Public beliefs about and attitudes towards ... - Wiley Online Library

16 downloads 0 Views 178KB Size Report
and St John's wort. However, respondents' ..... Hall P, Brockington IF, Levings J, Murphy C. A compar- ... Ingamells S, Goodwin AM, John C. The influence of psy-.
Acta Psychiatr Scand 2006: 113: 163–179 All rights reserved DOI: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2005.00699.x

Copyright  2005 Blackwell Munksgaard ACTA PSYCHIATRICA SCANDINAVICA

Review article

Public beliefs about and attitudes towards people with mental illness: a review of population studies Angermeyer MC, Dietrich S. Public beliefs about and attitudes towards people with mental illness: a review of population studies. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2006: 113: 163–179.  2005 Blackwell Munksgaard. Objective: To provide a review of population-based attitude research in psychiatry during the past 15 years. Method: An electronic search using PubMed, Medline, and Academic Search Premier plus a hand search of the literature was carried out for studies on public beliefs about mental illness and attitudes towards the mentally ill published between 1990 and 2004. Results: Thirty-three national studies and 29 local and regional studies were identified, mostly from Europe. Although the majority are of descriptive nature, more recent publications include studies testing theory-based models of the stigmatization of mentally ill people, analyses of time trends and cross-cultural comparisons, and evaluations of antistigma interventions. Conclusion: Attitude research in psychiatry made considerable progress over the past 15 years. However, there is still much to be done to provide an empirical basis for evidence-based interventions to reduce misconceptions about mental illness and improve attitudes towards persons with mental illness.

M. C. Angermeyer, S. Dietrich Department of Psychiatry, University of Leipzig, Leipzig, Germany

Key words: attitudes; population; mental disorders Matthias C Angermeyer, Department of Psychiatry, University of Leipzig, Johannisallee 20, D-04317 Leipzig, Germany. E-mail: [email protected] Accepted for publication September 28, 2005

Summations

• In the time period since 1990, misconceptions about mental disorders still prevailed among the general public. • There are indications of inter-cultural variations of beliefs and attitudes as well as of changes over time. • There are marked differences between the various mental disorders with regard to beliefs and attitudes. Considerations

• Up until now, population-based attitude research in psychiatry has mainly been descriptive in nature. Other approaches, e.g. the testing of theory-based models, are still under-represented. • So far, attitude research was mainly focused on schizophrenia and depression, other mental disorders have been neglected. • There is only little known about the relation between attitudes towards people with mental disorders and the actual behaviour towards them.

163

Angermeyer and Dietrich Introduction

In 1989, a review article was published in this journal by Dinesh Bhugra on ÔAttitudes towards mental illnessÕ focusing on studies of public attitudes towards mental illness, mentally ill people and their treatment environs (1). Ever since then, in many countries reforms of mental health care have been initiated or have made further progress (2, 3). One aim of these reforms was to better integrate mentally ill people into society. However, in recent years there has been a growing awareness among mental health professionals that mental illness is still surrounded by stigma. According to Sartorius it is the most important obstacle to the provision of mental health care for people with mental disorders (4). Moreover, as Jorm (5) was able to show, the public’s Ômental health literacyÕ is still unsatisfactory and needs to be improved. Otherwise, this may hinder public acceptance of evidence-based mental health care. In the 1990s, several programs have been launched around the globe, aimed at either improving public knowledge about mental disorders or reducing the stigmatization and discrimination of those suffering from these disorders (e.g. 6–9). In view of these developments we thought that it is time to carry out a new literature review on public beliefs and attitudes, covering the time period between 1990 and 2004. Aims of the Study

In particular, addressed:

the

following

questions

are

i) To what extent are mental disorders recognized as such and what beliefs about the origin, treatment and course of mental disorders prevail among the general public? ii) What attitudes towards mentally ill people are prevalent among the general public? iii) How are beliefs and attitudes related to people’s socio-demographic characteristics and how are they influenced by familiarity with mental illness? iv) What are the results of studies testing hypotheses derived from modern stigma theory? v) Have there been any changes in attitudes and beliefs over the years? vi) Which results yield cross-cultural comparisons of beliefs about mental disorders and attitudes towards people with mental disorder? vii) What are the results of studies evaluating the effect of interventions aimed at improving the public’s knowledge and at reducing the stigma attached to mental disorders? 164

The answers to these questions may give an impression of the magnitude of the problem, give some insights into how stigma operates and provide some clues as to what strategies may be best suited to modify existing misconceptions about mental disorders. Material and methods

We electronically searched PubMed, Medline and Academic Search Premier for studies covering the time period from January 1990 to December 2004. The search was conducted by entering each of the following keywords (attitudes*), (public attitudes*), (general public*), (beliefs*), (lay beliefs*), (stigma*), and (social distance*) in combination with (mental illness*), (mental disorder*), (schizophrenia*), (depression*), (Alzheimer’s disease*), (anxiety disorder*), (OCD*), (alcoholism*), (drug addiction*), (dementia*), (eating disorder*) and (bipolar disorder*) into each of these databases. Following the electronic search, hand searches of the literature identified were undertaken in the form of citation chasing. From these primary and secondary references, those reports were included in our review that met the following criteria: first, the paper was written in English. Second, the focus of the study was on the general public. Studies investigating attitudes and beliefs of particular subgroups (e.g. consumers, carers, health professionals, university students, etc.) were excluded. Third, studies were based on random or quota samples drawn from the general population or from primary care services. Studies using convenience samples were not taken into consideration. Searches for articles in October, November and December issues (2004) of the main journals publishing relevant articles were performed to identify any research that had not yet been entered into any of the above databases. The following journals were searched: Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, British Journal of Psychiatry, Psychological Medicine, European Psychiatry, International Journal of Social Psychiatry, American Journal of Public Health, Social Science and Medicine, European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, and Schizophrenia Bulletin. All articles included into our review were systematically screened by both authors for information relevant to the questions posed in the Introduction. Results

The above search strategy yielded an output of 3651 articles. However, this output constitutes a gross

Public beliefs about and attitudes towards mentally ill total since a number of studies appeared more than once after the different keyword combinations were entered into each of the databases. Of the articles identified, 110 met the inclusion criteria. While between 1990 and 1992 only two relevant articles had been published in the following years, a steep linear increase can be observed (1993–1995: 11 articles, 1996–1998: 23 articles, 1999–2001: 32 articles), resulting in a maximum of 41 articles between 2002 and 2004. The articles referred to 62 studies. Differentiating between surveys on a national level and those on a regional or local level, the characteristics of the studies included are reported in Tables 1 and 2. The tables show that a total of 33 national surveys have been conducted in 14 different countries, with 23 of these having been carried out in Europe, six in America, and two each in Asia and Oceania. An additional stock of 29 local or regional surveys can be added to the collection, with 14 of them having been conducted in Europe, five each in America and in Asia, four in Oceania and one in Africa. The majority of the surveys (44) are based on random samples of the general population. The number one interview technique used was personal interviewing (38), followed by telephone interviews (13). Only five of the surveys were conducted by mail. Response rates for the personal interview surveys varied between 64.0 and 98.3%, between 44.0 and 75.4% for the telephone surveys and between 34.5 and 60.0% for the mail surveys. Sample sizes ranged from a minimum of 90 to a maximum of 7278 individuals. While depression (31 surveys) and schizophrenia (29 surveys) were in the lead of the illnesses addressed in these studies, alcoholism (8), anxiety disorders (7), dementia (6) and drug addiction (4) were studied comparatively rarely. One survey each paid attention to mania, obsessive–compulisive disorder, prescription drug dependency, eating disorders, mental retardation and childhood hyperactivity. Twenty-six studies dealt with mental illness in general. Thirty surveys used vignettes as a stimulus. The majority of surveys were focused on public beliefs about mental illnesses (51 surveys) and attitudes towards persons with mental illnesses (45 surveys). Only 11 surveys investigated attitudes towards psychiatric facilities. Articles reporting descriptive data on public beliefs about mental illness and attitudes towards people with mental illness made up the lion’s share (10–19, 21, 22, 26–32, 34, 36, 40–45, 47–55, 58–65, 67, 69, 73–86, 89–95, 98–109, 111–113). In contrast, articles devoted to testing of theory-based models of the stigmatization of mentally ill people (33, 35, 37–39, 72, 109) were rare. The same applies to the analysis of time trends (20, 24, 25, 58, 68, 87), cross-cultural comparisons of beliefs and attitudes

