Schizophrenia Bulletin vol. 35 no. 2 pp. 403–414, 2009 doi:10.1093/schbul/sbn097 Advance Access publication on August 9, 2008
Systemic Hypotheses for Generalized Cognitive Deficits in Schizophrenia: A New Take on An Old Problem
Dwight Dickinson1–3 and Philip D. Harvey4
Introduction
2
With the field focused on development of procognitive treatment strategies, the debate about how best to characterize the cognitive performance deficits shown by individuals with schizophrenia has renewed currency. Do these deficits represent a single, generalized deficit, a number of discrete cognitive domain–specific deficits, or some combination of generalized and specific effects? In this article, we argue that a generalized cognitive deficit is at the core of the disorder and deserves more focused consideration from cognitive specialists in the field. Further, we weigh evidence that this broad deficit may have systemic biological underpinnings, at the level of the central nervous system (CNS) and more generally.
VISN 5 Mental Illness Research, Education and Clinical Center, 10 North Greene Street, Suite 6a (BT/MIRECC), Baltimore, MD 21201; 3Department of Psychiatry, University of Maryland School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD; 4Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta, GA
The schizophrenia research community, including government, industry, and academia, has made development of procognitive treatment strategies a priority. Much current research is directed at dividing broad impairments in cognition into more delineated components that might correspond to relatively specific neural systems and serve as targets for intervention. Sometimes overlooked in this ambitious agenda is the substantial neuropsychological literature that signals a more broadly generalized dysfunction in higher order cognitive functions in this illness. In this article, we argue that a generalized cognitive deficit is at the core of the disorder, is not a methodological artifact, and deserves more focused consideration from cognitive specialists in the field. Further, we weigh evidence that this broad deficit may have systemic biological underpinnings. At the level of the central nervous system, examples of findings that might help to account for broad cognitive impairment include gray and white matter irregularities, poor signal integration by neurons and neural networks, and abnormalities in glutamate and g-aminobutyric acid neurotransmission. Other, more speculative hypotheses focus on even broader somatic systems, including energy metabolism and inflammatory processes. Treatment implications of systemic conceptualizations of schizophrenia are also considered.
Cognitive Performance in Schizophrenia Background Drawing on evidence from other illnesses that performance on certain tests may be specifically affected by deterioration in specific brain regions, much schizophrenia research has focused on ‘‘localizing’’ neuropsychological tests. Among people with schizophrenia, however, performance is impaired across tests that are sensitive to ‘‘localized’’ impairments in frontal, temporal, hippocampal, parietal, striatal, and cerebellar functions. Indeed, cognitive performance deficits in schizophrenia have been identified in almost every measurable cognitive ability domain, from basic sensory and perceptual functions (including olfaction,1 vision,2 and audition3,4) through preconscious information processing (eg, mismatch negativity5) and early attention (eg, P3006) to higher order cognition, including selective and sustained attention, working memory, episodic memory in verbal and nonverbal domains, processing speed, and problem solving.7,8 These impairments are present in patients in every clinical state and at every time period of the illness. Deficits emerge from studies using widely varying assessment techniques, including simple checklist measures, clinical neuropsychological tests, and elegant experimental paradigms. Cognitive performance deficits are seen in high-risk groups prior to the onset of psychotic illness.9 Deficits are broadly evident and well established at first episode and remain fairly stable through middle age.10,11
Key words: schizophrenia/cognition/neuropsychology/ general cognitive deficit/gray matter/white matter/energy metabolism/inflammation
1 To whom correspondence should be addressed; tel: 410-6057817, fax: 410-605-7739, e-mail:
[email protected].
Published by Oxford University Press 2008.
403
D. Dickinson & P. D. Harvey
In nonacute patients, the deficits are largely independent of symptomatology and are only modestly affected by current pharmacological treatments that improve other aspects of the illness.11,12 More so than other dimensions of the schizophrenia syndrome, cognitive deficits are associated with levels of day-to-day functioning in the community.13 There is evidence of exacerbation of deficits in later life,14 perhaps due to the duration of initially untreated psychosis.15 However, there is essentially no evidence of changes in the profile of cognitive impairments even in cases where cognitive impairments worsen over time, and the structure of cognitive impairments appears stable across treatment-related improvements in clinical state.16 Finally, a similar range of cognitive deficits is reported, in milder form, in the close relatives of people with schizophrenia.17 ‘‘Generalized’’ Cognition in Schizophrenia The history of study of cognitive impairments in schizophrenia has long been influenced by hypotheses regarding regional brain dysfunction, but the specificity of the findings is actually quite limited. A growing literature indicates that the latent structure of cognitive performance in schizophrenia is more unitary than was assumed previously. Perhaps the strongest evidence of this comes from the recent analysis of baseline cognitive performance data from the Clinical Antipsychotic Trials of Intervention Effectiveness (CATIE) schizophrenia trial.18 CATIE used an ‘‘all-comers’’ approach to recruit 1493 typical schizophrenia patients from 57 participating institutions representative of the real-world settings where these patients receive treatment. The test battery included 10 cognitive tests with multiple dependent measures representing traditional neuropsychological domains of processing speed, reasoning, verbal memory, working memory, and vigilance, with an additional test representing social cognition. More than 1300 participants completed at least 8 tests of the 11 cognitive tests, and 1005 participants completed all 11. A variety of statistical analyses showed that individual test scores and domain composites were significantly intercorrelated and that domain composites were highly correlated with a global composite score. Exploratory principal components analysis yielded a single significant component accounting for 45.2% of the overall variance in test scores. (See also Green et al.19) Using confirmatory factor analyses, we examined several plausible models of the dimensional structure of cognitive impairments. A hierarchical, unifactorial model, with the 5 domain scores loading on a common hierarchical factor, provided the best fit to the observed data.18 Other recent work indicates that the CATIE findings are representative of the schizophrenia literature more broadly.20–22 This hierarchical structure is exactly what we found in investigations of ‘‘within-groups’’ cognitive structure in matched samples of schizophrenia patients and healthy controls.20 And our meta-analysis of cross-domain correlations, showed a similar, intercorre404
lated structure of schizophrenia cognitive data across many studies.21 Such findings are by no means specific to schizophrenia but, rather, echo an extensive literature addressing nonclinical samples. Numerous studies of healthy research participants indicate that diverse cognitive measures are correlated positively and to at least a moderate degree.23,24 It is generally held that this network of correlations is characterized by a hierarchical factor structure in which individual measures load on broad cognitive ability factors, such as verbal comprehension or working memory, which in turn load on a higher order latent factor representing general cognitive ability or ‘‘g.’’23–25 In other work, we also considered the structure of the ‘‘between-groups’’ deficit in schizophrenia cognitive performance relative to control performance. Two separate structural equation modeling analyses showed that more than 60% of the between-groups deficit in cognitive performance was mediated through a common cognitive ability factor—representing, in essence, a ‘‘deficit g.’’ 22,26 Both analyses found disproportionate effects of diagnosis on certain cognitive domains. But these effects were small in magnitude and were confined to selected cognitive domains. It seems to follow from the findings regarding unitary cognitive structure in schizophrenia, both within schizophrenia samples and between schizophrenia and control groups, that much of what is measured by traditional neuropsychological assessment reflects broad cognitive ability rather than genuine domain-specific performance. This conclusion raises important questions about the validity of domain-specific interpretations of neuropsychological test findings from numerous areas of research, including clinical trials and studies of schizophrenia genetics. Reservations About Generalized Cognition Although we emphasize the generalized performance deficit in this article, it is clear that cognitive deficits in schizophrenia are not monolithic. Investigators sometimes have failed to find deficits where they were predicted (eg, in certain facets of early attention27). More commonly, various investigations have shown relatively preserved abilities in areas of word reading and other language skills, recognition memory, simple motor skills, and other performance domains. Indeed, because word reading performance is relatively stable even in the face of illness, it has been used to reference the magnitude of neuropsychological impairment in other areas.16 At the more impaired end of the deficit spectrum, a vast literature has attempted to identify areas of ‘‘differential deficit’’ in schizophrenia, which might reflect abnormalities in discrete brain systems,28–31 although the results have been quite inconsistent.32–36 In sum, across the range of higher order cognitive functions, variation in average performance deficits across ability areas among those with schizophrenia is considerably smaller than the overall average deficit relative to normal
