized medial and lateral CPu lesions and fornix/fimbria lesions on responses to a local cue and to behavior based on cognitive–spatial information in the water maze. Rats were trained concurrently on the cue (visible platform) and spatial (submerged platform) components of the task, followed by a test in which responses to the two types of information were dissociated by a measure of competing response tendencies. Bilateral lesions of lateral CPu did not affect acquisition of either cue or spatial responding but produced a preference for the spatial response on the competition test. Bilateral lesions of the medial CPu retarded but did not prevent learning both components and produced a preference for the cue response on the competition test. The latter effect was accompanied by increased thigmotaxis (swimming in the periphery of the pool), primarily during the early acquisition trials, which was attributed to an impaired ability to respond to

learned spatial information. Fornix/fimbria lesions prevented spatial but not cue learning and produced a preference for the cue response on the competition test. Asymmetric lesions (unilateral hippocampus and contralateral medial CPu) produced mild retardation of acquisition of both the cue and spatial tasks and a preference for the cue response on the competition test. These findings dissociate the functions of the lateral and medial CPu and suggest that the hippocampus and medial CPu may be parts of a system that promotes responding based on learned cognitive–spatial information, particularly in competitive cue–place response situations.

Studies of mnemonic f unction in rats frequently employ spatial or “place” cues to test cognitive forms of learning (Tolman, 1948; O’Keefe and Nadel, 1978) and discrete cues to measure habit or stimulus–response (S –R) learning (Thorndike, 1911, 1933; Hull, 1943). Evidence suggests that different neural systems contribute to each form of learning (Milner et al., 1968; Hirsh, 1974; O’Keefe and Nadel, 1978). Lesions of the dorsal striatum (caudate putamen; C Pu) and hippocampus or fornix /fimbria were previously shown to have opposite effects on behavior in cued and spatial versions of the radial maze task (Packard et al., 1989; McDonald and White, 1993). These findings are consistent with the hypothesis that a corticostriatal system mediates S –R habit formation, whereas a hippocampus-based system contributes to cognitive– spatial learning (Mishkin et al., 1984; Mishkin and Petri, 1984; Petri and Mishkin, 1994). Anatomical studies have shown that the dorsal striatum is a heterogeneous structure, both in terms of its intrinsic compartmentalization of neurochemical constituents (Graybiel, 1990; Groves et al., 1995) and its regionally diverse connectivity with other cortical /subcortical structures (Alexander et al., 1986; Mc-

George and Faull, 1989; Groenewegen et al., 1990). Behavioral studies further suggest that the striatum is functionally differentiated (Winocur, 1974; Divac et al., 1978; Dunnett and Iversen, 1981, 1982a,b; Iversen, 1984; Viaud and White, 1989; Pisa and Cyr, 1990; Reading et al., 1991; Hauber and Schmidt, 1993, 1994; Fricker et al., 1996). Lesions of the lateral CPu disrupt sensorimotor function and S–R learning (Dunnett and Iversen, 1981, 1982a; White, 1989, 1997; Reading et al., 1991), whereas lesions of the medial CPu have been reported to produce impairments on cognitive–spatial tasks (Whishaw et al., 1987; Columbo et al., 1989; Devan et al., 1996; Furtado and Mazurek, 1996). In the water maze, rats’ ability to learn the location of a hidden (submerged) platform, a cognitive–spatial task, is impaired by fornix/fimbria (Sutherland and Rodriguez, 1989; Devan et al., 1996) or hippocampal (Morris et al., 1982; Sutherland et al., 1983) lesions, whereas acquiring the response of swimming directly to a visible platform, an instance of S–R learning, is impaired by lateral CPu lesions (McDonald and White, 1994). Because lesions confined to the medial CPu impaired responses based on place cues (Devan et al., 1999), we compared the effects of separate medial and lateral CPu lesions with fornix/fimbria lesions on a combined cue–place learning task in the water maze (McDonald and White, 1994). In the second experiment, we studied the relationship of the medial CPu to the hippocampus in the same water maze task using asymmetric lesions consisting of damage to the hippocampus on one side of the brain and damage to the medial CPu on the other side. This lesion preparation has been used to define routes of serial information processing between other brain structures (Mishkin, 1958, 1966; Horel and Keating, 1969, 1972; Olton, 1978; Gaffan and Harrison, 1987; Gaffan et al., 1988, 1989;

Received Oct. 16, 1998; revised Jan. 13, 1999; accepted Jan. 20, 1999. This work was supported by grants from the Medical Research Council of Canada and Fonds pour lat Formation de Chercheurs et l’Aide `a la Rechereche, province de Quebec to N.M.W., and a Hydro Quebec/McGill Major Fellowship to B.D.D. We thank Peter Wallet for assistance with animal testing, Emily Goad and Janet Raymond for assistance with histology, and Dr. Robert McDonald for helpful discussions. Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. Bryan Devan at his present address. Dr. Devan’s present address: The Krasnow Institute for Advanced Study, George Mason University, Mail Stop 2A1, Fairfax, VA 22030-4444. Parts of this paper were reported in abstract form on October 27, 1997 at the Annual Meeting of the Society for Neuroscience, New Orleans, LA. Copyright © 1999 Society for Neuroscience 0270-6474/99/192789-10$05.00/0

Key words: dorsal striatum; medial CPu; lateral CPu; fornix/ fimbria; hippocampal-striatal interactions; water maze; cue/ place learning; competition test; asymmetric lesion; functional limbic circuit

2790 J. Neurosci., April 1, 1999, 19(7):2789–2798

Everitt et al., 1991; Sutherland and Hoesing, 1993; Gaffan and Parker, 1996; Floresco et al., 1997; Han et al., 1997).