(23, 40, 42, 44, 46, 47, 49, 51, 53, 54, 66, 70, 71, 83, 105, 111) and the evaluation of interventions aimed at improving knowledge of mental disorders and reducing stigmatization of mentally ill people (56, 57, 83, 87, 96, 97, 114–116). In the following, the major findings of these various approaches of attitude research are presented. Descriptive studies on public beliefs about mental disorders

Studies on beliefs about mental disorders take an interest in the question as to what extent they are recognized as such and what beliefs about the causes, the course and the treatment of these illnesses are prevalent among the public. Several studies using case vignettes agree that many members of the lay public cannot correctly recognize mental disorders. Schizophrenic symptoms are more often seen as an expression of a mental illness (range 69–88%) than are depressive symptoms (26– 69%) or symptoms of alcoholism (16–49%) (23, 24, 34, 41, 50, 65, 66, 74, 80, 109). All studies using vignettes conclude that lay beliefs about the causes of mental disorders clearly differ from the results of psychiatric research in that psychosocial factors, particularly psychosocial stress, are predominating in comparison with biological factors. This holds even truer for depression than for schizophrenia (22, 23, 32, 34, 40, 41, 47, 65, 81, 93). If one concentrates on the studies using the same instrument for the assessment of causal beliefs (22, 23, 32, 34, 40) the following picture emerges: acute stress in the form of life events is the most frequently endorsed cause (schizophrenia: 72.5–87%, depression: 81–85.5%), followed by (chronic) stress in partnership and family (schizophrenia: 59–64%, depression: 70–74%), brain disease (schizophrenia: 48.5–71%, depression: 19– 59%) and heredity (schizophrenia: 39–67.5%, depression: 21–58%). Studies using diagnostic labels as stimulus come up with somewhat mixed results: while in the case of depression, psychosocial stress is again favoured over biological causes as explanation (57, 60, 87), in the case of schizophrenia the situation is different, with biological factors being as frequently endorsed as a cause (86) or even more frequently than psychosocial stress (22, 101–103). The results of studies assessing causal beliefs about mental illness in general are rather inconsistent in this regard (46, 62, 91, 95, 107, 119). As far as help-seeking beliefs are concerned, the results are inconsistent. This may, in part, have to do with the diversity of the care systems in the various countries. However, some general tendencies emerge. In the case of depression, the general practitioner is most frequently recommended as a 165

166

Luxembourg Norway Switzerland

Italy

Germany

Austria

Country

Random sample

Random sample Random sample

Random sample Quota sample

Random sample

Random sample Random sample

1990

1991 1991

1992 1992

1993

1993 2001

1992 1994 1998/1999

Quota sample Stratified random sample ? Random sample

Cluster sample§

Random sample

1990

2000/2001

Random sample

Quota sample

1998

1990

Quota sample

Sampling procedure

1991

Year

Age

15+ 15+ 18–84 16–76

18–70

18+ 18+

18+

18+ 16+

18+ 18+

18+

18+

18+

14+

15+

Table 1. Overview of national surveys published since 1990

Self-report questionnaire Telephone interview Telephone interview Personal interview Telephone interview

Personal interview Personal interview

Personal interview

Personal interview Personal interview

Personal interview Personal interview

Personal interview

Personal interview

Personal interview

Personal interview

Personal interview

Interview

– – ? 63

98.5

70.3* 65.1* à

73.0*/71.2à

70.1* à –

70.9* 67.8*

72.3*/67.4à

71.9*

68.9*





Response rate (%)

501 1063 697** 1737

714

2024* 5025* à

2143*/2094à

5125* à 2176* à

1912* 2030*

1022*/980à

2118*

2045*

1042

1443

Sample size

Mental illness Mental illness Schizophrenia Depression

Schizophrenia Depression Anxiety/Panic disorder Psychosis Dementia Schizophrenia Mental illness Schizophrenia Depression Mental illness Schizophrenia Depression Panic disorder with agoraphobia Schizophrenia Depression Alcoholism Schizophrenia Schizophrenia Depression Schizophrenia Paranoia Depression Drug addiction Alcoholism Addiction to prescribed drugs Mania Social phobia Alzheimer's disease Schizophrenia Depression Mental illness Schizophrenia Schizophrenia Depression Schizophrenia Depression Alcoholism Alzheimer's disease Drug abuse Mental illness Schizophrenia

Disorder

X

– Vignette 

– – Vignette 

Vignette–

X X X

X

X

– Vignette  Vignette  Label

X

X

Vignette 

Vignette  Vignette

Vignette  Vignette 

X

X

– Vignette 

Vignette 

X

X

Vignette

Vignette

Stimulus

Beliefs about mental illness

X

X

X X

X

X

X X

X

X

X X

X

X

Attitudes towards mentally ill people

X

X X

X

X

Attitudes towards psychiatric services

(43, 83) (45) (46) (47–55)

(41, 42)

(12, 14, 15) (20, 24, 25, 33, 40)

(12, 14–16, 26, 30, 32)

(12–15) (28, 29)

(12–15) (12–15)

(12–15, 17–19, 21–27)

(12–15, 18–20)

(12–17)

(11)

(10)

References

Angermeyer and Dietrich

Random sample Stratified random sampleàà

1998 1995

?

New Zealand

Random sample

Random sample Random sample Random sample 15–44

? 18–80 18–74

? ?

16+ ? 21+ 18+ 21+

15+ 15+ 15+ 15+ 16+

Age

interview interview interview review interview

Telephone interview

Telephone interview Telephone interview Personal interview

Personal interview Personal interview

Telephone interview Telephone interview Personal interview

Personal interview

Personal Personal Personal Personal Personal

Interview

65

59.6 65 85

76.4 64

– ? 44 63 76.1

– – – – 67

Response rate (%)

1017

1043 1273 2031

1387 135

1403 1000 1326 1507 1444

2009 2050 1946 1804 1737

Sample size Mental illness Depression Depression Depression Schizophrenia Schizophrenia Severe depression Alcohol addiction Drug addiction Panic disorder Eating disorder Dementia Depression Dementia Mental illness Mental illness Schizophrenia Depression Alcohol dependence Drug dependence Mental illness Mental illness Psychosis Depression Alcoholism Childhood Hyperactivity Mental illness Mental illness Schizophrenia Depression Mental illness Schizophrenia

Disorder

– Label

X

X X X

X X

– – Vignette

– – Vignette–  

X

X X X

X X X X

– Vignette  

Label Label

– Label Label Label Label Label

Stimulus

Beliefs about mental illness

X X

X X X

X X X

X

X X

Attitudes towards mentally ill people

X X

X

Attitudes towards psychiatric services

(83)

(75) (76) (77–82)

(73) (74)

(60) (61) (62) (63, 64) (65–72)

(56, 57) (57) (57) (58) (59)

References

*Old Laender.  DSM III-R. àNew Laender. §In 30 randomly selected geographical areas 25 persons were consecutively recruited in a randomly selected GP unit. –ICD-10. **Swiss–German and Swiss–French population only.   DSM IV. ààParticipants were drawn from the records of community health centres in each of the country's seven health regions. The various communities in the regions were selected to represent both urban and rural areas and to be geographically representative of the country's population.

Australia

1994 1996 1995

Hong Kong

Dominica

Quota sample Random sample Stratified random sample Random sample Random sample

1989 2002 1989 1990 1996

Ireland Canada US

Quota sample Quota sample Quota sample Quota sample Random sample

1991 1995 1997 1997 1998

UK

Sampling procedure

Year

Country

Table 1. (Continued)

Public beliefs about and attitudes towards mentally ill

167

168 Random sample Random sample Random sample Stratified random sample Quota sample

Random sample Random Random Random Random Stratified random sample Stratified random sample

2000 2001 1994

? 1996

?

1989

1993

? 2001 ? 1998/1999

? 1988

? ? ?

? 2000 2002

2002

Two boroughs in greater Athens/Greece Vizcaya/Spain Geneva/Switzerland

Northtown/UK

Malvern and Bromsgrove/UK

Herne Hilland and Streatham Hill/UK Two urban areas/UK Scotland/UK Co. Louth and Co. Meath/Ireland Calgary, Drumheller, Edmonton/Canada Qubec/Canada Delaware County/US

San Francisco/US Mexico City/Mexico 2 Kibbutzim/Israel

Istanbul/Turkey Sancakli/Turkey Novosibirsk/Russia

Ulaanbaatar/Mongolia

Random sample

18+

15+ 18+ 18+

20+

B) Total population Random sample Total population Random sample

55+ 18-60 20+

18+ ?