Cognitive Deficits in Schizophrenia
performance. Large meta-analyses suggest that a distribution of mean cognitive performance in schizophrenia shifted approximately 1 SD to the left relative to controls across numerous domains and measures, with most individual measure effects falling within one-third SD above or below that level.7,8,37 A large factor-analytic literature also bears on the debate about generalized cognitive performance in schizophrenia. Obtained factors differ somewhat among studies, depending on the batteries used. Nevertheless, almost all studies have found that cognitive performance in schizophrenia, as measured by traditional neuropsychological tests, is characterized by multiple factors.38 This literature provided a foundation for the recent designation of ‘‘separable’’ ability domains in schizophrenia that might serve as targets for new cognition-enhancing agents with the potential specifically to benefit (or worsen) performance in these ability areas.38 However, ‘‘separable’’ does not mean ‘‘independent,’’ even in the opinion of the proponents of this approach. Historically, the bulk of factor analyses in the schizophrenia literature have been conducted with orthogonal rotations, which constrain resulting factors to be independent.21 When factor analyses use oblique rotations or confirmatory methods that leave the factors free to associate, the cross-domain correlations are invariably substantial.18,20,39,40 Some in the field have argued that these factors, and the separate groups of traditional neuropsychological variables used to measure them, are sufficiently independent of one another to tap discrete neural systems or molecular targets.38,41 Others, highlighting the interactive and multidimensional nature of most neuropsychological tests and the observed intercorrelations among cognitive domains and measures, have questioned the directness of factor connections to discrete neural substrates.21,35,42 Perhaps the most reliable evidence in the schizophrenia literature supports a limited number of disproportionate effects of illness, rather than ‘‘differential’’ or ‘‘separate’’ effects. Across numerous cognitive test studies, investigators have observed small but significant, disproportionate effects on verbal episodic memory and processing speed tasks.7,8 Measures of processing speed, particularly digit symbol coding from the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, may be the most impaired element of performance.7 In the CATIE trial, digit symbol was the best predictor of global performance, accounting for as much as 60% of the variance in total scores derived from larger cognitive assessment batteries.18 It is unclear whether these disproportionate effects are the result of the measurement properties of different instruments.43,44 That is, we may just measure processing speed and verbal memory more reliably than other capacities, or these measures may be simultaneously sensitive to a greater number of somewhat differentially impaired abilities. However, even assuming these are true effects, it is increasingly clear that this profile of somewhat preserved
performances and somewhat disproportionate deficits comes against a backdrop of broadly generalized impairment, with most complex cognitive operations impaired to a quite similar degree.22,37 Schizophrenia researchers have raised a number of questions about the generalized cognitive performance deficit. One long-standing critique attributes the deficit to global, noncognitive, and possible nonspecific factors such as reduced motivation, demoralization, and the effects of institutionalization. (See Harvey and Sharma45(pp12–13) for a discussion.) It has also been suggested that antipsychotic medications might cause a general dulling of cognitive performance. However, reviews have documented modest beneficial effects of both conventional and atypical medications overall.46,47 Even if these findings reflect practice effects,48 it seems reasonable to believe that these medications are fairly neutral cognitively. In recent years, another prominent critique has been that the generalized deficit reflects the multifactorial character of the neuropsychological measures used to assess cognitive function, more so than the actual structure of cognition.42,49,50 This critique is difficult to answer directly. Although the first generation of differential deficit research seems inconclusive, new efforts are under way to adapt behavioral measurement techniques from cognitive neuroscience,51,52 and this work may enable researchers to more reliably link specific cognitive operations with specific genetic and neural targets.53–55 However, we might then learn that these newer measures are correlated among themselves, or with traditional neuropsychological performance. And even if the new measures are substantially independent of one another,56,57 that finding would not necessarily mean that the more generalized findings with traditional neuropsychological measures are somehow spurious or misleading. That these traditional measures are multifactorial may be a disadvantage from a mechanism-discovery standpoint, but an advantage from other perspectives. More so than highly refined experimental paradigms, the traditional tests index integrated, interactive assemblies of cognitive operations—in other words, cognition at a ‘‘systems’’ level. This is an essential level of understanding for any complex disorder, in some ways less reductionistic and more ‘‘ecologically valid’’ than the level of disaggregated cognitive operations. It is this more global level of cognitive performance that has demonstrated relevance to disability and functional status58,59 and it is questionable whether narrowly circumscribed deficit areas can be considered viable treatment targets until they can be shown to relate to functional disability. Systemic Accounts of Generalized Cognitive Impairment in Schizophrenia? It is more and more difficult to dismiss as artifact the generalized character of cognitive impairment in schizophrenia 405
D. Dickinson & P. D. Harvey
and the generalized nature of performance deficits in patients relative to control groups. As we have reviewed, broadly targeted investigations have typically found broadly generalized cognitive impairment, with considerable variance shared in common across performance domains.8,20–22 While not challenging the importance of targeted cognitive investigations, it may be that the field can also progress by taking a more systemic perspective on cognitive impairment in schizophrenia. Taking the generalized cognitive ability findings at face value, what neurobiological and pathophysiological hypotheses might account for them? Of course, one possibility is that a relatively specific lesion (eg, in dopamine-mediated frontalstriatal loops60) could create an information-processing bottleneck (eg, poor response selection and programming61) that in turn causes extensive problems with performance on neuropsychological and other measures. A variation on this idea is that performance on different cognitive measures is similarly impaired in schizophrenia because the measures tap into an overlapping set of distinctly impaired, ‘‘regionally specific’’ capacities. By this reasoning, a measure showing disproportionate impairment, like digit symbol coding, does so because it simultaneously assays a larger set of these discrete deficits than other measures. Arguments of these sorts were easier to make in 20 years ago.62,63 Now, they must contend with waves of additional evidence—first, the findings supporting generalized cognitive impairment and, second, findings of widespread and intermingled brain abnormalities that could play a central role in broad cognitive performance deficits. CNS Hypotheses Prominent CNS findings in schizophrenia that might account for generalized cognitive dysfunction include (1) extensive reductions in gray matter and neuronal arborization, (2) diminished myelin density and fiber coherence in major white matter tracts; (3) poor signal integration at the level of the neuron and the neural network, and (4) abnormalities associated with the brain’s main excitatory and inhibitory neurotransmitters, glutamate and c-aminobutyric acid (GABA). These findings are reviewed briefly below, followed by a discussion of more global systemic hypotheses. Gray Matter. Ample literature documents decreased gray matter volumes in schizophrenia, generally and in numerous more narrowly defined regions of interest.64–69 Importantly, related findings in healthy schizophrenia relatives and at-risk groups are emerging, as well.70–72 Most evidence suggests a neurodevelopmental origin for these reductions, although some evidence of progressive volume loss over time has been presented.69,73 There is evidence that the gray matter reductions are not attributable to a reduction in the number of cells per se, as much as to reduced neuronal arborization and connectivity in distal 406