MATERIALS AND METHODS Subjects. Ninety L ong–Evans hooded rats weighing ;300 – 400 gm at the time of surgery were used. They were housed individually in hanging wire-mesh cages located in a temperature-controlled room on a constant 12 hr light /dark cycle. All behavioral testing was conducted during the light phase at approximately the same time each day. Ad libitum food (Purina lab chow) and tap water were available in the home cage throughout the experiments. E xperimental conditions. In experiment 1, rats were assigned to four bilateral lesion groups: lateral C Pu (n 5 15), medial C Pu (n 5 11), fornix /fimbria (n 5 10), and a group of sham-operated controls (n 5 10). In experiment 2, rats were assigned to two unilateral lesion groups: medial C Pu (n 5 10) or hippocampus (n 5 10), or to the asymmetric lesion condition (unilateral hippocampus– contralateral medial C Pu) (n 5 12). There were also 12 sham-operated controls. Surger y. Each rat was handled for ;5 min /d for 3 d before surgery. All rats were given Xylazine analgesia (5 mg / kg, i.m.) and anesthetized with sodium pentobarbital (60 mg / kg, i.p.). Brain lesions were made using standard stereotaxic procedures. Flat skull stereotaxic coordinates were derived from the atlas of Paxinos and Watson (1986) using bregma of the skull surface as the reference point. The coordinates for dorsomedial C Pu lesions were 0.7 mm anterior (A), 2.4 mm lateral (L), and 5.4 mm ventral (V). The coordinates for dorsolateral C Pu lesions were 0.7 A, 3.4 L, and 5.4 V. Coordinates for fornix /fimbria lesions were 1.5 mm posterior (P), 0.8 and 2.2 L, and 4.5 V. The hippocampus was damaged using four separate lesions at the following coordinates: (1) 3.8 P, 2.0 L, 4.0 V; (2) 5.3 P, 3.5 L, 4.0 V; (3) 5.8 P, 5.0 L, 5.5 V; and (4) 5.8 P, 5.0 L, 7.5 V. C Pu and hippocampus lesions were made by passing 3 mA of direct anodal current for 15 sec through an insulated nichrome electrode exposed 0.8 mm at the tip. Fornix /fimbria lesions were made by radio frequency current (6 mA for 40 sec). The side of the unilateral lesions was counterbalanced within the appropriate groups. Sham animals received identical surgical treatment to the lesioned animals except that no electrode was lowered into the brain. Behavioral testing began 2 weeks after surgery. Apparatus. A white swimming pool measuring 172 cm in diameter and 63 cm in height was used. The pool was elevated 20 cm above the floor in a room that contained many stationary cues, including wall posters, book shelves, and a computer rack. The pool was filled to a depth of 38 cm with 22°C water, made opaque by the addition of 100 ml white nontoxic tempera paint before daily swimming trials. T wo platforms were used. The visible platform, used on cued trials, was constructed of wood (top surface, 12 3 12 cm). Black and white terry cloth strips were permanently attached to the sides and top surface. The visible platform protruded 3 cm above the surface of the water when placed in the pool. The hidden platform, used on place trials, was constructed of Plexiglas with white terry cloth attached to the top surface (10 3 10 cm). The top of the hidden platform was submerged 1.5 cm below the surface of the water. A video camera mounted above the pool was used to make recordings of swim trials. The recordings were used to measure escape latencies and peripheral pool time using a stop watch. Procedure. The cue –place water maze task was adapted from McDonald and White (1994). On the first 2 d of the experiment, the rats were trained to swim to the visible platform located in the center of the northwest quadrant of the pool. Each daily training session consisted of four trials on which each rat was released once from each of the four start points. A trial began by placing a rat in the pool (facing the wall) at a randomly selected start position and ended when the rat climbed onto the visible platform, or after 30 sec had elapsed. If the rat had not escaped after 30 sec, it was gently guided to the platform by hand. Each rat was left on the platform for 5 sec and then returned to its home cage, located in an adjoining room. For each rat there was a delay of 10 –15 min between successive trials within a daily session. During the delay, the remaining rats in the counterbalanced squad were run on the same trial. Thus, the intertrial interval varied slightly according to the rats’ level of performance but was approximately equal for all rats within each training session. On the day after the first 2 d of training with the visible platform (eight trials), each rat received a four-trial session in which the visible platform was replaced with the submerged platform at the same location. Identical training procedures were used during these hidden platform trials.

Devan and White • Striatal-Hippocampal Interactions

Subsequently, the 3 d sequence of two visible platform sessions followed by a hidden platform session was repeated two more times for a total of 36 trials (24 visible, 12 hidden) over a total of 9 d. Escape latency (time to reach the available platform) and peripheral pool time (time spent swimming within 20 cm of the edge of the pool) were measured on these acquisition trials. On day 10, the competition test was given. The visible platform was moved to the center of the southeast quadrant (opposite to its location during the acquisition trials). T wo trials were given from start positions equidistant to the center of the northwest and southeast quadrants. The video recordings were used to determine whether the rats swam within 5 cm of the perimeter of the former platform location in the northwest quadrant before escaping to the visible platform, now located in the southeast quadrant. Histolog y. After behavioral testing, the rats were deeply anesthetized with an intraperitoneal injection of 1 ml of 30% chloral hydrate and perf used intracardially with 0.9% saline followed by 10% formol saline. The brains were removed from the skulls and stored in 10% formol saline for at least 1 week. Frozen coronal sections were cut at 30 mm through the lesion area, and every fifth section was mounted on glass microscope slides. The mounted sections were stained with formol thionin (Donovick, 1974) and examined with a microscope. Statistical anal yses. T wo-way mixed ANOVAs with lesion type as the between-groups factor and trial block as the repeated-measures factor (with conservative dfs) were computed for the escape latency and peripheral pool time (thigmotaxis) measures of acquisition performance. Separate analyses were conducted for trial blocks with the visible and hidden platform. Significant interactions were followed up by one-way ANOVAs of the group factor at each level of the repeated-measures factor. This procedure is considered to be a conservative way to test simple main effects involving overall sources with different error terms (Olson, 1988, p. 723). If a one-way ANOVA yielded significance then Scheffe’s method was used to determine specific group differences. Oneway ANOVA followed by Scheffe’s post hoc test of lesion type were used to analyze latency and thigmotaxis scores on the competition test. In addition, likelihood ratio and Pearson’s x2 analyses of lesion type were conducted for the response category measure.

RESULTS Experiment 1: bilateral lesions of CPu subregions and fornix/fimbria Histology The minimal and maximal extent of damage to the structures lesioned in each group is shown in Figure 1. The dorsolateral CPu lesion primarily damaged the region of CPu underlying the corpus callosum and bordering the external capsule. At a posterior level, this lesion spared the more lateral portion of CPu adjacent to the external capsule. One animal in the dorsolateral CPu group sustained extensive damage to the medial CPu and lateral septum; data from this animal were not included in the final statistical analyses. The dorsomedial lesion was localized to the portion of CPu underlying the corpus callosum and bordering the lateral ventricles. This lesion did not produce any observable damage to the choroid plexus within the ventricles or to the lateral septum. The lesion also spared the nucleus accumbens anteriorly and ventromedial portions of CPu posteriorly. Fornix/fimbria lesions included damage to the medial and lateral portions of the structure. Sparing of the lateral–ventral tips was observed in some cases. Occasional damage was observed in the triangular septal nucleus and dorsal hippocampus, along with minimal electrode tract damage to overlying cortex.

Cue–place acquisition Figure 2 shows the escape latency data for each group. All groups learned to swim to the visible platform (connected symbols). However, rats with medial CPu lesions had longer latencies on the early trials. A two-way mixed ANOVA revealed a significant interaction between factors (F(3,41) 5 2.89; p , 0.05) as well as

Devan and White • Striatal-Hippocampal Interactions

J. Neurosci., April 1, 1999, 19(7):2789–2798 2791

Figure 1. Minimum (dark hatching) and maximum (light hatching) extent of damage to the fornix/fimbria, lateral C Pu, and medial CPu.