18+ 16+ 17–86 15+

?

15+

?

16–65 20–75

19–64

18+

16+

?

Age

Random sampleà Quota sample A) Random sample

sample sample sample sample

Quota sample

Quota sample

Random sample Random sample

? 2001

Sampling procedure Random sample

Year

1985

Helsinki, Seinjoki, Joensun, Pieksmki, Tampere, Finland Mannheim/Germany Berlin, Bonn, Dsseldorf, Essen, Cologne, Munich/Germany Nuremberg, Wrzburg/Germany

Region

Table 2. Overview of regional and local surveys published since 1990

Personal interview

Personal interview Personal interview Self-administered questionnaire Self-administered questionnaire Personal interview Personal interview Personal interview

? Postal survey

Postal survey Personal interview Personal interview Telephone interview

Personal interview

Personal interview

Personal interview

Personal interview ?

Telephone interview Telephone interview Personal interview

Self-administered questionnaire Telephone interview Telephone interview

Interview

95.2

98.3 84.9 74.5

97.8

? – 96.4

60.7 34.5

42.6 ? 77.5 71.9

70





? –

51.9 68.5 ?

? 75.4

69

Response rate (%)

952

707 208 745

90

209 800 108

1001 206

208 1061 155 1653

215

1987

154

400 324

1426 1508 360

82 7278

514

Sample size

Psychosis Depression Depression Schizophrenia Schizophrenia Depression Schizophrenia Depression

Schizophrenia Schizophrenia Depression GAD Dementia Mental illness Mental disorders

Mental illness Alcoholism Mental illness Schizophrenia

Depression Mental illness Schizophrenia Depression Panic attack Schizophrenia ÔPuerperal illnessÕ Mental illness Mental illness Schizophrenia Depression Mental illness

Depression Depression Mental illness

Mental illness Schizophrenia

Mental illness

Disorder

Vignette*

Vignette  Vignette  Vignette*

Vignette

Label – –

Label Vignette*

– Label

Vignette



– Vignette

Vignette

Label – Vignette

Label Label –

Label



Stimulus

X

X X X

X

X X X

X X

X

X

X

X

X

X X

X X

X

Beliefs about mental illness

X

X X X

X X

X X

X X

X

X

X X X

X

X

X X

X

Attitudes towards mentally ill people

X

X

X

Attitudes towards psychiatric services

(40, 110)

(108) (109) (40, 110)

(105) (106) (107)

(103) (104)

(98) (99) (100) (101, 102)

(94–97)

(91) (92, 93)

(89) (90)

(88)

(87)

(44) (85, 86)

(84)

References

Angermeyer and Dietrich

(117, 118) (119)

(114–116)

(112, 113)

(111)

X X X Vignette  – Depression Mental illness 3010 164 *DSM III-R.  DSM IV. àParticipants were recruited from four public sector primary-care clinics and were randomly selected from clinic logs.

70.2 55 Personal interview Postal survey Random sample Random sample 1998 1993

15+ ?

X Label 1094 60 Random sample ?

18–52

Postal survey

3109

Albury and surrounding rural area in New South Wales/Australia Canberra and south-east New South Wales/Australia South Australia Dunedin/New Zealand

Random sample

20–59

Postal survey

39

Depression

Vignette 

X

X X Vignette

Schizophrenia Depression Anxiety disorder Alcoholism Mental retardation Depression 450 ? Personal interview 17–70 Random sample ? BahirDar/Ethiopia

Sampling procedure Year Region

Table 2. (Continued)

Age

Interview

Response rate (%)

Sample size

Disorder

Stimulus

Beliefs about mental illness

Attitudes towards mentally ill people

Attitudes towards psychiatric services

References

Public beliefs about and attitudes towards mentally ill helping source (28–30, 52, 74, 78, 89, 118). In the case of schizophrenia, the public is more ready to recommend seeking help from a psychiatrist than in case of depression (28–30, 52, 58, 74). If mental disorders are recognized as such, the willingness to address a psychiatrist increases (52). All studies using case vignettes agree in that the public’s beliefs about psychological interventions (e.g. psychotherapy or counselling) are predominantly favourable, while very negative views prevail about pharmacological treatments (10, 19, 23, 24, 27–29, 41, 52, 78, 80, 82, 118). Surprisingly, with one exception (10), the particular liking of psychotherapy is more developed for schizophrenia than for depression (19, 27–29, 52, 78, 80, 82). For example, in a study from Australia (80) psychotherapy was considered by 55% of the respondents as being helpful for the treatment of schizophrenia and by 34% as being helpful for the treatment of depression while only 23% recommended antipsychotic medication and 29% antidepressants. Similar findings have been reported from a study in Switzerland using the same instrument (52). Similarly to causal beliefs, the results are somewhat different when explicit diagnostic labels are used. In the case of depression, again, psychosocial interventions are favoured over medication (57, 87), while in the case of schizophrenia, medication is more frequently considered as the appropriate form of treatment (58, 86, 101). In a survey in the US, most respondents agreed that psychiatric medications are effective, and fewer than half had concerns regarding potential problems. However, the majority of the respondents would not be willing to take them (72). In general, in the absence of a treatment, the course of mental disorders is seen rather pessimistically. The deterioration of the mentally ill person’s state is expected to be severer for schizophrenia than for depression. Conversely, on condition that an appropriate treatment is provided, the prognosis for both disorders is assessed quite optimistically (10, 23, 34, 58, 67, 78). No consistent relationships were found between gender and age of respondents on the one hand, and their illness beliefs on the other. Respondents with a higher educational level less frequently tended to make the person afflicted responsible for his or her illness and were more willing to recommend psychosocial interventions for treatment (23, 47, 69, 81, 111). Descriptive studies on attitudes towards people with mental disorders

People with mental disorders are experienced by the majority of the public to be in need of help and 169

Angermeyer and Dietrich dependent on others (20, 34, 36). Among the negative attributes attached to people with mental disorders the most prevalent one appears to be that they are unpredictable. This holds true more for people with schizophrenia (54–85%) or alcoholism (71%) than for people with depression (28–56%) or anxiety disorders (50%) (13, 34, 35, 41, 59, 104). Less frequently, persons with mental disorders are considered as violent and dangerous. Here, a similar picture emerges as with unpredictability. In particular, people with schizophrenia (18–71%) and alcoholism (65–71%) are seen as dangerous. To a lesser extent, this also applies to people with depression (14–33%) and anxiety disorders (26%) (13, 34, 35, 59, 62, 65, 66, 85, 101). The majority of the public show pro-social reactions, i.e. they feel sorry for persons with mental illnesses and they also feel the need to help them. This is particularly true for depression. Next come feelings of uneasiness, uncertainty and fear, reactions which are mostly set off by persons with schizophrenia. Infrequently, aggressive reactions occur (20, 34). There is an observable tendency towards distancing from persons with mental illness. With increasing intimacy of social relationships increases the desire for social distance. Rejection is most pronounced towards persons with drug abuse and alcoholism, followed by those with schizophrenia and is less pronounced towards people with depression and anxiety disorders (10, 15, 34, 51, 65, 67, 104, 111). All vignette studies examining the effect of subject-generated mental illness labels on public attitudes show that labelling leads to more rejection and other adverse reactions (20, 33, 41, 51, 66, 98, 109). Attitudes towards people with mental illness vary to a small extent only depending on sociodemographic characteristics. For example, in a German study, gender, age, education and residency accounted for only 1.4% of the variance (36). As concerns gender, the results are quite inconsistent. In the majority of cases (18 times) where the influence of gender on attitudes was examined no association between the two was observed (12, 15, 20, 40, 53, 54, 60, 66, 69, 88, 107, 109, 119). In 11 instances men expressed more negative attitudes than women (17, 34, 51, 54, 88, 119), in six instances the opposite was found (17, 34, 75, 76, 86). In most instances (32 times), negative attitudes were positively associated with age (12, 15, 20, 40, 41, 51, 54, 60, 66, 67, 69, 84, 88, 92, 93, 107, 109, 119), while the reverse relationship was reported only once (53). To what extent this may be due to either ageing or cohort effects remains an open question. In 10 instances, age did not matter (34, 66, 67, 88, 94, 100, 109, 119). In 20 170

of 38 observations a positive relationship showed between educational level and attitudes: persons with a higher educational level tended to distance themselves less from the mentally ill and expressed more liberal views (20, 34, 41, 53, 54, 67, 75, 76, 84, 88, 92–94, 107, 111, 119). In 18 cases, no relationship with education was reported (12, 15, 34, 40, 66, 88, 94, 100, 109, 119). The few studies dealing with urban–rural differences come up with quite contradictory results. While two studies found a stronger desire for social distance among rural residents (101), another study found the opposite (66). While in one study the perception of mentally ill people as being unpredictable was more pronounced among rural residents (42), in two other studies no urban–rural differences were found with regard to perceived dangerousness (63, 67). In 61 cases the relationship between familiarity with mental illness, i.e. having personally experienced a mental illness or having personal contact with people suffering from mental illness, and attitudes was examined. Thirty times it was reported that people had more positive attitudes if they were familiar with mental illness (15, 51, 54, 58, 59, 60, 64, 68, 71, 72, 76, 77, 84, 92, 93, 98, 119). The opposite was found in only one instance (59). In 30 cases no association between familiarity and attitudes was reported (16, 48, 54, 71, 77, 94, 98, 100, 101, 107, 119). Studies testing theory-based models of the stigmatization of mentally ill people