dendritic processes.74,75 The suggestion of reduced synaptic connectivity, in turn, has been used to support a number of cognitive models of schizophrenia,76 including ‘‘cognitive dysmetria’’77 and the ‘‘disconnection hypothesis.’’78 The connection of reduced gray matter to broad cognitive performance in schizophrenia is increasingly well established, with evidence reported of numerous associations to association cortices and other regions.65,79–83 White Matter. White matter abnormalities have been proposed as another explanation of functional connection failures in schizophrenia.84 However, it is only over the past decade or so that new imaging technologies have facilitated studies of the organization and integrity of white matter in this illness (for a comprehensive review, see Walterfang et al85). Though inconsistent, studies using diffusion tensor imaging techniques have found various abnormalities, including decreased coherence and density in major fiber tracts.86,87 Recent studies extended these findings to recent onset patients88 and unaffected relatives.72,89 Substantial attention has also focused on myelin integrity.84,90–92 Alteration in numbers, distribution, structural integrity, and genetics of oligodendrocytes has been reported in the prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia.93–96 Hypotheses regarding reduced fiber tract coherence and myelin integrity have obvious intuitive appeal in a disease characterized by slowed processing and diffuse cognitive dysfunction. Findings connecting these white matter abnormalities to cognitive performance are also emerging.3,65,88,97 Interestingly, the association of white matter function with glutamate98 and acetylcholine99 neurotransmission provides possible links across categories of neurobiological models. Neural Network Integration. Evidence is mounting that brain regions communicate by recognizing the concurrence of neural activity across neurons, neural assemblies, and neural networks and that schizophrenia is characterized by reduced communication and coordination.100 In general, the literature suggests that oscillatory responses in the beta and gamma bands are involved in integrating distributed neural activity to perform a variety of integrative cognitive functions.101 Research now implicates irregularities in these frequency bands as mechanisms of cognitive dysfunction in schizophrenia.4,102–104 Other work provides evidence that the signaling problems across these frequency bands are associated with increased response variability (‘‘noise’’) resulting from slow and variable phase resetting in pyramidal neurons after exposure to new stimuli.55 This abnormality was observed in patients and their relatives and was associated with cognitive performance.55 These investigators emphasized the likely association of these findings with dopamine D1 and D2 receptor–mediated optimization of signal-to-noise ratios in cortical microcircuits, which contributes broadly to the stability of cortical representations of internal and
Cognitive Deficits in Schizophrenia
external stimuli.105 Others tie these network integration abnormalities to GABA neurotransmission (see below).106 Glutamate and GABA. Hypotheses concerning the brain’s main excitatory and inhibitory neurotransmitters, glutamate and GABA, are converging in the schizophrenia literature.107–110 One prominent example focuses on disturbances in N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptor–mediated glutamate neurotransmission.108,111,112 NMDA receptors are ubiquitous in the neocortex110 and play a critical role in experience-dependent plasticity and the fast integration of information from multiple pathways.61,113 Slowed and inefficient processing across a wide range of cognitive operations could be a manifestation of dysfunction in such a fundamental neurotransmission system.112 Networks of GABA neurons give rise to oscillations in the gamma frequency band, discussed above, and allow synchronization of pyramidal cells in multiple cortical regions,110 providing a direct link to associated broad cognitive deficits.102 Drawing these lines of research together, the activity of GABA neurons is regulated, in part, by glutamatergic inputs and therefore likely reflects the influence of irregular NMDA receptor action. General System Hypotheses Each of the CNS systems noted above is present throughout the brain, with the potential for broad impact on cognitive operations. However, other findings and hypotheses provide the basis for an even wider ‘‘general systems’’ perspective on cognitive dysfunction in schizophrenia. Areas of interest include lipid metabolism,114,115 oxidative stress,116 energy metabolism,117,118 and inflammatory processes associated with vascular functioning.119,120 Especially intriguing are the growing number of findings suggesting possible genetic underpinnings for general systems hypotheses.116,118,121 Such systems not only are essential for brain cell development and integrity but also impact processes throughout the body. Further, they are complexly interactive. If such abnormalities are supported through further research, they might encourage an enlarged conception of schizophrenia as a general somatic disorder, rather than only a brain disease. The work associating these systems with cognition in schizophrenia is very preliminary and speculative. However, in light of recent empirical findings separately linking type 2 diabetes and chronic inflammation to impaired cognitive performance in schizophrenia,122,123 we examine aspects of energy metabolism and inflammation in hopes of opening a discussion of the possibility and potential importance of such broad systemic influences on cognitive performance in schizophrenia. Insulin Resistance. Metabolism of glucose accounts for most brain energy needs,124,125 and cognitive performance is sensitive to acute changes in blood glucose levels. Across clinical and nonclinical groups, acute glucose
administration improves episodic memory and other cognitive capacities.126,127 This acute glucose benefit has been demonstrated in schizophrenia patients without diabetes or related clinical conditions.125,128,129 In light of these findings of glucose facilitation, the fact that chronic hyperglycemia and type 2 diabetes are associated with cognitive impairments requires further explanation. The phenomenon of insulin resistance is a likely part of the explanation. Insulin regulates the uptake, oxidation, and storage of glucose in insulin-sensitive tissues, such as adipose tissue, liver, and skeletal muscle.130 Insulin also plays important roles in the CNS. It modulates glucose utilization in a regionally selective way, in particular in the medial temporal lobe and prefrontal cortex.125,131 It influences levels of cognition-relevant neurotransmitters, especially acetylcholine. Insulin also influences neuronal physiology, particularly NMDA glutamate receptor–mediated long-term potentiation (LTP), consistent with a role in memory processes.125,132 As with glucose, acute insulin administration has been shown to facilitate memory and other cognitive performance in experimental paradigms.132 In individuals experiencing ongoing insulin resistance, however, the ordinary functions of insulin are subverted. Insulin resistance is characterized by a self-reinforcing cycle of persistent high levels of insulin in peripheral tissue and a reduced sensitivity of the tissue to insulin’s essential metabolic functions, such as mediating glucose uptake into muscle.132 Over time, insulin resistance also downregulates insulin transport across the blood-brain barrier into the brain, ultimately leaving the brain in an insulin-depleted and functionally hypoglycemic state.124,132 Insulin resistance may be present years before the loss of glycemic control characteristic of diabetes.133 Even after control for confounders such as body mass and cardiovascular disease, insulin resistance and hyperinsulinemia have been shown to be associated broadly with cognitive function.134,135 Insulin dysregulation also has been proposed as a possible mechanism in neurodegenerative diseases known to impair cognition.131 Thus, it is quite clear that insulin dysregulation has central as well as peripheral effects and that these effects are associated with changes in brain structure and in the development of cognitive impairment. Chronic Inflammation. Recent studies indicate that lowgrade inflammationinperipheral tissue might have a central role in the pathogenesis of insulin resistance and type 2 diabetes.130,136 Proinflammatory cytokines (eg, interleukin 6 or IL-6) and closely associated acute-phase reactants (eg, C-reactive protein or CRP) are increased during insulin resistance. Importantly, the insulin resistance/inflammation association is potentiated by obesity and lifestyle factors.132 Proinflammatory cytokines, similarly to insulin, cross the blood-brain barrier and have regionally specific direct effects on brain function.131 IL-6, in particular, also like insulin, primarily modulates action in medial temporal and 407
D. Dickinson & P. D. Harvey