Figure 2. Mean escape latencies for each group in experiment 1. Connected symbols represent visible platform trial blocks (days 1–2, 4 –5, and 7– 8; four trials per block). Disconnected symbols represent hidden platform trial blocks (days 3, 6, and 9; four trials per block) and the competition test (day 10; two trials per block).

significant main effects of group (F(3,41) 5 4.93; p , 0.01) and trial block (F(1,41) 5 202.51; p , 0.001). Post hoc tests revealed a significant difference between the medial C Pu and sham group on trial block 2 ( p , 0.01). No other group differences were significant. Mean escape latencies on the hidden platform trials are also shown in Figure 2 (days 3, 6, and 9). Rats with medial C Pu lesions took longer than sham-lesioned rats to escape on the first hidden platform session. In contrast, rats with fornix /fimbria lesions took longer than shams to escape on the last hidden platform session. Analyses of the escape latency data for hidden platform trials showed a significant group by trial block interaction (F(3,41) 5 3.30; p , 0.05) as well as significant main effects of group (F(3,41) 5 3.79; p , 0.01) and trial block (F(1,41) 5 27.09; p , 0.001). Post hoc tests showed that the medial C Pu group differed significantly from both the sham and lateral groups on trial block 3 ( p values ,

Figure 3. Mean percentage of swim time spent in the peripheral portion of the pool for each group in experiment 1. Connected symbols represent visible platform trial blocks (days 1–2, 4 –5, and 7– 8; four trials per block). Disconnected symbols represent hidden platform trial blocks (days 3, 6, and 9; four trials per block) and the competition test (day 10; two trials per block).

0.05), whereas the fornix/fimbria group differed significantly from the sham and lateral CPu groups on trial block 9 ( p values , 0.01). Hence, lesions of the medial CPu retarded but did not prevent place learning, whereas lesions of the fornix/fimbria blocked place learning. Figure 3 shows the mean percentage of time spent in the peripheral part of the pool (thigmotaxis) for each group. Rats with medial CPu lesions tended to be more thigmotactic than the other groups. A two-way mixed ANOVA on the visible platform means revealed a significant group by trial block interaction (F(3,41) 5 2.79; p , 0.01) as well as significant main effects of group (F(3,41) 5 8.34; p , 0.001) and trial block (F(1,41) 5 92.61; p , 0.0001). Post hoc tests showed significant differences between the medial CPu and all other groups (sham: blocks 1, 2 and 7, p values , 0.05; lateral CPu: block 5, p , 0.05; fornix/fimbria:

Devan and White • Striatal-Hippocampal Interactions

2792 J. Neurosci., April 1, 1999, 19(7):2789–2798

Table 1. Number of rats that swam to the former platform location (place responders) versus the new visible platform position (cue responders) on the competition test (day 10)

Place responders Cue responders

Sham

Lateral C Pu

Medial C Pu

Fornix / Fimbria

6 4

13 1

0 11

1 9

blocks 7 and 8, p , 0.005). Analyses of the hidden platform means revealed a significant group by trial block interaction (F(3,41) 5 4.60; p . 0.05) in addition to significant main effects of group (F(3,41) 5 4.78; p , 0.01) and trial block (F(1,41) 5 52.70; p , 0.001). Post hoc tests showed significant differences between the medial C Pu and all other groups on trial block 3 ( p values , 0.05) but no group differences on trial blocks 6 or 9 ( p values . 0.10). Thus, the rats with medial C Pu lesions showed a greater tendency than the rats in the other groups to swim in the peripheral part of the pool on the early trials.

Competition test The mean escape latencies for each group on the competition test are shown in Figure 2 (day 10). Rats with fornix and medial CPu lesions escaped faster than controls and rats with lateral CPu lesions. A one-way ANOVA on the mean latencies averaged across the two competition test trials revealed a significant group effect (F(3,41) 5 12.38; p , 0.001). Post hoc tests showed that the fornix and medial C Pu groups had significantly longer latencies than the sham and lateral C Pu groups ( p values , 0.01). The mean percentage of time spent in the peripheral part of the pool on the competition test is shown in Figure 3 (day 10). Rats with medial C Pu lesions were more thigmotactic than the other groups. A one-way ANOVA revealed a significant group effect (F(3,41) 5 12.38; p , 0.001). Post hoc tests showed that the medial C Pu group differed significantly from the sham, lateral CPu, and fornix groups ( p values , 0.05). The rats’ responses on the competition test were categorized as cue or place by examining the swim paths. If an animal swam from the start point to within 5 cm of the perimeter of the former platform location in the northwest quadrant before escaping to the visible platform in the southeast quadrant on either competition test trial, it was labeled a place responder. If an animal did not meet this criterion, it was labeled a cue responder. The number of place and cue responders in each group is shown in Table 1. Compared to controls, the lateral C Pu group showed a significant place –response bias (x2 5 3.89; p , 0.05), whereas both the fornix and medial C Pu groups showed significant cue– response biases (x2 5 5.93, p , 0.05; x2 5 9.24, p , 0.01, respectively). Additional tests revealed that the lateral CPu group differed significantly from both the fornix (x2 5 16.47; p , 0.0001) and medial C Pu groups (x2 5 21.27; p , 0.0001) but that the fornix and medial C Pu groups did not differ significantly from each other (x2 5 1.15; p . 0.20). Figure 4 shows some representative swim paths of individual rats categorized as cue or place responders on the competition test. Escape latencies corresponding to each path are also shown. The differences in these latencies across response categories (cue or place) in the lesioned rats were minimal (Fig. 4: A vs B and C vs D) and not reliably predictable from response category (Fig. 4: E vs F). Hence, while latency provides an indication of the rats’ progress in acquiring the tasks early in training, it does not

Figure 4. Representative swim paths of cue and place responders on the first trial of the competition test. The visible platform is in the bottom left quadrant, and the location of the platform during the preceding training trials is in the top right quadrant. Escape latencies for the trials illustrated are shown in each case. The rats with medial caudate putamen lesions ( A, C) did not cross the former platform location, whereas the rats with lateral caudate putamen lesions (B, D) did. Differences between escape latencies ( A, B and C, D) were small (1.7 and 1.3 sec, respectively). As illustrated in E and F, the escape latencies of control rats classified as cue responders were not necessarily shorter than those classified as place responders, even if the distance they had to swim was shorter.

accurately reveal which of the two response tendencies predominate in a given rat, as revealed by the competition test.