Two recently developed conceptualizations of stigma served as the main theoretical framework for empirical studies, namely Link and Phelan’s (120) sociological concept of the Ôstigma processÕ and Corrigan and Watson’s (121) social–psychological concept of the Ôpublic stigmaÕ. According to Link and Phelan (120), the stigma process sets off by recognizing and labelling a difference between a person and other people. The next step involves the linking of the labelled person with the negative stereotypes that predominate in society about this group of people. This way, the person now belongs to a distinct category of people from whom the beholder dissociates. The stigma process culminates in that the person concerned is exposed to different forms of discrimination and the negative social consequences resulting from this. The authors differentiate between individual and structural discrimination as well as self-stigmatization. The first refers to the behaviour of individuals that is directed straight against the members of a stigmatized group. The most frequently applied measure of individual

Public beliefs about and attitudes towards mentally ill discrimination is the desire for social distance (122). Structural discrimination describes the negative consequences that result from the imbalances and injustice inherent in social structures, political decisions and legal regulations for the members of a stigmatized group. The third form of discrimination (self-stigmatization) comes from within the mentally ill persons themselves in that they adopt the stereotypes prevailing in society about people with mental illness. The link between the two components of the stigma process, stereotype and discrimination, has been investigated in the meantime, with the interest lying on the impact of the different aspects of the stereotype of the mentally ill on individual discrimination and on the acceptance of structural discrimination of people with schizophrenia. Of the different stereotype components, the idea of unpredictability and dangerousness had the strongest impact on the social distance desired towards a person with schizophrenia: the more this view was adopted, the higher was the desire for social distance. The converse applies to the idea that people with schizophrenia were particularly intelligent and gifted. The acceptance of structural discrimination, on the contrary, was mainly influenced by the belief that those affected are to blame for their own illness: the more distinct the inclination to blame the ill, the higher the tendency towards approving of structural discriminations (35). Of particular interest for the structural discrimination of mentally ill persons is the result of a survey in Germany, which investigated the public’s preferences when it comes to the allocation of financial resources to health care and medical research. While somatic illnesses like cancer or cardiovascular diseases ranked first, depression, schizophrenia and alcoholism were the taillights (38, 39). Findings similar to what has been found in Germany are reported from the US: respondents who labelled persons with mental disorders as suffering from mental illness expressed a preference for greater social distance; and the degree of dangerousness that the public ascribed to these persons appeared to mediate the influence of the labelling effect (66). According to Corrigan and Watson (121), public stigma denotes the reaction of the general public to a certain group of individuals based on the stigma that is attached to this group. It comprises three components: stereotype, prejudice and discrimination. Stereotypes are collectively shared beliefs about a group of individuals. Most of the people are familiar with these stereotypes, however, not everybody adopts them. If they are adopted, prejudices develop, which result in negative emo-

tional reactions and from these, in turn, discriminatory behaviour may develop. Using data from a study conducted in Germany, this model was applied to investigate the effect of labelling of schizophrenia and depression as mental illnesses. In the case of schizophrenia, this kind of labelling elicited the belief that those affected with this illness are dangerous and unpredictable. Consequences were negative emotional reactions, like increasing fear and aggression. These resulted in an increasing desire for social distance. No such associations could be demonstrated for depressive disorders (33). Furthermore, the model was used to study the effect of familiarity with mental illness on the attitudes towards people with schizophrenia and depression. It could be shown that with growing familiarity, the tendency towards considering the ill person to be dangerous and unpredictable was decreasing, and people had less fear and social distance was desired less frequently (37). Analysis of time trends in beliefs about mental illnesses and attitudes towards people with mental illness

For identifying developmental trends in patterns of beliefs and attitudes over time, results of crosssectional surveys, which were conducted at different points in time, were compared with each other. Unfortunately, there are only a few of these studies and they came up with rather contradictory results. The study covering the longest period of time was undertaken in the US. Phelan et al. (68) compared the data of a survey dating from 1996 and those taken from a study conducted by Star back in 1950. They found that while today, the image of mental disorders is somewhat more differentiated among the American public, mentally ill individuals are nowadays more frequently considered to be dangerous as compared to the 1950s. Covering a much shorter time period (only 2 years), a study from Hong Kong came up with similar results: while the public’s knowledge of mental illness had slightly improved, their attitudes towards mental patients had become slightly more negative (76). In contrast, Madianos et al. (88) reported that the residents of two boroughs in greater Athens expressed more positive attitudes towards the mentally ill in 1994 than back in 1979/1980. According to the authors, the results could be explained in the context of a positive and tolerant social climate in the Athens area, strengthened by the implementation of local community mental health intervention programmes. Finally, a comparison of data from two surveys conducted in West German states in 1990 and 2001 revealed that the public’s emotional reactions and their desire for 171

Angermeyer and Dietrich social distance towards persons with major depression had by and large remained unchanged (20). Psychotropic drugs were assessed more favourably by the German public in 2001 than 11 years ago (25). Based on a series of surveys conducted in the years 1990–1992, it could be shown that there was a marked increase of the preference for social distance from people with schizophrenia immediately following violent attacks by two individuals suffering from schizophrenia against prominent German politicians (12–14). Cross-cultural comparisons of beliefs and attitudes

A distinction can be made here between surveys which investigate variations in the pattern of beliefs and attitudes within a single country and others, which investigate variations between different countries. Differences in beliefs and attitudes have been studied in five countries. A comparison between the old West German states and the New Laender (former German Democratic Republic, GDR) in 1990, i.e. shortly after the German reunification, showed that lay beliefs about schizophrenia were almost identical. However, the tendency to define depressive symptoms as an illness and to recommend psychiatric treatment was less pronounced in the former GDR (23). Eleven years later these differences had diminished and the beliefs of the East German population had become similar to those of the West German population (24). Surveys from Switzerland show that, as compared with French-speaking Swiss, there was a marked reluctance of the German-speaking Swiss to resort to specialist help, particularly to mental health professionals, in case of mental illness (46). Another comparison of the three language regions in Switzerland revealed that people living in the Italian-speaking part expressed a stronger desire for social distance than people living in the German- and French-speaking parts (51). People from the French- and Italian-speaking parts were more willing to accept restrictive measures against persons with mental illness than those from the German-speaking part of Switzerland (54). People living in the French-speaking part were also more in favour of compulsory admissions (53). NonSwiss residents held more positive attitudes towards volunteering in psychiatry, i.e. they were more willing to regularly visit a long-term patient in a psychiatric institution, to help in a club for ex-patients, or to look after a mentally ill person not belonging to the household (49). A study from Italy reported that people living in southern Italy were less ready to acknowledge 172

patientsÕ social competence and civil rights than those from other parts of the country (42). A survey in the US yielded that people from the South of the US tended to endorse more frequently that the person’s bad character or stresses in life are responsible for the occurrence of mental disorders (70). Their preferences for social distance from persons with mental disorders were the same as in the rest of the US (66). Asian and Hispanic respondents perceived mental patients as significantly more dangerous than did white respondents (71). African Americans were more likely than whites to reject the idea that mental illnesses are caused by either genetics or an unhealthy family upbringing. They also tended to have more negative attitudes than whites towards professional mental health treatment (70). The results of a study among older adults recruited from primarycare clinics in San Fransisco indicated that Anglo older adults are significantly more knowledgable about Alzheimer’s disease than African American, Asian, and Latino older adults (105). In New Zealand, as compared to the general population the awareness of mental illnesses and the number of mental illnesses recalled was lower among Maori and much lower among pacific people. While among Maori the acceptance of people with mental illness in general or schizophrenia in particular was similar to the general population it was lower among pacific people (83). International comparisons are made possible by surveys conducted in different countries using the same methodology. Unfortunately, up until now results from only two representative studies among the general population addressing the question of inter-country differences with regard to beliefs and attitudes have been published. A comparison between Novosibirsk (Russia), Ulaanbaatar (Mongolia) and Germany revealed that people from the former two cities show a stronger tendency towards attributing the cause of mental illness to the afflicted individual (40). The same tendency has been reported from another non-western culture (109). As already outlined earlier, there was a close association for schizophrenia between labelling as mental illness and the stereotype of dangerousness in Germany. This association was neither found in Novosibirsk nor in Ulaanbaatar (110). In another study the attitudes towards the mentally ill among the residents of two Central European regions which were at considerably different stages of development in moving towards community-based care were compared. People living in the community care area (a district of the city of Mannheim in Germany) showed slightly more rejection. However, they also took a more