prefrontal regions137 and impairs LTP and cognitive behavior in animal models.138 Further, numerous human and animal studies link insulin resistance and inflammation, on the one hand, with vascular dysregulation and cardiovascular disease, on the other.124,136 Other studies have linked inflammation and cognitive performance in healthy middle-aged individuals137 and well-functioning elderly.139 Recent evidence also documents a 3-way association between inflammation, the insulin resistance syndrome, andcognitive impairment, with vascular effects as a primary mediating hypothesis.140 In sum, evidence is accumulating that insulin resistance and inflammation form an interactive complex that helps to account for cognitive impairment in diabetes and perhaps in cases of metabolic dysregulation before diabetes diagnostic thresholds are met. The effects of insulin resistance may be more direct, affecting aspects of energy metabolism in the brain and undermining other insulin-mediated processes. The effects of inflammation may be secondary to cytokine-induced reductions in the reactivity of CNS microvasculature in the shorter term and to permanent microvascular damage induced by chronic inflammation over the longer term. Obesity and lifestyle factors (especially exercise and smoking) are likely to play an important potentiating role in this complex of effects. The Schizophrenia Evidence. Irregularities of energy metabolism might contribute to cognitive dysfunction in schizophrenia in different ways,141 but hypotheses focused on insulin resistance and inflammation and their interaction are attractive for several reasons. The prevalence of diabetes in schizophrenia is 2–4 times higher than the general population rate.142 Additionally, more than 40% of schizophrenia patients meet criteria for the metabolic or ‘‘insulin resistance’’ syndrome, which strongly predicts diabetes and cardiovascular disease.143,144 Evidence is growing that schizophrenia is associated with metabolic disturbance, even at first episode, prior to treatment.145,146 This very early onset might suggest that effects of these changes could be related to cognitive impairments seen prior to the onset of the illness, such as during premorbid and prodromal periods. The onset of diabetes occurs at significantly younger ages in schizophrenia than in comparison groups, with the sharpest disparity among individuals aged less than 49 years.147,148 Also, in roughly one-quarter of cases, the illness emerges without significant weight gain.133,148 Evidence is accumulating that widely used second-generation antipsychotic (SGA) medications worsen problems of metabolic dysregulation and disease.133,148,149 Early evidence that SGAs improved cognitive performance has been called into question,19,150 accentuating concerns about metabolic effects without the concurrent procognitive benefits that were previously expected. With this background in mind, we hypothesized that schizophrenia patients with diabetes might show exacer408
bated cognitive impairment compared with matched schizophrenia patients without diagnosed metabolic disease.123 Results indicated that the schizophrenia/diabetes group was globally impaired relative to the ‘‘schizophrenia only’’ group (effect size [ES] = 0.26), with disproportionate effects in the domains of processing speed and visual/spatial ability (ES ’ 0.40). Additionally, these impairments were associated with diabetes severity markers (hemoglobin A1C, age of diabetes diagnosis, and diabetes duration). Given significant cognitive impairment in metabolically normal schizophrenia, we do not suggest that metabolic dysregulation is causal for cognitive impairment. Rather, it may be the case for some subset of patients that metabolic compromise provides a partial explanation of cognitive dysfunction and helps to account for its generalized character. In the schizophrenia genetics literature, it has become routine to refer to ‘‘multiple genes of small effect.’’ This may serve as one useful metaphor for conceptualizing generalized cognition: it is doubtless biologically complex, with many contributing and interacting factors. The biological substrate of the diabetes exacerbation effect is unclear. However, insulin resistance could be one contributing mechanism of cognitive impairment. Specific insulin abnormalities have been reported in schizophrenia (eg, a decrease in insulin receptor [IR]–mediated signal transduction151). In general, insulin irregularities emerge earlier than glycemic dyscontrol133 and are associated with cognition in nonpsychiatric groups independently of diabetes, glucose, body mass index, and other confounds,134 even in middle-aged subjects.135 Further, recent schizophrenia studies have documented increased insulin resistance (ie, homeostatic model assessment) with certain antipsychotic treatments even in the absence of changes in blood glucose levels.152–154 Given the known association of insulin resistance with inflammation, immune system response could be another contributor to generalized cognitive dysfunction in some individuals with schizophrenia. Numerous studies have documented immune system activation and increases in proinflammatory cytokines in schizophrenia,120,155,156 as well as changes with antipsychotic therapy.155,157 Most importantly for present purposes, Dickerson et al122 recently completed the first analysis of the relationship between CRP and cognitive performance in large, community-based sample of schizophrenia patients. They found elevated CRP significantly associated with worse global cognitive performance (ES = 0.30). In another recent study, adjunctive anti-inflammatory medication was associated with a trend toward improvement of cognition in patients.158 These findings suggest that, even independently of insulin resistance and diabetes, schizophrenia may be characterized by unusual vulnerability to inflammatory processes and that this inflammation is related to cognitive performance. Indeed, Hanson and Gottesman119 have elaborated a broad
Cognitive Deficits in Schizophrenia
theory of schizophrenia etiology and course founded on inflammation susceptibility and resulting vascular compromise. As a hypothesis for further research, we propose that insulin resistance interacting with inflammatory processes may be associated with some of the variance in broad cognitive functioning in schizophrenia. Medication, obesity, and lifestyle are likely to amplify these effects, creating a special risk for poor cognitive outcomes. Figure 1 depicts this possible complex of influences and outcomes. Implications of Systemic Hypotheses for Treatment Importantly, systemic illness conceptualizations of cognitive deficits in schizophrenia also open new avenues for intervention research. Since the discovery of chlorpromazine and the development of additional antipsychotic medications, there has been a focus on specific transmitter-based interventions in schizophrenia, especially on antipsychotic blockade of the dopamine D2 receptor. This focus was paralleled by the development of serotonin receptor inhibitor treatments for depression, cholinesterase inhibitor treatments for Alzheimer’s disease, and stimulant treatments affecting dopamine and norepinephrine neurotransmission for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. To date, most research on cognition-enhancing medications for schizophrenia has also followed this path—focusing on cholinesterase inhibitors, norepinephrine agents, glutamatergic agents, dopaminergic agents, selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors—with mixed and minimal results to date.159 Yet, some of the somatic dimensions described earlier, such as inflammation and irregularities of energy metabolism, may be far more tractable from an intervention
standpoint than either broad cognition or the full schizophrenia syndrome. Certainly, inflammation and diabetes are modifiable through education, lifestyle changes, and treatment. Further research could address whether early treatment with thiazolidinediones (to improve insulin sensitivity160), sulfonylureas (to increase insulin production160), salsalates (to reduce inflammation161), or statins (to improve lipid profile and moderate inflammation162) shows benefits for cognitive performance and whether any change is related to improvements in relevant biomarkers and indices of functional status. Impaired sleep is associated both with metabolic irregularity and schizophrenia.163 Behavioral,164 mechanical,165 and pharmacologic166 interventions to improve sleep and sleep architecture might similarly spill over into improved cognitive performance. Effects of diet and exercise programs on metabolic,167 inflammatory,164 and cognitive168 outcomes also merit investigation.
Conclusions Our reading of the literature indicates that there is a substantial general component to the cognitive performance deficit in schizophrenia that is more strongly connected than other markers to functional outcome in the community. Despite a long history, efforts to disaggregate this generalized impairment have had only mixed success. Although currently available clinical neuropsychological measures are not optimal for a detailed assessment of all aspects of the cognitive impairment in schizophrenia, it does not appear that psychometric limitations of traditional tests can explain away the general deficit. At the same time, emerging evidence suggests an account of the broad deficit based on plausible, systemic biological
Fig. 1. A Hypothetical Model of How Energy Metabolism, Inflammation, and Other Factors Might Combine to Affect Cognitive (and Functional) Outcomes in Schizophrenia.