Summary and discussion The results of this experiment reveal a dissociation between the effects of medial and lateral CPu lesions on the combined cue– place task in the water maze. Lateral CPu lesions did not affect acquisition rate but produced a significant place-response bias on the competition test. This finding is consistent with the results of a previous study (McDonald and White, 1994) and with the hypothesis that lesions of the lateral CPu impair learned responding to local cues (i.e., S–R learning). As shown both in the present study and by McDonald and White (1994), the deficit in responding to the local cue revealed by the rats’ responses on the competition test is not reflected in their latencies on the cued training trials. Moreover, the strength of the competition test finding may be influenced by the specific training parameters used. In a previous study (Devan et al., 1999),

Devan and White • Striatal-Hippocampal Interactions

J. Neurosci., April 1, 1999, 19(7):2789–2798 2793

Figure 5. Minimum (dark hatching) and maximum (light hatching) extent of damage to the structures lesioned unilaterally in experiment 2. Although the reconstructions are illustrated within a single hemisphere, the side of lesion was counterbalanced within each group: unilateral-medial C Pu, unilateral-hippocampus, and asymmetric (crossed-unilateral) lesion. Plates adapted from Swanson (1992).

rats with lateral C Pu lesions exhibited a weaker place –response bias and a more subtle cue deficit than those in the present study. However, the rats in the previous study were f ully trained on the place response before receiving any cue training whereas in the present study, and in the one by McDonald and White (1994), cue and place training were given concurrently. The 5 d delay imposed by intervening cue training in the Devan et al. (1999) study may have weakened spatial response tendencies resulting in more cue-directed responses on the competition test, thus masking the effect of lateral C Pu lesions as presently and previously (McDonald and White, 1994) shown after contemporaneous experience with both types of training. In contrast, lesions of the medial C Pu increased escape latencies and thigmotaxis during acquisition and produced a cue– response bias on the competition test. The latter result is consistent with other reported cue enhancement effects (Mikulas, 1966; Winocur, 1980; Mitchell et al., 1985; Mitchell and Hall, 1988; Devan et al., 1996) after C Pu lesions. The present observations are consistent with those of previous experiments in showing that thigmotaxis tends to occur during the early trials of escape training (Devan et al., 1996, 1999). Devan et al. (1996, 1999) suggested that this form of lesioninduced thigmotaxis may be caused by a transient deficit in initiating responses that move the animal away from the pool wall, toward a location defined by spatial cues. Such a difference could also be the source of the cue –response bias of these rats on the competition test. Hence, increased thigmotaxis and enhanced cue responding may both result from a common underlying cause, i.e., a weakened tendency to respond to spatial cues. Lesions of the fornix /fimbria also produced a cue –response bias in the present study, as previously shown by McDonald and

White (1994). This bias is thought to be a reflection of the failure of rats with these lesions to acquire the cognitive–spatial information required to perform the place response (O’Keefe and Nadel, 1978). Although lesions of both the medial CPu and fornix/fimbria decreased the rats’ tendency to swim to the hidden platform location on the competition test, they may have done so for different reasons. This suggestion is partly based on differences in the effects of the two lesions on the acquisition trials. Fornix/ fimbria lesions completely blocked place learning, whereas medial CPu lesions produced only transient increases in escape latencies, accompanied by thigmotaxis, on early cue and place acquisition trials. These findings are consistent with the hypothesis that the fornix/fimbria deficit is caused by impaired acquisition of cognitive–spatial information and that the medial CPu deficit may be caused by impaired acquisition of responses based on learned spatial information. The latter deficit is most clearly revealed in situations characterized by competing place- and cue-based response alternatives.

Experiment 2: unilateral and asymmetric lesions of medial CPu and hippocampus Histology The minimal and maximal extent of damage to the structure or structures lesioned in each group is shown in Figure 5. Unilateral lesions of the medial CPu were similar to the bilateral lesions in the previous experiment. Unilateral lesions of the hippocampus included extensive damage to the rostrodorsal and posterior parts of the structure but typically spared the ventral part at the temporal pole. The dentate gyrus and CA subfields (Ammon’s horn) were damaged at both dorsal and posterior sites. In addi-

Devan and White • Striatal-Hippocampal Interactions

2794 J. Neurosci., April 1, 1999, 19(7):2789–2798

Figure 6. Mean escape latencies for each group in experiment 2. Connected symbols represent visible platform trial blocks (days 1–2, 4 –5, and 7– 8; four trials per block). Disconnected symbols represent hidden platform trial blocks (days 3, 6, and 9; four trials per block) and the competition test (day 10; two trials per block). Inset graphs illustrate significant group differences on visible trial blocks on an expanded ordinate: inset 1 (blocks 4 and 5) is expanded 33; inset 2 (blocks 7 and 8) is expanded 53.

tion, the subiculum (including presubiculum and parasubiculum) was lesioned posteriorly with occasional damage extending more ventrally into the entorhinal cortex. As shown in Figure 5, lesions in the asymmetric group were similar to those in the corresponding unilateral lesion groups.

Cue–place acquisition Figure 6 shows the mean escape latencies for each group. All groups learned to swim to the visible platform, however, escape latencies on these trials were slightly elevated in the asymmetric lesion group (Fig. 6, insets). A two-way mixed ANOVA revealed a significant group by trial block interaction (F(3,40) 5 2.77; p , 0.05), as well as significant main effects of group (F(3,30) 5 12.94; p , 0.0001) and trial block (F(1,30) 5 247.67; p , 0.00001). Post hoc tests revealed that the asymmetric lesion group differed from the sham (blocks 2, 4, 5, 7, and 8; p values , 0.05), unilateralmedial C Pu (blocks 2, 5, 7, and 8; p values , 0.01), and unilateralhippocampal (blocks 5, 7, and 8; p values , 0.01) groups. Because the mean differences were on the order of 1 sec or less on trials blocks 5, 7, and 8, the f unctional implications of these effects may be of minor importance. The asymmetric lesion group also took longer than the shams to find the hidden platform (Fig. 6). A two-way mixed ANOVA revealed a significant effect of group (F(3,40) 5 7.07; p , 0.001) and trial block (F(1,40) 5 30.99; p , 0.00001) but no significant interaction between factors (F(3,40) 5 1.76; p . 0.10). Post hoc tests revealed that the asymmetric group differed from the sham group ( p , 0.001) but not from the unilateral-hippocampal or unilateral-medial C Pu groups ( p values . 0.05). Figure 7 shows the mean percentage of time spent in the peripheral part of the pool for each group. Rats with asymmetric lesions were more thigmotactic than controls. A two-way

Figure 7. Mean percentage of swim time spent in the peripheral portion of the pool for each group in experiment 2. Connected symbols represent visible platform trial blocks (days 1–2, 4 –5, and 7– 8; four trials per block). Disconnected symbols represent hidden platform trial blocks (days 3, 6 and 9; four trials per block) and the competition test (day 10; two trials per block).