Public beliefs about and attitudes towards mentally ill rational and sophisticated position towards the mentally ill. People living in the custodially oriented catchment area (Grand Duchy of Luxembourg), on the contrary, showed a more vague pattern in their attitudes (44). Evaluation of interventions aimed at improving knowledge of mental illnesses and reducing stigmatization of mentally ill people

Six studies are available in which population surveys have been used to evaluate interventions aimed at transferring knowledge or reducing stigma. Four studies were focused on depression. Paykel et al. (57) evaluated the ÔDefeat Depression CampaignÕ, an activity of the Royal College of Psychiatrists in association with the Royal College of General Practitioners between 1992 and 1996 in the UK (56). Surveys of public attitudes and knowledge were conducted in late 1991, early 1995 and mid-1997. The share of those subjects who saw depression as a medical illness increased significantly. In addition, the acceptance of antidepressants increased, with more respondents regarding them as effective in 1997. In contrast, the proportion of those regarding antidepressants as addictive remained almost unchanged: threefourth of respondents agreed to this view. Hegerl et al. (87) evaluated the effect of the ÔNuremberg Alliance Against DepressionÕ, a campaign aimed at informing the public about the causes and treatment of depression. Prior to the beginning of the campaign and 1 year later, telephone surveys were conducted with a random sample in the cities of Nuremberg and Wurzburg, the latter functioning as the control region. As concerns illness beliefs, the result was rather sobering. Although the share of respondents making a lack of self-control responsible for the disorder had decreased in Nuremberg more clearly than it had in Wurzburg, fewer respondents from both cities agreed to the view that a dysfunction of the brain was the cause. RespondentsÕ opinion in terms of using antidepressants for the treatment remained almost unchanged, the same applies to respondentsÕ opinion as to the negative effects of antidepressants. Jorm et al. (114) evaluated the effect of a consumer guide called ÔHelp for Depression: What Works (and What Doesn’t)Õ, containing evidencebased information about treatments for depression. A pretest, which included a scale assessing depression, was mailed as a screening test for depression to a random sample of Canberra citizens and citizens from the area of south-east New South Wales. The mail package also contained questions

asking about attitudes to different health professionals and treatments. The consumer guide was sent to half of a subsample that scored above a set cut-off point on the depression scale and indicated interest in participating in the study. The remaining half of the sample received a brochure providing basic information about depression. Six months later, a second postal survey was conducted. Those who had received the consumer guide more often rated cognitive-behaviour therapy to be helpful. The same was true for electroconvulsive therapy and St John’s wort. However, respondentsÔ views on antidepressants or interpersonal psychotherapy remained unchanged. In another study conducted in the same area, the effect of two internet depression sites on stigma have been investigated. A total of 525 individuals with elevated scores on the depression assessment scale were randomly allocated to a depression information website, a cognitive-behavioural skills training website or an attention control condition. Personal stigmatizing attitudes to depression and the perception of what most people believe about people with mental illness were assessed before and after the intervention. Relative to the control, the internet sites significantly reduced personal stigmatizing attributes, although the effects were small. While informing the individuals had no effect on the perceptions of stigma; the cognitive-behavioural component was associated with an increase in perceived stigma relative to the control (115, 116). In order to evaluate a national media campaign to counter stigma and discrimination associated with mental illness that has been launched in New Zealand, a series of four surveys had been conducted. It could be shown that advertizing has a significant impact on the public’s attitudes and knowledge. There was a significant increase in reported acceptance of people with experience of mental illness while the attitude towards people with schizophrenia remained virtually unchanged. As compared with the general population, Maori and pacific people did not show the same level of improvement in attitudes (83). Prior to the opening of two staffed group homes for severely mentally ill people, situated in the London borough of Lambeth, immediate neighbours living on the same street as these facilities were interviewed. In one area, an educational campaign was conducted comprising a primarily didactic component (an information pack containing a video and information sheets), a primarily social component (social events and social overtures from staff) and a mixed component (a formal reception and informal discussion sessions). One 173

Angermeyer and Dietrich year after the start of the educational campaign the survey was repeated in both areas. There was no significant difference between increase in knowledge in the two areas over time. However, there was an overall decrease in the fear and exclusion dimension of the CAMI in the experimental area at follow-up compared to the control area. The acceptance of patients increased in the experimental area (96, 97).

vi)

Discussion

The main findings of the last 15 years of attitude research in psychiatry may be summarized as follows: i) A substantial part of the public cannot recognize specific mental disorders. When confronted with pathological behaviour, as depicted in a vignette, people most frequently tend to adopt psychosocial stress as causal explanation and to recommend psychological interventions for treatment. If diagnostic labels are being used in case of depression the same is found while in case of schizophrenia biological explanations are as frequently or even more frequently endorsed and psychotropic medication recommended. ii) The majority of the public consider people with mental disorders as in need of help and show pro-social reactions. However, a substantial part perceives them as unpredictable and dangerous and reacts with fear. There is an observable tendency to distance oneself from people with mental disorders. iii) Although there are various similarities between mental disorders as concerns public beliefs and attitudes, there are also marked differences. For instance, people with schizophrenia or alcoholism are more frequently considered as unpredictable and violent than people with depression and anxiety disorders. Rejection is most pronounced towards people with drug abuse and alcoholism, followed by those with schizophrenia, and is less pronounced towards people with depression and anxiety disorders. iv) While the association between gender and attitudes is inconsistent, there exists strong evidence that negative attitudes are positively associated with age and negatively with educational attainment. However, the explanatory power of socio-demographic characteristics is only poor. v) If there is any relationship there is a positive association between familiarity with mental 174

vii)

viii)

ix)

illness and the acceptance of people with mental disorders. Consequently, to facilitate the contact with people with mental disorders may prove effective in reducing negative attitudes. Models derived from modern stigma theories allow to systematically investigate the relations between the various stigma components as, for instance, the effect of labeling on attitudes or that of the different aspects of the stereotype of the mentally ill on social distance or on the acceptance of structural discrimination. Up until now, too few trend analyses have been carried out to allow firm conclusions to be drawn as concerns the development of beliefs and attitudes over time. While there are some indications that the gap between public beliefs and scientific evidence has become narrower, attitudes do not show a consistent trend in one direction or the other. Comparisons between various regions or ethnic groups within countries as well as comparisons between countries reveal considerable differences in beliefs about mental disorders and in attitudes towards people with mental illness. There are some indications that people from non-western ethnic groups are less aware of and knowledgeable about mental illness and tend to attribute the cause of the illness more frequently to the afflicted individual. The evaluation of interventions aimed at improving the public’s knowledge of depression, using population surveys, suggests that this may be achieved in some areas while others, particularly people’s views on medication, may prove more resistant to change. Some findings suggest that informing people may not necessarily affect their attitudes.

From our review the following conclusions for future research may be drawn: i) In recent years, attitude research in psychiatry has made considerable progress. However, the vast majority of studies are still dedicated to the descriptive recording of public beliefs about mental illness and public attitudes towards persons with mental illnesses. There is a pressing need for more studies using more sophisticated approaches, like the testing of theory-based models, cross-cultural comparisons and trend analyses. They only may provide us with insights into the mechanisms of and contextual influences on stigma that are needed for successful antistigma interventions.