409
D. Dickinson & P. D. Harvey
factors, either originating in the CNS (eg, gray and white matter abnormalities) or more generally influencing CNS functions (eg, energy metabolism and inflammation). This argument complements rather than replaces explanations of schizophrenia cognitive deficits based on more precisely defined cognitive and neural abnormalities in the illness—but it strongly suggests that the search for causes of and treatments for cognitive impairments should include attention to general and systemic factors. Funding VA Rehabilitation Research and Development Service (to D.D.); National Institute of Mental Health (MH63116, MH78775 to P.D.H.). Acknowledgments Dr Dickinson has no financial disclosures or conflicts of interest. In the past year, Dr Harvey served as a consultant for Pfizer, Ortho-McNeil, Neurogen, Dainippon Sumitomo America, Merck, and Wyeth. He received research support from Bristol Myers Squibb and Johnson and Johnson. He was on the speaker’s bureau for Eli Lilly. References 1. Moberg PJ, Arnold SE, Doty RL, et al. Olfactory functioning in schizophrenia: relationship to clinical, neuropsychological, and volumetric MRI measures. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2006;28:1444–1461. 2. Butler PD, Martinez A, Foxe JJ, et al. Subcortical visual dysfunction in schizophrenia drives secondary cortical impairments. Brain. 2007;130:417–430. 3. Leitman DI, Hoptman MJ, Foxe JJ, et al. The neural substrates of impaired prosodic detection in schizophrenia and its sensorial antecedents. Am J Psychiatry. 2007;164: 474–482. 4. Symond MB, Harris AWF, Gordon E, Williams LM. ‘‘Gamma synchrony’’ in first-episode schizophrenia: a disorder of temporal connectivity? Am J Psychiatry. 2005;162: 459–465. 5. Umbricht D, Krljes S. Mismatch negativity in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Schizophr Res. 2005;76:1–23. 6. Jeon YW, Polich J. Meta-analysis of P300 and schizophrenia: patients, paradigms, and practical implications. Psychophysiology. 2003;40:684–701. 7. Dickinson D, Ramsey M, Gold JM. Overlooking the obvious: a meta-analytic comparison of digit symbol coding tasks and other cognitive measures in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2007;64:532–542. 8. Heinrichs RW, Zakzanis KK. Neurocognitive deficit in schizophrenia: a quantitative review of the evidence. Neuropsychology. 1998;12:426–445. 9. Erlenmeyer-Kimling L, Rock D, Roberts SA, et al. Attention, memory, and motor skills as childhood predictors of schizophrenia-related psychoses: the New York High-Risk Project. Am J Psychiatry. 2000;157:1416–1422.
410
10. Albus M, Hubmann W, Scherer J, et al. A prospective 2-year follow-up study of neurocognitive functioning in patients with first-episode schizophrenia. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2002;252:262–267. 11. Hughes C, Kumari V, Soni W, et al. Longitudinal study of symptoms and cognitive function in chronic schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2003;59:137–146. 12. Rund BR, Melle L, Friis S, et al. Neurocognitive dysfunction in first-episode psychosis: correlates with symptoms, premorbid adjustment, and duration of untreated psychosis. Am J Psychiatry. 2004;161:466–72. 13. Green MF, Kern RS, Braff DL, Mintz J. Neurocognitive deficits and functional outcome in schizophrenia: are we measuring the ‘‘right stuff’’? Schizophr Bull. 2000;26:119–136. 14. Friedman JI, Harvey PD, Coleman T, et al. Six-year followup study of cognitive and functional status across the lifespan in schizophrenia: a comparison with Alzheimer’s disease and normal aging. Am J Psychiatry. 2001;158: 1441–1448. 15. Waddington JL, Youssef HA, Kinsella A. Sequential crosssectional and 10-year prospective study of severe negative symptoms in relation to duration of initially untreated psychosis in chronic schizophrenia. Psychol Med. 1995;25:849–857. 16. Harvey PD, Friedman JI, Bowie C, et al. Validity and stability of performance-based estimates of premorbid educational functioning in older patients with schizophrenia. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2006;28:178–192. 17. Snitz BE, Macdonald AW, 3rd, Carter CS. Cognitive deficits in unaffected first-degree relatives of schizophrenia patients: a meta-analytic review of putative endophenotypes. Schizophr Bull. 2006;32:179–194. 18. Keefe RS, Bilder RM, Harvey PD, et al. Baseline neurocognitive deficits in the CATIE schizophrenia trial. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2006;31:2033–46. 19. Green MF, Marder SR, Glynn SM, et al. The neurocognitive effects of low-dose haloperidol: a two-year comparison with risperidone. Biol Psychiatry. 2002;51:972–978. 20. Dickinson D, Ragland JD, Calkins ME, Gold JM, Gur RC. A comparison of cognitive structure in schizophrenia patients and healthy controls using confirmatory factor analysis. Schizophr Res. 2006;85:20–29. 21. Dickinson D, Gold JM. Less unique variance than meets the eye: overlap among traditional neuropsychological dimensions in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 2008;34:423–434. 22. Dickinson D, Ragland JD, Gold JM, Gur RC. General and specific cognitive deficits in schizophrenia: Goliath defeats David? Biol Psychiatry. (In press). 23. Carroll JB. Human Cognitive Abilities: A Survey of FactorAnalytic Studies. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press; 1993. 24. Jensen AR. The g Factor: The Science of Mental Ability. Westport, Conn: Praeger; 1998. 25. Deary IJ. Human intelligence differences: a recent history. Trends Cogn Sci. 2001;5:127–130. 26. Dickinson D, Iannone VN, Wilk CM, Gold JM. General and specific cognitive deficits in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2004;55:826–833. 27. Gold JM, Fuller RL, Robinson BM, et al. Intact attentional control of working memory encoding in schizophrenia. J Abnorm Psychol. 2006;115:658–673. 28. Goldberg TE, Weinberger DR, Berman KF, Pliskin NH, Podd MH. Further evidence for dementia of the prefrontal type in schizophrenia? A controlled study of teaching the
Cognitive Deficits in Schizophrenia
29.
30. 31.
32.
33.
34.
35. 36.
37. 38.
39.
40.
41.
42.
43. 44.
45.
46.
47.
48.
Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1987; 44:1008–1014. Goldberg TE, Weinberger DR, Pliskin NH, Berman KF, Podd MH. Recall memory deficit in schizophrenia. A possible manifestation of prefrontal dysfunction. Schizophr Res. 1989;2:251–257. McKenna PJ, Tamlyn D, Lund CE, et al. Amnesic syndrome in schizophrenia. Psychol Med. 1990;20:967–972. Saykin AJ, Gur RC, Gur RE, et al. Neuropsychological function in schizophrenia. Selective impairment in memory and learning. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1991;48:618–624. Blanchard JJ, Neale JM. The neuropsychological signature of schizophrenia: generalized or differential deficit? Am J Psychiatry. 1994;151:40–48. Bilder RM, Goldman RS, Robinson D, et al. Neuropsychology of first-episode schizophrenia: initial characterization and clinical correlates. Am J Psychiatry. 2000;157:549–559. Braff DL, Heaton R, Kuck J, et al. The generalized pattern of neuropsychological deficits in outpatients with chronic schizophrenia with heterogeneous Wisconsin Card Sorting Test results. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1991;48:891–898. Keefe RS. The contribution of neuropsychology to psychiatry. Am J Psychiatry. 1995;152:6–15. Mohamed S, Paulsen JS, O’Leary D, Arndt S, Andreasen N. Generalized cognitive deficits in schizophrenia: a study of first-episode patients. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1999;56:749–754. Heinrichs RW. The primacy of cognition in schizophrenia. Am Psychol. 2005;60:229–242. Nuechterlein KH, Barch DM, Gold JM, et al. Identification of separable cognitive factors in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2004;72:29–39. Dickinson D, Iannone VN, Gold JM. Factor structure of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III in schizophrenia. Assessment. 2002;9:171–180. Gladsjo JA, McAdams LA, Palmer BW, et al. A six-factor model of cognition in schizophrenia and related psychotic disorders: relationships with clinical symptoms and functional capacity. Schizophr Bull. 2004;30:739–754. Egan MF, Goldberg TE, Gsheidle T, et al. Relative risk for cognitive impairments in siblings of patients with schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2001;50:98–107. MacDonald AW, 3rd, Carter CS. Cognitive experimental approaches to investigating impaired cognition in schizophrenia: a paradigm shift. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2002; 24:873–882. Chapman LJ, Chapman JP. Problems in the measurement of cognitive deficit. Psychol Bull. 1973;79:380–385. Miller MB, Chapman JP, Chapman LJ, Collins J. Task difficulty and cognitive deficits in schizophrenia. J Abnorm Psychol. 1995;104:251–258. Harvey PD, Sharma T. Understanding and Treating Cognition in Schizophrenia: A clinician’s Handbook. London, UK: Taylor & Francis; 2002. Mishara AL, Goldberg TE. A meta-analysis and critical review of the effects of conventional neuroleptic treatment on cognition in schizophrenia: opening a closed book. Biol Psychiatry. 2004;55:1013–1022. Woodward ND, Purdon SE, Meltzer HY, Zald DH. A meta-analysis of neuropsychological change to clozapine, olanzapine, quetiapine, and risperidone in schizophrenia. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol. 2005;8:457–472. Goldberg TE, Goldman RS, Burdich RE, et al. Cognitive improvement after treatment with second-generation
49.