ANOVA on the data for the visible platform sessions revealed a nonsignificant effect of group (F(3,40) 5 1.66; p . 0.10), a significant effect of trial block (F(1,40) 5 116.74; p , 0.00001), and a significant group by trial block interaction (F(3,40) 5 2.33; p , 0.05). Post hoc tests showed that the asymmetric group differed significantly from the sham group on trial block 2 ( p , 0.05). Analyses of hidden platform sessions showed a significant effect of trial block (F(1,40) 5 33.28; p , 0.00001) but no effect of group or interaction between factors (F values , 2.8; p values . 0.05).

Competition test The mean escape latencies for each group on the competition test are shown in Figure 6 (day 10), whereas the mean percentages of peripheral pool time are shown in Figure 7 (day 10). One-way ANOVAs computed for each measure failed to reveal any significant group differences (escape latency, F(3,40) 5 0.94, p . 0.40; peripheral pool time, F(3,40) 5 2.14, p . 0.10). The number of place and cue responders in each group, defined using the same criteria as in experiment 1, is shown in Table 2. The asymmetric lesion group showed a significant cue–response bias in comparison to controls (x2 5 4.44; p , 0.05). In contrast, the unilateral-medial CPu and unilateral-hippocampal groups did not differ from the sham or asymmetric lesion groups ( p values . 0.09).

Summary and discussion In this experiment, asymmetric lesions of the hippocampus and medial CPu impaired escape learning on both visible and hidden platform trials, increased thigmotaxis (peripheral pool time) on early trials, and produced a significant cue–response bias on the competition test. Unilateral lesions of the hippocampus or medial CPu had none of these effects. Hence, these findings show a synergistic effect when the two forms of unilateral damage were combined to produce asymmetric lesions.

Devan and White • Striatal-Hippocampal Interactions

J. Neurosci., April 1, 1999, 19(7):2789–2798 2795

Table 2. Number of rats that swam to the former platform location (place responders) versus the new visible platform position (cue responders) on the competition test (day 10)

Place responders Cue responders

Sham

Unilateral medial C Pu

Unilateral hippocampus

Asymmetric med. C Pu / Hippocampus

7 5

5 5

4 6

2 10

On the early acquisition trials, the effects of asymmetric lesions were similar to (but not the same as) those of bilateral medial CPu lesions in experiment 1; both lesion groups exhibited increased escape latencies and increased thigmotaxis, especially on the early trials. As already suggested, this pattern of effects may be caused by an impaired ability to learn to initiate the appropriate response of swimming to the platform location on the basis of cognitive – spatial information. On the later acquisition trials, the increased latencies of the asymmetric lesion group resembled those of the fornix/fimbria group in experiment 1, although the deficit was not as severe in the asymmetric as in the fornix /fimbria group (Figs. 2 and 5, compare block 9). Thus, whereas bilateral fornix /fimbria lesions appeared to prevent learning the location of the hidden platform almost completely, the asymmetric lesion produced only a partial deficit of this type. This is consistent with the hypothesis that medial C Pu lesions do not impair acquisition of the spatial information required to locate the hidden platform. On the competition test, the asymmetric lesioned rats exhibited a clear cue –response bias, resembling both the medial CPu and fornix /fimbria groups in experiment 1. Based on the hypothesized effects of the two lesions, this bias may have been caused by disruption of the flow of information between the two structures that is required to initiate a learned response to spatial-cognitive information in the presence of a competing local cue, the visible platform.

DISCUSSION The results of experiment 1 suggest that the medial and lateral subregions of the C Pu are f unctionally heterogeneous and may interact competitively at a behavioral level when inconsistent response alternatives are available. The findings also indicate that under similar behavioral conditions the hippocampal system may interact competitively with the lateral C Pu and cooperatively with the medial C Pu. In experiment 2, the effects of the asymmetric lesions suggest that this cooperative interaction may be based on serial information processing within a f unctional neural circuit that includes (but is not limited to) both hippocampus and medial C Pu. Previous experiments using different versions of the radial arm maze task (Packard et al., 1989; McDonald and White, 1993, 1995) showed that large lesions of the dorsal striatum (including parts of both medial and lateral subregions) produced different behavioral effects from lesions of the fornix /fimbria or hippocampus. It was suggested that the two structures may mediate different kinds of mnemonic f unctions that may compete for behavioral expression in certain situations. A similar conclusion was suggested by the results of a study of place versus response learning on a cross maze task (Packard and McGaugh, 1996) involving temporary inactivation of lateral C Pu or hippocampus. The present findings are consistent with this interpretation, but they suggest that it may apply only to the lateral C Pu. In contrast,

the medial CPu subregion of the dorsal striatum may interact cooperatively with the hippocampal system, mediating the behavioral expression of certain hippocampus-dependent mnemonic functions. Lesions of the medial CPu have been reported to produce impairments in acquisition and retention of both place and cue tasks in the water maze (Whishaw et al., 1987; Devan et al., 1996). The latter findings cannot be attributed to a selective deficit in cognitive–spatial learning because, as in the present study, cue navigation was also compromised to some extent, and thigmotaxis was present. The fact that impairments of both place and cue acquisition after medial CPu lesions are accompanied by transient increases in thigmotaxis (as also reported by Devan et al., 1996, 1999) suggests that the behavioral impairment may be related to a deficiency in learned response initiation, such as that previously shown to occur on simple reaction time tasks after medial but not lateral CPu lesions (Brown and Robbins, 1989; Hauber and Schmidt, 1994). Moreover, as shown in the acquisition trials of the present experiment, rats with medial CPu lesions can acquire information about spatial/place cues and express such information when it provides the only possible solution to a task. This spared function may be mediated by allocortical and/or neocortical output to the pyramidal motor system. As shown in the competition test, however, when an alternative solution involving local cues is available, rats with medial CPu damage tend to respond to those cues rather than to the cognitive–spatial information they have acquired (see also, Whishaw et al., 1987). Differences in the anatomical connections of the lateral and medial CPu are consistent with the proposed functional dissociation between these two parts of the dorsal striatum. The lateral CPu receives descending projections primarily from sensorimotor neocortex (McGeorge and Faull, 1989) and ascending dopaminergic input from the substantia nigra pars compacta (Heimer et al., 1995). These patterns of connectivity combined with the results of several studies involving lesions and post-training manipulation of memory consolidation functions (Packard and White, 1991; White and Viaud, 1991; McDonald and White, 1994; Packard and McGaugh, 1996) suggest that this striatal subregion may specialize in S–R habit formation (for review, see White, 1989, 1997). In contrast, the patterns of connectivity and the present lesion findings suggest that the medial CPu may contribute to the control of behavior by the cognitive–spatial functions of other structures afferent to it. The medial CPu receives input from several mesocortical and allocortical areas (see introductory remarks), including the hippocampal formation. In the present study, asymmetric lesions of the hippocampus and medial CPu produced effects similar to bilateral but not unilateral lesions of these structures. Because the asymmetric lesions did not affect direct interhemispheric connections through the commissures, it is likely that the lesion effects resulted specifically from the