Public beliefs about and attitudes towards mentally ill ii) The vast majority of surveys covered by this review have been conducted in Western countries and only nine out of 61 originate from the rest of the world. There is a need for more transcultural studies using representative samples [instead of convenience samples as have been used in a number of previous studies (e.g. 123, 124)]. iii) Current attitude research mainly took an interest in schizophrenia and depression. Only a few studies have looked into alcoholism, drug abuse, anxiety disorders and dementia. Other disorders such as bipolar disorder have not been the focus of population studies yet. Strictly speaking, the titles of the majority of papers alluding to Ômental disordersÕ or Ômental illnessÕ in general (e.g. 10, 15, 16, 23, 26) are somewhat misleading. iv) Moreover, population surveys have thus far focused almost exclusively on attitudes towards mentally ill persons. With the exception of a few studies only (65, 67, 98), comparisons with people affected with somatic illnesses or mentally healthy people (Ônormal personsÕ) have not been carried out. Thus, it remains largely unknown to what extent the public’s beliefs about and attitudes towards mental disorders differ from the beliefs about and attitudes towards other social groups. v) In order to avoid the overgeneralization of research findings, a clear distinction between beliefs and attitudes related to pathological behaviour (as assessed by means of vignettes) and those related to diagnostic labels (for instance ÔschizophreniaÕ or ÔdepressionÕ) seems necessary. vi) The assessment of beliefs and attitudes about mentally ill people in general appears of limited value in view of the marked differences between the various mental disorders. vii) An important limitation of the research reviewed in this paper, with very few exceptions (96, 97, 112, 113), is that the relationship between attitudes and behaviour has not yet been examined. Thus, it remains an open question as to what extent beliefs about mental illnesses and attitudes towards mentally ill people are in fact related to actual behaviour. More research is needed to shed light on this relationship; however, it must be acknowledged that this is a very difficult area of research. viii) Although some of the mechanisms which form the basis of discrimination against persons with mental illnesses have been subjected to a closer analysis, and with evaluations of edu-

cational programmes having yielded some useful suggestions for the planning of interventions aimed at reducing the stigma attached to mental illness, we are still far from evidence-based antistigma programmes. This is not the least due to the fact that popular strategies for combating mental illness stigma have received contradictory evaluations, resulting in deep disagreement as to their effectiveness in actually reducing stigma and discrimination (125). For instance, while some studies (66, 101) are supportive of the mental illness is a brain disease strategy, other studies (36, 40, 51) found evidence that promulgating biological concepts among the public might not contribute to a desired reduction in social distance towards people with mental disorders. References 1. Bhugra D. Attitudes towards mental illness. A review of the literature. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1989;80:1–12. 2. Becker T, Vazquez-Barquero JL. The European perspective of psychiatric reform. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2001;104(suppl. 410):8–14. 3. Goodwin S. Comparative mental health policy. London: Sage, 1997. 4. Sartorius N. Fighting stigma: theory and practice. World Psychiatry 2002;1:26–27. 5. Jorm AF. Mental health literacy. Public knowledge and beliefs about mental disorders. Br J Psychiatry 2000;177:396–401. 6. Rutz W. Seelische Gesundheit, Stigma und Ausgrenzung aus europa¨ischer Perspektive. Die Destigmatisierungsprogramme der Weltgesundheitsorganisation (WHO). In: Gaebel W, Mo¨ller H-J, Ro¨ssler W, eds. Stigma – Diskriminierung – Bewa¨ltigung. Stuttgart: Kohlhammer, 2005. 7. Lopez-Ibor JJ Jr. The WPA and the fight against stigma because of mental illness. World Psychiatry 2002;1:30–31. 8. Crisp AH, ed. Every family in the land. London: Royal Society of Medicine Press, 2004. 9. Rosen A, Walter G, Casey D, Hocking B. Combating psychiatric stigma: an overview of contemporary initiatives. Aust Psychiatry 2000;8:19–26. 10. Jorm AF, Angermeyer MC, Katschnig H. Public knowledge of and attitudes to mental disorders: a limiting factor in the optimal use of treatment services. In: Andrews G, Henderson S, eds. Unmet need in psychiatry. Problems, resources, responses. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 2000:399–413. 11. Freidl M, Lang T, Scherer M. How psychiatric patients perceive the public’s stereotype of mental illness. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 2003;38:269–275. 12. Angermeyer MC, Matschinger H. Violent attacks on public figures by persons suffering from psychiatric disorders: their effect on social distance towards the mentally ill. Eur Arch Psychiat Clin Neurosci 1995;245:159–164. 13. Angermeyer MC, Matschinger H. The effect of violent attacks by schizophrenic persons on the attitude of the public towards the mentally ill. Soc Sci Med 1996;43:1721–1728.

175

Angermeyer and Dietrich 14. Angermeyer MC, Schulze B. Reinforcing stereotypes: the focus on forensic cases in news reporting and its influence on public attitudes towards the mentally ill. Int J Law Psychiatry 2001;24:469–486. 15. Angermeyer MC, Matschinger H. Social distance towards the mentally ill: results of representative surveys in the Federal Republic of Germany. Psychol Med 1997;27:131– 141. 16. Angermeyer MC, Matschinger H. The effect of personal experience with mental illness on the attitude towards individuals suffering from mental disorders. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 1996;31:321–326. 17. Angermeyer MC, Matschinger H, Holzinger A. Gender and attitudes towards people with schizophrenia. Results of a representative survey in the Federal Republic of Germany. Int J Soc Psychiatry 1998;44:107–116. 18. Angermeyer MC, Da¨umer R, Matschinger H. Benefits and risks of psychotropic medication in the eyes of the general public: results of a survey in the Federal Republic of Germany. Pharmacopsychiatry 1993;26:114–120. 19. Angermeyer MC, Matschinger H. Public attitude towards psychiatric treatment. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1996;94:326– 336. 20. Angermeyer MC, Matschinger H. Public attitudes to people with depression: have there been any changes over the last decade? J Affect Disord 2004;83:177–182. 21. Angermeyer MC, Matschinger H. Lay beliefs about schizophrenic disorder: the results of a population survey in Germany. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1994;89:39–45. 22. Angermeyer MC, Matschinger H. The effect of diagnostic labelling on the lay theory regarding schizophrenic disorders. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 1996;31:316– 320. 23. Angermeyer MC, Matschinger H. Lay beliefs about mental disorders: a comparison between the western and the eastern parts of Germany. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 1999;34:275–281. 24. Beck M, Matschinger H, Angermeyer MC. Social representations of depression in the western and the eastern part of Germany: do differences still persist 12 years after reunification? Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 2003;38:520–525. 25. Angermeyer MC, Matschinger H. Public attitudes towards psychotropic drugs: have their been any changes within recent years? Pharmacopsychiatry 2004;37:152–156. 26. Angermeyer MC, Matschinger H. Social representations of mental illness among the public. In: Guimo´n J, Fischer W, Sartorius N, eds. The image of madness. The public facing mental illness and psychiatric treatment. Basel: Karger, 1999. 27. Angermeyer MC, Matschinger H. The public’s attitude toward drug treatment of schizophrenia. In: Guimo´n J, Fischer W, Sartorius N, eds. The image of madness. The public facing mental illness and psychiatric treatment. Basel: Karger, 1999. 28. Benkert O, Graf-Morgenstern M, Hillert A et al. Public opinion on psychotropic drugs: an analysis of the factors influencing acceptance or rejection. J Nerv Ment Dis 1997;185:151–158. 29. Hillert A, Sandmann J, Ehmig SC, Weisbecker H, Kepplinger HM, Benkert O. The general public’s cognitive and emotional perception of mental illnesses: an alternative to attitude-research. In: Guimo´n J, Fischer W, Sartorius N, eds. The image of madness. The public facing mental illness and psychiatric treatment. Basel: Karger, 1999. 30. Angermeyer MC, Matschinger H, Riedel-Heller SG. Whom to ask for help in case of a mental disorder? Preferences of

176

31.

32.

33.

34.

35.

36.

37.

38.

39.

40.

41.

42.

43.

44.

45.