50. 51.
52.
53.
54.
55.
56.
57.
58.
59.
60. 61. 62. 63.
64.
65.
66.
antipsychotic medications in first-episode schizophrenia: is it a practice effect? Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2007;64: 1115–1122. Carter CS. Applying new approaches from cognitive neuroscience to enhance drug development for the treatment of impaired cognition in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 2005; 31:810–815. Jonides J, Nee DE. Assessing dysfunction using refined cognitive methods. Schizophr Bull. 2005;31:823–829. Carter CS, Barch DM. Cognitive neuroscience-based approaches to measuring and improving treatment effects on cognition in schizophrenia: the CNTRICS initiative. Schizophr Bull. 2007;33:1131–1137. Javitt DC, Spencer KM, Thaker GK, Winterer G, Hajos M. Neurophysiological biomarkers for drug development in schizophrenia. Nat Rev Drug Discov. 2008;7:68–83. MacDonald AW, 3rd, Carter CS, Flory JD, Ferrell RE, Manuck SB. COMT val158Met and executive control: a test of the benefit of specific deficits to translational research. J Abnorm Psychol. 2007;116:306–312. Tan HY, Chen Q, Goldberg TE, et al. Catechol-O-methyltransferase Val158Met modulation of prefrontal-parietalstriatal brain systems during arithmetic and temporal transformations in working memory. J Neurosci. 2007;27: 13393–13401. Winterer G, Coppola R, Goldberg T, et al. Prefrontal broadband noise, working memory, and genetic risk for schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 2004;161:490–500. Hall MH, Schulze K, Bramon E, et al. Genetic overlap between P300, P50, and duration mismatch negativity. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2006;141:336–343. Louchart-de la Chapelle S, Nkam I, Hary E, et al. A concordance study of three electrophysiological measures in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 2005;162:466–474. Green M, Kern R, Robertson M, Sergi M, Kee K. Relevance of neurocognitive deficits for functional outcome in schizophrenia. In: Sharma T, Harvey P, eds. Cognition in Schizophrenia. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press; 2000:178–192. Green MF, Kern RS, Heaton RK. Longitudinal studies of cognition and functional outcome in schizophrenia: implications for MATRICS. Schizophr Res. 2004;72:41–51. Robbins TW. The case of frontostriatal dysfunction in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 1990;16:391–402. Friston K. Disconnection and cognitive dysmetria in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 2005;162:429–432. Frith CD, Done DJ. Towards a neuropsychology of schizophrenia. Br J Psychiatry. 1988;153:437–443. Goldberg TE, Weinberger DR. Probing prefrontal function in schizophrenia with neuropsychological paradigms. Schizophr Bull. 1988;14:179–183. Antonova E, Sharma T, Morris R, Kumari V. The relationship between brain structure and neurocognition in schizophrenia: a selective review. Schizophr Res. 2004;70: 117–145. Antonova E, Kumari V, Morris R, et al. The relationship of structural alterations to cognitive deficits in schizophrenia: a voxel-based morphometry study. Biol Psychiatry. 2005; 58:457–467. Davatzikos C, Shen D, Gur RC, et al. Whole-brain morphometric study of schizophrenia revealing a spatially complex set of focal abnormalities. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2005;62: 1218–1227.
411
D. Dickinson & P. D. Harvey
67. Honea R, Crow TJ, Passingham D, Mackay CE. Regional deficits in brain volume in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis of voxel-based morphometry studies. Am J Psychiatry. 2005; 162:2233–2245. 68. Honea RA, Meyer-Lindenberg A, Hobbs KB, et al. Is gray matter volume an intermediate phenotype for schizophrenia? A voxel-based morphometry study of patients with schizophrenia and their healthy siblings. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;63:465–474. 69. McCarley RW, Wiblie CG, Frumin M, et al. MRI anatomy of schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 1999;45:1099–1119. 70. Boos HB, Aleman A, Cahn W, Pol HH, Kahn RS. Brain volumes in relatives of patients with schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2007;64:297–304. 71. Borgwardt SJ, Riecher-Rossler A, Dazzan P, et al. Regional gray matter volume abnormalities in the at risk mental state. Biol Psychiatry. 2007;61:1148–1156. 72. Fan Y, Gur RE, Gur RC, et al. Unaffected family members and schizophrenia patients share brain structure patterns: a high-dimensional pattern classification study. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;63:118–124. 73. Ellison-Wright I, Glahn DC, Laird AR, Thelen SM, Bullmore E. The anatomy of first-episode and chronic schizophrenia: an anatomical likelihood estimation meta-analysis. Am J Psychiatry. 2008. 74. Selemon LD, Goldman-Rakic PS. The reduced neuropil hypothesis: a circuit based model of schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 1999;45:17–25. 75. Kalus P, Muller TJ, Zuschratter W, Senitz D. The dendritic architecture of prefrontal pyramidal neurons in schizophrenic patients. Neuroreport. 2000;11:3621–3625. 76. McGlashan TH, Hoffman RE. Schizophrenia as a disorder of developmentally reduced synaptic connectivity. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2000;57:637–648. 77. Andreasen NC, Paradiso S, O’Leary DS. ‘‘Cognitive dysmetria’’ as an integrative theory of schizophrenia: a dysfunction in cortical-subcortical-cerebellar circuitry? Schizophr Bull. 1998;24:203–218. 78. Friston KJ. Schizophrenia and the disconnection hypothesis. Acta Psychiatr Scand Suppl. 1999;395:68–79. 79. Bilder RM, Bogerts B, Ashtari M, et al. Anterior hippocampal volume reductions predict frontal lobe dysfunction in first episode schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 1995;17:47–58. 80. Lawyer G, Nyman H, Agartz I, et al. Morphological correlates of cognitive dysfunction in schizophrenia as studied with Bayesian regression. BMC Psychiatry. 2006;6:31. 81. McCarley RW, Niznikiewiez MA, Salisbury DF, et al. Cognitive dysfunction in schizophrenia: unifying basic research and clinical aspects. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1999;249(suppl 4): 69–82. 82. Salgado-Pineda P, Baeza I, Perez-Gomez M, et al. Sustained attention impairment correlates to gray matter decreases in first episode neuroleptic-naive schizophrenic patients. Neuroimage. 2003;19:365–375. 83. Sanfilipo M, et al. Cognitive performance in schizophrenia: relationship to regional brain volumes and psychiatric symptoms. Psychiatry Res. 2002;116:1–23. 84. Randall PL. Schizophrenia, abnormal connection, and brain evolution. Med Hypotheses. 1983;10:247–280. 85. Walterfang M, Wood SJ, Velakoulis D, Pantelis C. Neuropathological, neurogenetic and neuroimaging evidence for white matter pathology in schizophrenia. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2006;30:918–948.