Devan and White • Striatal-Hippocampal Interactions

2796 J. Neurosci., April 1, 1999, 19(7):2789–2798

disconnection of ipsilateral connections, that is, efferent hippocampal projections to medial C Pu on the hippocampus lesion side, and both hippocampal afferents and medial C Pu efferents on the medial C Pu lesion side. There are at least four possible f unctional links between the hippocampal formation and the dorsomedial striatum. First, although direct projections from the CA1 subfield of the hippocampus and subiculum project primarily to the ventral striatum via the fornix /fimbria (Heimer and Wilson, 1975; Groenewegen et al., 1987; Brog et al., 1993), Groenewegen et al. (1987) have shown that subicular afferents also reach the most medial, ventral, rostral and caudal parts of the C Pu. Second, the perirhinal cortex projects to the dorsal hippocampal CA1 field (Liu and Bilkey, 1996), to the entorhinal cortex (Amaral and Witter, 1995), and to the nucleus accumbens/medial C Pu (McGeorge and Faull, 1989; Vaudano et al., 1990; Burwell et al., 1995), and hippocampal output from the entorhinal cortex reaches the nucleus accumbens and medial C Pu (Krayniak et al., 1981; Sørensen and Witter, 1983; Swanson and Ko ¨hler, 1986). Third, hippocampal output may influence striatal f unction through connections with the posterior cingulate cortex, which projects strongly to the medial CPu (Domesick, 1969; McGeorge and Faull, 1989). Finally, the hippocampal formation projects to sectors of the medial prefrontal cortex (Swanson, 1981; Swanson and Ko ¨hler, 1986; Jay et al., 1989; Sesack et al., 1989; Jay and Witter, 1992; Conde´ et al., 1995), which in turn project to the ventral and dorsomedial striatum (Gerfen, 1984; McGeorge and Faull, 1989; Groenewegen et al., 1990). The possibility that spatial information may be conveyed to the medial C Pu via one or more of these indirect routes is supported by reports that bilateral lesions of the entorhinal /perirhinal cortex (Schenk and Morris, 1985; Nagahara et al., 1995), perforant path (Skelton and McNamara, 1992), posterior cingulate cortex (Sutherland et al., 1988; Sutherland and Hoesing, 1993), and medial prefrontal cortex (Sutherland et al., 1982; Kolb et al., 1994) all impair the ability to learn the location of a hidden platform in the water maze. Furthermore, Floresco et al. (1997) found that temporary asymmetric lesions of the ventral hippocampus and prelimbic cortex selectively disrupted performance of a delayed win-shift task involving spatial cues, but had no effect on a random foraging task. Temporary disconnection of the ventral hippocampus and nucleus accumbens had an opposite pattern of effects. Thus, different limbic circuits may mediate different kinds of functional interactions between the hippocampal formation and the striatum. In conclusion, the present results provide empirical evidence for a dissociation between medial and lateral subregions of the CPu and for different behavioral interactions between these CPu subregions and the hippocampal system. These interactions can be competitive or cooperative, depending on: (1) at a behavioral level, the type or types of information available in a particular learning task and (2) at a neural level, the subregion of dorsal striatum that is engaged by this information. The findings also suggest that the medial and lateral regions of the CPu can function in parallel and with some degree of independence, based on their f unctionally segregated corticostriatal connections. The lateral C Pu may contribute to S –R-based behavior and the medial C Pu, as part of a system that includes the fornix /fimbria and hippocampus, may contribute to behavior based on cognitive– spatial forms of information processing.

REFERENCES Alexander GE, DeL ong MR, Strick PL (1986) Parallel organization of f unctionally segregated circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex. Annu Rev Neurosci 9:357–381. Amaral DG, Witter M P (1995) Hippocampal formation. In: The rat nervous system, Ed 2 (Paxinos G, ed), pp 443– 493. San Diego: Academic. Brog JS, Salyapongse A, Deutch AY, Z ahm DS (1993) The patterns of afferent innervation of the core and shell in the “accumbens” part of the rat ventral striatum: immunohistochemical detection of retrogradely transported fluoro-gold. J Comp Neurol 338:255–278. Brown VJ, Robbins TW (1989) Elementary processes of response selection mediated by distinct regions of the striatum. J Neurosci 9:3760 –3765. Burwell RD, Witter M P, Amaral DG (1995) Perirhinal and postrhinal cortices of the rat: a review of the neuroanatomical literature and comparison with findings from the monkey brain. Hippocampus 5:390 – 408. Colombo PJ, Davis HP, Volpe BT (1989) Allocentric spatial and tactile memory impairments in rats with dorsal caudate lesions are affected by preoperative behavioral training. Behav Neurosci 103:1242–1250. Conde´ F, Maire-Lepoivre E, Audinat E, Cre´pel F (1995) Afferent connections of the medial frontal cortex of the rat. II. Cortical and subcortical afferents. J Comp Neurol 352:567–593. Devan BD, Goad EH, Petri HL (1996) Dissociation of hippocampal and striatal contributions to spatial navigation in the water maze. Neurobiol Learn Mem 66:305–323. Devan BD, McDonald RJ, White NM (1999) Effects of medial and lateral caudate-putamen lesions on place- and cue-guided behaviors in the water maze: relation to thigmotaxis. Behav Brain Res, in press. Divac I, Markowitsch HJ, Pritzel M (1978) Behavioral and anatomical consequences of small intrastriatal injections of kainic acid in the rat. Brain Res 151:523–532. Domesick V B (1969) Projections from the cingulate cortex in the rat. Brain Res 12:296 –320. Donovick PJ (1974) A metachromatic stain for neural tissue. Stain Technol 49:49 –51. Dunnett SB, Iversen SD (1981) Learning impairments following selective kainic acid-induced lesions within the neostriatum of rats. Behav Brain Res 2:189 –209. Dunnett SB, Iversen SD (1982a) Neurotoxic lesions of ventrolateral but not anteromedial neostriatum in rats impair differential reinforcement of low rates (DRL) performance. Behav Brain Res 6:213–226. Dunnett SB, Iversen SD (1982b) Sensorimotor impairments following localized kainic acid and 6-hydroxydopamine lesions of the neostriatum. Brain Res 248:121–127. Everitt BJ, Morris K A, O’Brien A, Robbins TW (1991) The basolateral amygdala-ventral striatal system and conditioned place preference: f urther evidence of limbic-striatal interactions underlying rewardrelated processes. Neuroscience 42:1–18. Floresco SB, Seamans JK , Phillips AG (1997) Selective roles for hippocampal, prefrontal cortical, and ventral striatal circuits in radial-arm maze tasks with or without a delay. J Neurosci 17:1880 –1890. Fricker R A, Annett L E, Torres EM, Dunnett SB (1996) The placement of a striatal ibotenic acid lesion affects skilled forelimb use and the direction of drug-induced rotation. Brain Res Bull 41:409 – 416. Furtado JC S, Mazurek M F (1996) Behavioral characterization of quinolinate-induced lesions of the medial striatum: relevance for Huntington’s disease. E xp Neurol 138:158 –168. Gaffan D, Harrison S (1987) Amygdalectomy and disconnection in visual learning for auditory secondary reinforcement by monkeys. J Neurosci 7:2285–2292. Gaffan D, Parker A (1996) Interaction of perirhinal cortex with the fornix-fimbria: memory for objects and “object-in-place” memory. J Neurosci 16:5864 –5869. Gaffan EA, Gaffan D, Harrison S (1988) Disconnection of the amygdala from visual association cortex impairs visual reward-association learning in monkeys. J Neurosci 8:3144 –3150. Gaffan D, Gaffan EA, Harrison S (1989) Visual-visual associative learning and reward-association learning: the role of the amygdala. J Neurosci 9:558 –564. Gerfen CR (1984) The neostriatal mosaic: compartmentalization of corticostriatal input and striatonigral output systems. Nature 311:461– 464. Graybiel AM (1990) Neurotransmitters and neuromodulators in the basal ganglia. Trends Neurosci 13:244 –254.