46.

the lay public. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 1999;34:202–210. Angermeyer MC, Matschinger H, Riedel-Heller SG. What to do about mental disorder – help-seeking recommendations of the lay public. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2001;103:220–225. Matschinger H, Angermeyer MC. Lay beliefs about the causes of mental disorders: a new methodological approach. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 1996;31: 309–315. Angermeyer MC, Matschinger H. The stigma of mental illness: effects of labelling on public attitudes towards people with mental disorders. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2003;108:304–309. Angermeyer MC, Matschinger H. Public beliefs about schizophrenia and depression: similarities and differences. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 2003;38:526–534. Angermeyer MC, Matschinger H. The stereotype of schizophrenia and its impact on the discrimination of people with schizophrenia: results from a representative survey in Germany. Schizophr Bull 2004;30:1049–1061. Angermeyer MC, Beck M, Matschinger H. Determinants of the public’s preference for social distance from people with schizophrenia. Can J Psychiatry 2003;48:663–668. Angermeyer MC, Matschinger H, Corrigan PW. Familiarity with mental illness and social distance from people with schizophrenia and major depression: testing a model using data from a representative population survey. Schizophr Res 2004;69:175–182. Beck M, Dietrich S, Matschinger H, Angermeyer MC. Alcoholism: low standing with the public? Attitudes towards spending financial resources on medical care and research on alcoholism. Alcohol Alcohol 2003;38:602– 605. Matschinger H, Angermeyer MC. The public’s preferences concerning the allocation of financial resources to health care: results from a representative population survey in Germany. Eur Psychiatry 2004;19:478–482. Dietrich S, Beck M, Bujantugs B, Kenzine D, Matschinger H, Angermeyer MC. The relationship between public causal beliefs and social distance to mentally ill people. Aust N Z J Psychiatry 2004;38:348–354. Magliano L, Fiorillo A, de Rosa C, Malangone C, Maj M. Beliefs about schizophrenia in Italy: a comparative nationwide survey of the general public, mental health professionals, and patientsÕ relatives. Can J Psychiatry 2004;49:322–330. Magliano L, de Rosa C, Fiorillo A, Malangone C, Maj M, the National Mental Health Project Working Group. Perception of patientsÕ unpredictability and beliefs on the causes and consequences of schizophrenia. A community survey. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 2004;39:410– 416. Ro¨ssler W, Salize HJ. Factors affecting public attitudes towards mental health care. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 1995;245:20–26. Ro¨ssler W, Salize HJ, Voges B. Does community-based care have an effect on public attitudes toward the mentally ill? Eur Psychiatry 1995;10:282–289. Hamre P, Dahl AA, Malt UF. Public attitudes to the quality of psychiatric treatment, psychiatric patients, and prevalence of mental disorders. Nord J Psychiatry 1994;48:275–281. Bra¨ndli H. The image of mental illness in Switzerland. In: Guimo´n J, Fischer W, Sartorius N, eds. The image of madness. The public facing mental illness and psychiatric treatment. Basel: Karger, 1999.

Public beliefs about and attitudes towards mentally ill 47. Lauber C, Falcato L, Nordt C, Ro¨ssler W. Lay beliefs about causes of depression. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2003;108(suppl. 418):96–99. 48. Lauber C, Falcato L, Ro¨ssler W. Attitudes to compulsory admission in psychiatry. Lancet 2000;355:2080. 49. Lauber C, Nordt C, Falcato L, Ro¨ssler W. Determinants of attitude to volunteering in psychiatry: results of a public opinion survey in Switzerland. Int J Soc Psychiatry 2002;48:209–219. 50. Lauber C, Nordt C, Falcato L, Ro¨ssler W. Do people recognise mental illness? Factors influencing mental health literacy. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 2003;253:248–251. 51. Lauber C, Nordt C, Falcato L, Ro¨ssler W. Factors influencing social distance toward people with mental illness. Community Ment Health J 2004;40:265–274. 52. Lauber C, Nordt C, Falcato L, Ro¨ssler W. Lay recommendations on how to treat mental disorders. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 2001;36:553–556. 53. Lauber C, Nordt C, Falcato L, Ro¨ssler W. Public attitude to compulsory admission of mentally ill people. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2002;105:385–389. 54. Lauber C, Nordt C, Sartorius N, Falcato L, Ro¨ssler W. Public acceptance of restrictions on mentally ill people. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2000;102(suppl. 407):26–32. 55. Graf J, Lauber C, Nordt C, Ruesch P, Meyer PC, Ro¨ssler W. Perceived stigmatization of mentally ill people and its consequences for the quality of life in a Swiss population. J Nerv Ment Dis 2004;192:542–547. 56. Priest RG, Vize C, Roberts A, Roberts M, Tylee A. Lay people’s attitudes to treatment of depression: results of opinion poll for defeat depression campaign just before its launch. Br Med J 1996;313:858–859. 57. Paykel ES, Hart D, Priest RG. Changes in public attitudes to depression during the defeat depression campaign. Br J Psychiatry 1998;173:519–522. 58. MORI. Attitudes towards schizophrenia. A survey of public opinion. London: MORI, Unpublished Research Report, 1997. 59. Crisp A, Gelder MG, Rix S, Meltzer HI, Rowlands OJ. Stigmatisation of people with mental illnesses. Br J Psychiatry 2000;177:4–7. 60. McKeon P, Carrick S. Public attitudes to depression: a national survey. Ir J Psychol Med 1991;8:116–121. 61. Alzheimer Society. Stigma and Alzheimer disease media kit, 2003;6 http://www.alzheimer.ca/english/media/stigma03-poll.htm. 62. Borinstein AB. Public attitudes toward persons with mental illness. Health Aff 1992;11:186–196. 63. Phelan JC, Link BG. Fear of people with mental illnesses: the role of personal and impersonal contact and exposure to threat or harm. J Health Soc Behav 2004;45:68–80. 64. Alexander LA, Link BG. The impact of contact on stigmatizing attitudes toward people with mental illness. J Ment Health 2003;12:271–289. 65. Link BG, Phelan JC, Bresnahan M, Stueve A, Pescosolido BA. Public conceptions of mental illness: labels, causes, dangerousness, and social distance. Am J Public Health 1999;89:1328–1333. 66. Martin JK, Pescosolido BA, Tuch SA. Of fear and loathing: the role of Ôdisturbing behaviorÕ, labels, and causal attributions in shaping public attitudes toward people with mental illness. J Health Soc Behav 2000;41:208–223. 67. Pescosolido BA, Monahan J, Link BG, Stueve A, Kikuzawa S. The public’s view of the competence, dangerousness, and need for legal coercion of persons with mental health problems. Am J Public Health 1999;89:1339–1345.

68. Phelan JC, Link BG, Stueve A, Pescosolido BA. Public conceptions of mental illness in 1950 and 1996: what is mental illness and is it to be feared? J Health Soc Behav 2000;41:188–207. 69. Schnittker J. Gender and reactions to psychological problems: an examination of social tolerance and perceived dangerousness. J Health Soc Behav 2000;44:224– 240. 70. Schnittker J, Freese J, Powell B. Nature, nurture, neither, nor: black–white differences in beliefs about the causes and appropriate treatment of mental illness. Soc Forces 2000;78:1101–1130. 71. Whaley AL. Ethnic and racial differences in perceptions of dangerousness of persons with mental illness. Psychiatr Serv 1997;48:1328–1330. 72. McSween JL. The role of group interest, identity, and stigma in determining mental health policy preferences. J Health Polit Policy Law 2002;27:773–800. 73. Croghan TW, Tomlin M, Pescosolido BA et al. American attitudes toward and willingness to use psychiatric medication. J Nerv Ment Dis 2003;191:166–174. 74. Kohn R, Sharma D, Camilleri CP, Levav I. Attitudes towards mental health in the Commonwealth of Dominica. Rev Panam Salud Pu´blica/Pan Am J Publ Health 2000;7:148–154. 75. Chou KL, Mak KY, Chung PK, Ho K. Attitudes towards mental patients in Hong Kong. Int J Soc Psychiatry 1996;42:213–219. 76. Chou KL, Mak KY. Attitudes to mental patients among Hong Kong Chinese: a trend study over two years. Int J Soc Psychiatry 1998;33:215–224. 77. Jorm AF, Korten AE, Jacomb PA, Christensen H, Henderson S. Attitudes towards people with a mental disorder: a survey of the Australian public and health professionals. Aust N Z J Psychiatry 1999;33:77–83. 78. Jorm AF, Korten AE, Rodgers B et al. Belief systems of the general public concerning the appropriate treatments for mental disorders. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 1997;32:468–473. 79. Jorm AF, Christensen H, Medway J, Korten AE, Jacomb PA, Rodgers B. Public belief systems about the helpfulness of interventions for depression: associations with history of depression and professional help-seeking. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 2000;35:211–219. 80. Jorm AF, Korten AE, Jacomb PA, Christensen H, Rodgers B, Pollitt P. ÔMental health literacyÕ: a survey of the public’s ability to recognise mental disorders and their beliefs about the effectiveness of treatment. Med J Aust 1997;166:182–186. 81. Jorm AF, Korten AE, Jacomb PA, Christensen H, Rodgers B, Pollitt P. Public beliefs about causes and risk factors for depression and schizophrenia. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 1997;32:143–148. 82. Jorm AF, Korten AE, Jacomb PA et al. Helpfulness of interventions for mental disorders: beliefs of health professionals compared with the general public. Br J Psychiatry 1997;171:233–237. 83. Akroyd S, Wyllie A. Impacts of national media campaign to counter stigma and discrimination associated with mental illness: survey 4. Auckland: Phoenix Research, 2002. 84. Ojanen M. Attitudes towards mental patients. Int J Soc Psychiatry 1992;38:120–130. 85. Gaebel W, Baumann AE. Interventions to reduce the stigma associated with severe mental illness: experiences from the Open the Doors program in Germany. Can J Psychiatry 2003;48:657–662.