412
86. Kanaan RA, Kim JS, Kaufmann WE, et al. Diffusion tensor imaging in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2005;58:921–929. 87. Lim KO, Hedehus M, Moseley M, et al. Compromised white matter tract integrity in schizophrenia inferred from diffusion tensor imaging. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1999;56: 367–374. 88. Szeszko PR, Robinson DG, Ashtari M, et al. Clinical and neuropsychological correlates of white matter abnormalities in recent onset schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008;33:976–984. 89. McIntosh AM, Moorhead TW, Job D, et al. The effects of a neuregulin 1 variant on white matter density and integrity. Mol Psychiatry. In press. 90. Bartzokis G, Altshuler L. Reduced intracortical myelination in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 2005;162:1229–1230. 91. Konrad A, Winterer G. Disturbed structural connectivity in schizophrenia primary factor in pathology or epiphenomenon? Schizophr Bull. 2008;34:72–92. 92. Kubicki M, Park H, Westin CF, et al. DTI and MTR abnormalities in schizophrenia: analysis of white matter integrity. Neuroimage. 2005;26:1109–1118. 93. Hakak Y, Walker JR, Li C, et al. Genome-wide expression analysis reveals dysregulation of myelination-related genes in chronic schizophrenia. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2001; 98:4746–4751. 94. Hof PR, Haroutunian V, Copland C, Davis KL, Buxbaum JD. Molecular and cellular evidence for an oligodendrocyte abnormality in schizophrenia. Neurochem Res. 2002;27: 1193–1200. 95. Uranova N, Orlovskaya D, Vikhreva O, et al. Electron microscopy of oligodendroglia in severe mental illness. Brain Res Bull. 2001;55:597–610. 96. Uranova NA, Vostrikov VM, Orlovskaya DD, Rachmanova VI. Oligodendroglial density in the prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia and mood disorders: a study from the Stanley Neuropathology Consortium. Schizophr Res. 2004;67:269–275. 97. Lim KO, Ardekani BA, Nierenberg J, et al. Voxelwise correlational analyses of white matter integrity in multiple cognitive domains in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 2006; 163:2008–2010. 98. Davis KL, Stewart DG, Friedman JL, et al. White matter changes in schizophrenia: evidence for myelin-related dysfunction. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2003;60:443–456. 99. Bartzokis G. Acetylcholinesterase inhibitors may improve myelin integrity. Biol Psychiatry. 2007;62:294–301. 100. Ford JM, Krystal JH, Mathalon DH. Neural synchrony in schizophrenia: from networks to new treatments. Schizophr Bull. 2007;33:848–852. 101. Uhlhaas PJ, Singer W. Neural synchrony in brain disorders: relevance for cognitive dysfunctions and pathophysiology. Neuron. 2006;52:155–168. 102. Cho RY, Konecky RO, Carter CS. Impairments in frontal cortical gamma synchrony and cognitive control in schizophrenia. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2006;103:19878–19883. 103. Ford JM, Roach BJ, Faustman WO, Mathalon DH. Out-ofsynch and out-of-sorts: dysfunction of motor-sensory communication in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;63:736–743. 104. Uhlhaas PJ, Linden DE, Singer W, et al. Dysfunctional long-range coordination of neural activity during Gestalt perception in schizophrenia. J Neurosci. 2006;26:8168–8175. 105. Winterer G, Weinberger DR. Genes, dopamine and cortical signal-to-noise ratio in schizophrenia. Trends Neurosci. 2004; 27:683–690.
Cognitive Deficits in Schizophrenia
106. Lewis DA, Hashimoto T, Volk DW. Cortical inhibitory neurons and schizophrenia. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2005;6: 312–324. 107. Costa E, Davis JM, Dong E, et al. A GABAergic cortical deficit dominates schizophrenia pathophysiology. Crit Rev Neurobiol. 2004;16:1–23. 108. Javitt DC, Zukin SR. Recent advances in the phencyclidine model of schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 1991;148: 1301–1308. 109. Lewis DA, Moghaddam B. Cognitive dysfunction in schizophrenia: convergence of gamma-aminobutyric acid and glutamate alterations. Arch Neurol. 2006;63:1372–1376. 110. Lewis DA, Gonzalez-Burgos G. Pathophysiologically based treatment interventions in schizophrenia. Nat Med. 2006;12: 1016–1022. 111. Coyle JT. The glutamatergic dysfunction hypothesis for schizophrenia. Harv Rev Psychiatry. 1996;3:241–253. 112. Javitt DC, Liederman E, Cienfuegos A, Shelley AM. Panmodal processing imprecision as a basis for dysfunction of transient memory storage systems in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 1999;25:763–775. 113. Javitt DC, Steinschneider M, Schroeder CE, Arezzo JC. Role of cortical N-methyl-D-aspartate receptors in auditory sensory memory and mismatch negativity generation: implications for schizophrenia. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1996;93:11962–11967. 114. Horrobin DF. The membrane phospholipid hypothesis as a biochemical basis for the neurodevelopmental concept of schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 1998;30:193–208. 115. Mahadik SP, Evans DR. Is schizophrenia a metabolic brain disorder? Membrane phospholipid dysregulation and its therapeutic implications. Psychiatr Clin North Am. 2003;26: 85–102. 116. Prabakaran S, Swatton JE, Ryan MM, et al. Mitochondrial dysfunction in schizophrenia: evidence for compromised brain metabolism and oxidative stress. Mol Psychiatry. 2004; 9:643, 684–697. 117. Deutch AY, Bubser M. The orexins/hypocretins and schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 2007;33:1277–1283. 118. Lovestone S, Killick R, Di Forti M, Murray R. Schizophrenia as a GSK-3 dysregulation disorder. Trends Neurosci. 2007; 30:142–149. 119. Hanson DR, Gottesman II. Theories of schizophrenia: a genetic-inflammatory-vascular synthesis. BMC Med Genet. 2005;6:7. 120. Potvin S, Stip E, Sepehry AA, et al. Inflammatory cytokine alterations in schizophrenia: a systematic quantitative review. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;63:801–808. 121. Lencz T, Morgan TV, Athanasion M, et al. Converging evidence for a pseudoautosomal cytokine receptor gene locus in schizophrenia. Mol Psychiatry. 2007;12:572–580. 122. Dickerson F, Stallings C, Origoni A, Boronow J, Yolken R. C-reactive protein is associated with the severity of cognitive impairment but not of psychiatric symptoms in individuals with schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2007;93: 261–265. 123. Dickinson D, Gold JM, Dickerson FB, Medoff D, Dixon LB. Evidence of exacerbated cognitive deficits in schizophrenia patients with co-morbid diabetes. Psychosomatics. 2008; 49:123–131. 124. Convit A. Links between cognitive impairment in insulin resistance: an explanatory model. Neurobiol Aging. 2005;26 (suppl 1): 31–35.