Devan and White • Striatal-Hippocampal Interactions

Groenewegen HJ, Berendse H W, Wolters JG, L ohman AHM (1990) The anatomical relationship of the prefrontal cortex with the striatopallidal system, the thalamus and the amygdala: evidence for a parallel organization. Prog Brain Res 85:95–118. Groenewegen HJ, Vermeulen-Van der Z ee E, te Kortxchot A, Witter MP (1987) Organization of the projections from the subiculum to the ventral striatum in the rat. A study using anterograde transport of Phaeseolus vulgaris leucoagglutinin. Neuroscience 23:103–120. Groves PM, Garcia-Munoz M, Linder JC, Manley MS, Martone M E, Young SJ (1995) Elements of the intrinsic organization and information processing in the neostriatum. In: Models of information processing in the basal ganglia (Houk JC, Davis JL, Beiser DG, eds) pp 51–96. Cambridge: MIT. Han JS, McMahan RW, Holland P, Gallagher M (1997) The role of an amygdalo-nigrostriatal pathway in associative learning. J Neurosci 17:3913–3919. Hauber W, Schmidt WJ (1993) Discrete quinolinic acid lesions of the lateral but not of the medial caudate-putamen reversed haloperidolinduced catalepsy in rats. J Neural Transm 94:103–114. Hauber W, Schmidt WJ (1994) Differential effects of lesions of the dorsomedial and dorsolateral caudate-putamen on reaction time performance in rats. Behav Brain Res 60:211–215. Heimer L, Wilson RD (1975) The subcortical projections of the allocortex: Similarities in the neural associations of the hippocampus, the piriform cortex, and the neocortex. In: Golgi centennial symposium: perspectives in neurobiology (Santini M, ed), pp 177–193. New York: Raven. Heimer L, Zahm DS, Alheid GF (1995) Basal ganglia. In: The rat nervous system, Ed 2 (Paxinos G, ed), pp 579 – 628. San Diego: Academic. Hull CL (1943) Principles of behavior. New York: Appleton-CenturyCrofts. Hirsh R (1974) The hippocampus and contextual retrieval of information from memory: a theory. Behav Biol 12:421– 444. Horel JA, Keating EG (1969) Partial K lu ¨ver-Bucy syndrome produced by cortical disconnection. Brain Res 16:281–284. Horel JA, Keating EG (1972) Recovery from a partial Klu ¨ver-Bucy syndrome in the monkey produced by disconnection. J Comp Physiol Psychol 79:105–114. Iversen SD (1984) Behavioral aspects of the corticosubcortical interaction with special reference to frontostriatal relations. In: Cortical integration (Reinoso-Sua´rez F, Ajmone-Marsan C, eds) pp 237–254. New York: Raven. Jay TM, Glowinski J, Thierry A-M (1989) Selectivity of the hippocampal projection to the prelimbic area of the prefrontal cortex in the rat. Brain Res 505:337–340. Jay TM, Witter MP (1992) Distribution of hippocampal CA1 and subicular efferents in the prefrontal cortex of the rat by means of anterograde transport of Phaseolus vulgaris-leucoagglutinin. J Comp Neurol 313:574 –586. Kolb B, Buhrmann K, McDonald RJ, Sutherland RJ (1994) Dissociation of the medial prefrontal, posterior parietal, and posterior temporal cortex for spatial navigation and recognition memory in the rat. C ereb Cortex 6:664 – 680. Krayniak PF, Meibach RC, Siegel A (1981) A projection from the entorhinal cortex to the nucleus accumbens in the rat. Brain Res 209:427– 431. Liu P, Bilkey DK (1996) Direct connection between perirhinal cortex and hippocampus is a major constituent of the lateral perforant path. Hippocampus 6:125–134. McDonald RJ, White NM (1993) A triple dissociation of memory systems: Hippocampus, amygdala, and dorsal striatum. Behav Neurosci 107:3–22. McDonald RJ, White NM (1994) Parallel information processing in the water maze: Evidence for independent memory systems involving dorsal striatum and hippocampus. Behav Neural Biol 61:260 –270. McDonald RJ, White NM (1995) Hippocampal and nonhippocampal contributions to place learning in rats. Behav Neurosci 109:579 –593. McGeorge AJ, Faull RL M (1989) The organization of the projection from the cerebral cortex to the striatum in the rat. Neuroscience 29:503–537. Mikulas WL (1966) Effects of lights at the choice point on spatial alternation and position learning by normal rats and rats with bilateral lesions of the caudate nucleus. Psychonom Sci 5:275–276. Milner B, Corkin S, Teuber H-L (1968) Further analysis of the hip-