177

Angermeyer and Dietrich ¨ ske H. Public 86. Gaebel W, Baumann AE, Witte AM, ZA attitudes towards people with mental illness in six German cities. Eur Arch Psychiatr Clin Neurosci 2002;252:278–287. 87. Hegerl U, Althaus D, Stefanek J. Public attitudes towards treatment of depression: effects of an information campaign. Pharmacopsychiatry 2003;36:288–291. 88. Madianos MG, Economou M, Hatjiandreou M, Papageorgiou A, Rogakou E. Changes in public attitudes towards mental illness in the Athens area (1979/1980–1994). Acta Psychiatr Scand 1999;99:73–78. 89. Ylla´ L, Hidalgo MS. Differentiating between the professions of psychologists and psychiatrists: a field study in Viscaya. In: Guimo´n J, Fischer W, Sartorius N, eds. The image of madness. The public facing mental illness and psychiatric treatment. Basel: Karger, 1999. 90. Fischer W, Goerg D, Zbinden E, Guimo´n J. Determining factors and the effects of attitudes towards psychotropic medication. In: Guimo´n J, Fischer W, Sartorius N, eds. The image of madness. The public facing mental illness and psychiatric treatment. Basel: Karger, 1999. 91. Huxley P. Location and stigma: a survey of community attitudes to mental illness. Part 1. Enlightnement and stigma. J Ment Health 1993;2:73–80. 92. Brockington IF, Hall P, Levings J, Murphy C. The community’s tolerance of the mentally ill. Br J Psychiatry 1993;162:93–99. 93. Hall P, Brockington IF, Levings J, Murphy C. A comparison of responses to the mentally ill in two communities. Br J Psychiatry 1993;162:99–108. 94. Wolff G, Pathare S, Craig T, Leff J. Community attitudes to mental illness. Br J Psychiatry 1996;168:183–190. 95. Wolff G, Pathare S, Craig T, Leff J. Community knowledge of mental illness and reaction to mentally ill people. Br J Psychiatry 1996;168:191–198. 96. Wolff G, Pathare S, Craig T, Leff J. Public education for community care. A new approach. Br J Psychiatry 1996;168:441–447. 97. Wolff G, Pathare S, Craig T, Leff J. Public education for community care: a new approach. In: Guimo´n J, Fischer W, Sartorius N, eds. The image of madness. The public facing mental illness and psychiatric treatment. Basel: Karger, 1999. 98. Ingamells S, Goodwin AM, John C. The influence of psychiatric hospital and community residence labels on social rejection of the mentally ill. Br J Clin Psychol 1996;35:359–367. 99. Lancaster B, Dudleston A. Attitudes towards alcohol: views of the general public, problem drinkers, alcohol service users and their families and friends. Edinburgh: Scottish Executive Central Research Unit, 2002. 100. Murphy BM, Black P, Duffy M, Kieran J, Mallon J. Attitudes towards the mentally ill in Ireland. Ir J Psychol Med 1993;10:75–79. 101. Stuart H, Arboleda-Flo´rez J. Community attitudes toward people with schizophrenia. Can J Psychiatry 2001;46:245–252. 102. Thompson AH, Stuart H, Bland RC, Arboleda-Flo´rez J, Warner R, Dickson RA. Attitudes about schizophrenia from the pilot site of the WPA worldwide campaign against the stigma of schizophrenia. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 2002;37:475–482. 103. Stip E, Caron J, Lane CJ. Schizophrenia: people’s perceptions in Quebec. Can Med Assoc J 2001;164:1299–1300. 104. Socall DW, Holtgraves T. Attitudes toward the mentally ill: the effects of labels and beliefs. Sociol Q 1993;33:435– 445.

178

105. Ayalon L, Arean PA. Knowledge of Alzheimer’s disease in four ethnic groups of older adults. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2004;19:51–57. 106. Garcia-Silberman S. Attitudes toward mental illness and psychiatry: preliminary results. Salud Ment 1998;21:40– 50. 107. Levav I, Shemesh A, Grinshpoon A, Aisenberg E, Shershevsky Y, Kohn R. Mental health-related knowledge, attitudes and practices in two kibbutzim. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 2004;39:758–764. 108. Ozmen E, Ogel K, Aker T, Sagduyou A, Tamar D, Boratav C. Public attitudes to depression in urban Turkey. The influence of perceptions and causal attributions on social distance towards individuals suffering from depression. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 2004;39:1010–1016. 109. Taskin EO, Sen FS, Aydemir O, Demet MM, Ozmen E, Icelli I. Public attitudes to schizophrenia in rural Turkey. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 2003;38:586–592. 110. Angermeyer MC, Buyantugs L, Kenzine DV, Matschinger H. Effects of labelling on public attitudes towards people with schizophrenia: are there cultural differences? Acta Psychiatr Scand 2004;109:420–425. 111. Mulatu MS. Perceptions of mental and physical illnesses in North-western Ethiopia. J Health Psychol 1999;4:531– 549. 112. Jorm AF, Medway J, Christensen H, Korten AE, Jacomb PA, Rodgers B. Attitudes towards people with depression: effects on the public’s help-seeking and outcome when experiencing common psychiatric symptoms. Aust N Z J Psychiatry 2000;34:612–618. 113. Jorm AF, Medway J, Christensen H, Korten AE, Jacomb PA, Rodgers B. Public beliefs about the helpfulness of interventions for depression: effects on actions taken when experiencing anxiety and depression symptoms. Aust N Z J Psychiatry 2000;34:619–626. 114. Jorm AF, Griffiths KM, Christensen H, Korten AE, Parslow RA, Rodgers B. Providing information about the effectiveness of treatment options to depressed people in the community: a randomised controlled trial of effects on mental health literacy, help-seeking and symptoms. Psychol Med 2003;33:1071–1079. 115. Christensen H, Griffiths KM, Jorm AF. Delivering interventions for depression by using the internet: randomised controlled trial. Br Med J 2004;328:265. 116. Griffiths KM, Christensen H, Jorm AF, Evans K, Groves C. Effect of web-based depression literacy and cognitivebehavioural therapy interventions on stigmatising attitudes to depression. Br J Psychiatry 2004;185:342–349. 117. Fisher LJ, Goldney RD. Differences in community mental health literacy in older and younger Australians. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2003;18:33–40. 118. Goldney RD, Fisher LJ, Wilson DH. Mental health literacy: an impediment to the optimum treatment of major depression in the community. J Affect Disord 2001;64:277–284. 119. Ng S, Martin J, Romans S. A community’s attitudes towards the mentally ill. N Z Med J 1995;8:55–508. 120. Link BG, Phelan JC. Conceptualizing stigma. Annu Rev Sociol 2001;27:262–285. 121. Corrigan PW, Watson AC. Understanding the impact of stigma on people with mental illness. World Psychiatry 2002;1:6–20. 122. Bogardus ES. Measuring social distance. J Appl Sociol 1925;1:216–226. 123. Hugo CJ, Boshoff DEL, Traut A, Zungu-Dirwayi N, Stein DJ. Community attitudes toward and knowledge of

Public beliefs about and attitudes towards mentally ill mental illness in South Africa. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 2003;38:715–719. 124. Furnham A, Murao M. A cross-cultural comparison of British and Japanese lay theories of schizophrenia. Int J Psychiatry 2000;46:4–20.

125. Corrigan PW, Watson AC. At issue: stop the stigma: call mental illness a brain disease. Schizophr Bull 2004;30:477–479.

179