125. Newcomer JW, Craft S, Fucetola R, et al. Glucose-induced increase in memory performance in patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 1999;25:321–335. 126. Craft S, Dagogo-Jack SE, Wiethop BV, et al. Effects of hyperglycemia on memory and hormone levels in dementia of the Alzheimer type: a longitudinal study. Behav Neurosci. 1993;107:926–940. 127. Craft S, Murphy C, Wenstrom J. Glucose effects on complex memory and non-memory tasks: the influence of age, sex, and glucoregulatory response. Psychobiology. 1994;22: 95–105. 128. Fucetola R, Newcomer JW, Craft S, Melson AK. Ageand dose-dependent glucose-induced increases in memory and attention in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 1999;88: 1–13. 129. Stone WS, Seidman LJ, Wojcik JD, Green AI. Glucose effects on cognition in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2003; 62:93–103. 130. Tilg H, Moschen AR. Adipocytokines: mediators linking adipose tissue, inflammation and immunity. Nat Rev Immunol. 2006;6:772–783. 131. Craft S, Watson GS. Insulin and neurodegenerative disease: shared and specific mechanisms. Lancet Neurol. 2004;3: 169–178. 132. Craft S. Insulin resistance syndrome and Alzheimer’s disease: age- and obesity-related effects on memory, amyloid, and inflammation. Neurobiol Aging. 2005;26(suppl 1): 65–69. 133. Newcomer JW. Second-generation (atypical) antipsychotics and metabolic effects: a comprehensive literature review. CNS Drugs. 2005;19(suppl 1): 1–93. 134. Abbatecola AM, Paolisso G, Lamponi M, et al. Insulin resistance and executive dysfunction in older persons. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2004;52:1713–1718. 135. Young SE, Mainous AG, 3rd, Carnemolla M. Hyperinsulinemia and cognitive decline in a middle-aged cohort. Diabetes Care. 2006;29:2688–2693. 136. Hak AE, Pols HA, Stehouwer CD, et al. Markers of inflammation and cellular adhesion molecules in relation to insulin resistance in nondiabetic elderly: the Rotterdam study. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2001;86:4398–4405. 137. Marsland AL, Petersen KL, Sathanoor R, et al. Interleukin6 covaries inversely with cognitive performance among middle-aged community volunteers. Psychosom Med. 2006;68: 895–903. 138. Bauer S, Kerr BJ, Patterson PH. The neuropoietic cytokine family in development, plasticity, disease and injury. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2007;8:221–232. 139. Yaffe K, Lindquist K, Penninx BW, et al. Inflammatory markers and cognition in well-functioning AfricanAmerican and white elders. Neurology. 2003;61:76–80. 140. Yaffe K, Kanaya A, Lindquist K, et al. The metabolic syndrome, inflammation, and risk of cognitive decline. JAMA. 2004;292:2237–2242. 141. Liddle PF, Lane CJ, Ngan ET. Immediate effects of risperidone on cortico-striato-thalamic loops and the hippocampus. Br J Psychiatry. 2000;177:402–407. 142. Bushe C, Holt R. Prevalence of diabetes and impaired glucose tolerance in patients with schizophrenia. Br J Psychiatry. 2004;47(Suppl): S67–S71. 143. Cohn T, Prud’homme D, Streiner D, Kameh H, Remington G. Characterizing coronary heart disease risk in chronic schizophrenia: high prevalence of the metabolic syndrome. Can J Psychiatry. 2004;49:753–760.
413
D. Dickinson & P. D. Harvey
144. McEvoy JP, Meyer JM, Goff DC, et al. Prevalence of the metabolic syndrome in patients with schizophrenia: baseline results from the Clinical Antipsychotic Trials of Intervention Effectiveness (CATIE) schizophrenia trial and comparison with national estimates from NHANES III. Schizophr Res. 2005;80:19–32. 145. Cohn TA, Remington G, Zipursky RB, et al. Insulin resistance and adiponectin levels in drug-free patients with schizophrenia: a preliminary report. Can J Psychiatry. 2006; 51:382–386. 146. Ryan MC, Collins P, Thakore JH. Impaired fasting glucose tolerance in first-episode, drug-naive patients with schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160:284–289. 147. Cohen D, Dekker JJ, Peen J, Gispen-de Wied CC. Prevalence of diabetes mellitus in chronic schizophrenic inpatients in relation to long-term antipsychotic treatment. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2006;16:187–194. 148. Koller EA, Doraiswamy PM. Olanzapine-associated diabetes mellitus. Pharmacotherapy. 2002;22:841–852. 149. ADA and APA.Consensus development conference on antipsychotic drugs and obesity and diabetes by the American Diabetes Association and the American Psychiatric Association. Diabetes Care. 2004;27:596–601. 150. Goldberg TE, Egan MF, Gscheidle T, et al. Executive subprocesses in working memory: relationship to catechol-Omethyltransferase Val158Met genotype and schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2003;60:889–896. 151. Zhao Z, Ksiezak-Reding H, Riggio S, Haroutunian V, Pasinetti GM. Insulin receptor deficits in schizophrenia and in cellular and animal models of insulin receptor dysfunction. Schizophr Res. 2006;84:1–14. 152. Henderson DC, Cagliero E, Copeland PM, et al. Glucose metabolism in patients with schizophrenia treated with atypical antipsychotic agents: a frequently sampled intravenous glucose tolerance test and minimal model analysis. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2005;62:19–28. 153. Simpson GM, Glick ID, Weiden PJ, Romano SJ, Siu CO. Randomized, controlled, double-blind multicenter comparison of the efficacy and tolerability of ziprasidone and olanzapine in acutely ill inpatients with schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2004;161:1837–1847. 154. Wu RR, Zhao JP, Liu ZN, et al. Effects of typical and atypical antipsychotics on glucose-insulin homeostasis and lipid metabolism in first-episode schizophrenia. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2006;186:572–578. 155. Maes M, Bocchio-Chiavetto L, Bignotti S, et al. Effects of atypical antipsychotics on the inflammatory response system
414
156.
157.
158.
159.
160.
161.
162.
163.
164.
165.
166. 167.
168.
in schizophrenic patients resistant to treatment with typical neuroleptics. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2000;10:119–124. Zhang XY, Zhou DF, Zhang PY, et al. Elevated interleukin2, interleukin-6 and interleukin-8 serum levels in neurolepticfree schizophrenia: association with psychopathology. Schizophr Res. 2002;57:247–258. Zhang XY, Zhou DF, Cao LY, et al. Changes in serum interleukin-2, -6, and -8 levels before and during treatment with risperidone and haloperidol: relationship to outcome in schizophrenia. J Clin Psychiatry. 2004;65:940–947. Muller N, Riedel M, Schwarz MJ, Engel RR. Clinical effects of COX-2 inhibitors on cognition in schizophrenia. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2005;255:149–151. Bowie CR, Harvey PD. Treatment of cognitive deficits in schizophrenia. Curr Opin Investig Drugs. 2006;7: 608–613. Ryan CM, Freed MI, Rood JA, et al. Improving metabolic control leads to better working memory in adults with type 2 diabetes. Diabetes Care. 2006;29:345–351. Fleischman A, Shoelson SE, Bernier R, Goldfine AB. Salsalate improves glycemia and inflammatory parameters in obese young adults. Diabetes Care. 2008;31:289–294. Carloni S, Mazzoni E, Cimino M, et al. Simvastatin reduces caspase-3 activation and inflammatory markers induced by hypoxia-ischemia in the newborn rat. Neurobiol Dis. 2006;21:119–126. Van Cauter E, Linkowski P, Kerkofs M, et al. Circadian and sleep-related endocrine rhythms in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1991;48:348–356. Vgontzas AN, Bixler EO, Chrousos GP. Sleep apnea is a manifestation of the metabolic syndrome. Sleep Med Rev. 2005;9:211–224. Karanti A, Landen M. Treatment refractory psychosis remitted upon treatment with continuous positive airway pressure: a case report. Psychopharmacol Bull. 2007;40: 113–7. Singareddy RK, Balon R. Sleep and suicide in psychiatric patients. Ann Clin Psychiatry. 2001;13:93–101. Thyfault JP, Cree MG, Zheng G, et al. Contraction of insulin-resistant muscle normalizes insulin action in association with increased mitochondrial activity and fatty acid catabolism. Am J Physiol Cell Physiol. 2007;292: C729–C39. Colcombe SJ, Kramer AF, Erikson KI, et al. Cardiovascular fitness, cortical plasticity, and aging. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2004;101:3316–3321.