J. Neurosci., April 1, 1999, 19(7):2789–2798 2797

pocampal amnesic syndrome: 14-year follow-up study of H.M. Neuropsychologia 6:215–234. Mishkin M (1958) Visual discrimination impairment after cutting cortical connections between the inferotemporal and striate areas in monkeys. Am Psychol 13:414. Mishkin M (1966) Visual mechanisms beyond the striate cortex. In: Frontiers in physiological psychology (Russell RW, ed), pp 93–119. New York: Academic. Mishkin M, Petri HL (1984) Memories and habits: some implications for the analysis of learning and retention. In: Neuropsychology of memory (Squire LR, Butters N, eds), pp 287–296. New York: Guilford. Mishkin M, Malamut B, Bachevalier J (1984) Memories and habits: two neural systems. In: Neurobiology of learning and memory (Lynch G, McGaugh JL, Weinberger NM, eds), pp 65–77. New York: Guilford. Mitchell JA, Hall G (1988) Learning in rats with caudate-putamen lesions: unimpaired classical conditioning and beneficial effects of redundant stimulus cues on instrumental and spatial learning deficits. Behav Neurosci 102:504 –514. Mitchell JA, Channell S, Hall G (1985) Response-reinforcer associations after caudate-putamen lesions in the rat: spatial discrimination and overshadowing-potentiation effects in instrumental learning. Behav Neurosci 99:1074 –1088. Morris RGM, Garrud P, Rawlins JN P, O’Keefe J (1982) Place navigation impaired in rats with hippocampal lesions. Nature 297:681– 683. Nagahara AH, Otto T, Gallagher M (1995) Entorhinal-perirhinal lesions impair performance of rats on two versions of place learning in the Morris water maze. Behav Neurosci 109:3–9. O’Keefe J, Nadel L (1978) The hippocampus as a cognitive map. Oxford: C larendon. Olson CL (1988) Statistics: making sense of data. Dubuque: Wm. C. Brown. Olton DS (1978) The f unction of septo-hippocampal connections in spatially organized behaviour. In: Functions of the septo-hippocampal system (C iba Foundation Symposium 58), pp 327–342. New York: Elsevier/ E xcerpta. Packard MG, McGaugh JL (1996) Inactivation of hippocampus or caudate nucleus with lidocaine differentially affects expression of place and response learning. Neurobiol Learn Mem 65:65–72. Packard MG, White NM (1991) Dissociation of hippocampus and caudate nucleus memory systems by posttraining intracerebral injection of dopamine agonists. Behav Neurosci 105:295–306. Packard MG, Hirsh R, White NM (1989) Differential effects of fornix and caudate nucleus lesions on two radial maze tasks: evidence for multiple memory systems. J Neurosci 9:1465–1472. Paxinos G, Watson C (1986) The rat brain in stereotaxic coordinates, Ed 2. San Diego: Academic. Petri HL, Mishkin M (1994) Behaviorism, cognitivism and the neuropsychology of memory. Am Scientist 82:30 –37. Pisa M, C yr J (1990) Regionally selective roles of the rat’s striatum in modality specific discrimination learning and forelimb reaching. Behav Brain Res 37:281–292. Reading PJ, Dunnett SB, Robbins TW (1991) Dissociable roles of the ventral, medial and lateral striatum on the acquisition and performance of a complex visual stimulus response habit. Behav Brain Res 45:147–161. Schenk F, Morris RGM (1985) Dissociation between components of spatial memory in rats after recovery from the effects of retrohippocampal lesions. E xp Brain Res 58:11–28. Sesack SR, Deutch AY, Roth RH, Bunney BS (1989) Topographical organization of the efferent projections of the medial prefrontal cortex in the rat: an anterograde tract-tracing study with Phaseolus vulgaris leucoagglutinin. J Comp Neurol 290:213–242. Skelton RW, McNamara RK (1992) Bilateral knife cuts to the perforant path disrupt spatial learning in the Morris water maze. Hippocampus 2:73– 80. Sørensen K E, Witter M P (1983) Entorhinal efferents reach the caudatoputamen. Neurosci Lett 35:259 –264. Sutherland RJ, Hoesing JM (1993) Posterior cingulate cortex and spatial memory: A microlimnology analysis. In: The neurobiology of the cingulate cortex and limbic thalamus: a comprehensive handbook (Vogt BA, Gabriel M, eds), pp 461– 477. Boston: Birkhauser. Sutherland RJ, Rodriguez AJ (1989) The role of the fornix/fimbria and some related subcortical structures in place learning and memory. Behav Brain Res 32:265–277.

2798 J. Neurosci., April 1, 1999, 19(7):2789–2798

Sutherland RJ, Kolb B, Whishaw IQ (1982) Spatial mapping: definitive disruption by hippocampal or medial frontal cortical damage in the rat. Neurosci Lett 31:271–276. Sutherland RJ, Whishaw IQ, Kolb B (1983) A behavioural analysis of spatial localization following electrolytic, kainate- or colchicineinduced damage to the hippocampal formation in the rat. Behav Brain Res 7:133–153. Sutherland RJ, Whishaw IQ, Kolb B (1988) Contributions of cingulate cortex to two forms of spatial learning and memory. J Neurosci 8:1863–1872. Swanson LW (1981) A direct projection from Ammon’s horn to prefrontal cortex in the rat. Brain Res 217:150 –154. Swanson LW (1992) Structure of the rat brain. Amsterdam: Elsevier. Swanson LW, Ko ¨hler C (1986) Anatomical evidence for direct projections from the entorhinal area to the entire cortical mantle in the rat. J Neurosci 6:3010 –3023. Tolman EC (1948) Cognitive maps in rats and men. Psychol Rev 55:189 –208. Thorndike EL (1911) Animal Intelligence. New York: Macmillan. Thorndike EL (1933) A proof of the law of effect. Science 17:173–175. Vaudano E, Legg CR, Glickstein M (1990) Afferent and efferent con-

Devan and White • Striatal-Hippocampal Interactions

nections of temporal association cortex in the rat: a horseradish peroxidase study. Eur J Neurosci 3:317–330. Viaud MD, White NM (1989) Dissociation of visual and olfactory conditioning in the neostriatum of rats. Behav Brain Res 32:31– 42. Whishaw IQ, Mittleman G, Bunch ST, Dunnett SB (1987) Impairments in the acquisition, retention and selection of spatial navigation strategies after medial caudate-putamen lesions in rats. Behav Brain Res 24:125–138. White NM (1989) A f unctional hypothesis concerning the striatal matrix and patches: mediation of S-R memory and reward. Life Sci 45:1943–1957. White NM (1997) Mnemonic f unctions of the basal ganglia. Curr Opin Neurobiol 7:164 –169. White NM, Viaud M (1991) L ocalized intracaudate dopamine D2 receptor activation during the post-training period improves memory for visual or olfactory conditioned emotional responses in rats. Behav Neural Biol 55:255–269. Winocur G (1974) Functional dissociation within the caudate nucleus of rats. J Comp Physiol Psychol 86:432– 439. Winocur G (1980) The hippocampus and cue utilization. Physiol Psychol 8:280 –288.

Suggest Documents