The presynaptic ribbon maintains vesicle populations at the ... - eLife

0 downloads 0 Views 3MB Size Report
synapses, yet its role in information transfer to spiral ganglion neurons (SGNs) ... at the active zone the ribbon may support MVR, occurring through various .... In the WT the average vesicle diameter was 33.6 ± 3 nm (n = 21) and in the. 148 ..... Intracellular Ca2+ buffering can alter release properties depending on the relative.
1

The presynaptic ribbon maintains vesicle populations at the hair cell afferent fiber

2

synapse

3

Lars Becker1, Michael E. Schnee1, Mamiko Niwa1*, Willy Sun2, Stephan Maxeiner3**,

4

Sara Talaei1, Bechara Kachar2, Mark A. Rutherford4, Anthony J. Ricci1,3

5

1 Department of Otolaryngology Stanford University, United States, 2 National Institute

6

of Deafness and Communicative Disorders, United States, 3 Molecular and Cellular

7

Physiology, Stanford University, United States, 4 Department of Otolaryngology,

8

Washington University in St. Louis, United States,

9

*Present address: Department of Otolaryngology Johns Hopkins University, United

10

States

11

** Present address: Department of Neuroanatomy, Institute for Anatomy and Cell

12

Biology, Medical School Saarland University, Germany

13

14

15

16

17

18

19

1

20

Abstract: The ribbon is the structural hallmark of cochlear inner hair cell (IHC) afferent

21

synapses, yet its role in information transfer to spiral ganglion neurons (SGNs) remains

22

unclear. We investigated the ribbon’s contribution to IHC synapse formation and

23

function using KO mice lacking RIBEYE. Despite loss of the entire ribbon structure,

24

synapses retained their spatiotemporal development and KO mice had a mild hearing

25

deficit. IHCs of KO had fewer synaptic vesicles and reduced exocytosis in response to

26

brief depolarization; high stimulus level rescued exocytosis in KO. SGNs exhibited a

27

lack of sustained excitatory postsynaptic currents (EPSCs). We observed larger

28

postsynaptic glutamate receptor plaques, potentially in compensation for the reduced

29

EPSC rate in KO. Surprisingly, large amplitude EPSCs were maintained in KO, while a

30

small population of low amplitude slower EPSCs was increased in number. The ribbon

31

facilitates signal transduction at physiological stimulus levels by retaining a larger

32

residency pool of synaptic vesicles.

33

34

35

Key words: auditory hair cell, synaptic transmission, RIBEYE, glutamate receptor,

36

multivesicular release

37

38

39

40

2

41

Introduction

42

The synaptic ribbon is an electron dense structure associated with presynaptic active

43

zones at sensory synapses in the retina, lateral line and inner ear organs. RIBEYE, the

44

major protein associated with the ribbon (Zanazzi and Matthews 2009), consists of a

45

unique amino-terminal A domain that forms the ribbon scaffold and a carboxy-terminal B

46

domain with lysophosphatidic acid acyltransferase activity which is almost identical to

47

CtBP2, a transcriptional co-repressor (Schmitz, Konigstorfer et al. 2000). The ribbon is

48

anchored to the presynaptic membrane by bassoon protein in mammalian IHCs

49

(Khimich, Nouvian et al. 2005) as well as retinal photoreceptors and bipolar cells (dick

50

2001). Although it is comprised predominantly of RIBEYE (Zenisek, Horst et al. 2004),

51

the ribbon complex may associate with over 30 different synaptic proteins (Uthaiah and

52

Hudspeth 2010, Kantardzhieva, Peppi et al. 2012).

53

IHCs respond to sound with graded receptor potentials that drive rapid and precise

54

synaptic release, encoding intensity and timing information (Fuchs 2005, Matthews and

55

Fuchs 2010). The ribbon is thought to sustain continuous encoding by tethering a pool

56

of vesicles that replenishes the local pool of docked and primed vesicles upon

57

depletion. Other functions ascribed to the ribbon include: regulating vesicle trafficking

58

from the cytoplasm to the active zone, serving as a vesicle trap to bind freely diffusing

59

vesicles, a location for vesicle formation, a physical barrier for Ca2+ ensuring a high

60

local Ca2+ signal, and as a primary structural component of the synapse anchoring Ca2+

61

channels close to synaptic vesicle release sites (Parsons 1994, Frank, Rutherford et al.

62

2010, Graydon, Cho et al. 2011, Kantardzhieva, Peppi et al. 2012).

3

63

Postsynaptic EPSCs at ribbon synapses range in amplitude by up to 20X. The smallest

64

and largest EPSCs have similar kinetics, which together suggested the existence of a

65

mechanism to synchronize the release of multiple quanta (multivesicular release, MVR;

66

(Glowatzki and Fuchs 2002, Li, Keen et al. 2009, Schnee, Castellano-Munoz et al.

67

2013, Rudolph, Tsai et al. 2015). By holding vesicles and voltage-gated Ca2+ channels

68

at the active zone the ribbon may support MVR, occurring through various mechanisms

69

including: i) coordinated fusion of multiple single vesicles, ii) release of large pre-fused

70

vesicles, iii) rapid sequential fusion in which the 1st fusion event triggers fusion of

71

additional vesicles, or some combination of these mechanisms (Matthews and Fuchs

72

2010).

73

Previous attempts to study ribbon function used genetic disruption of the anchoring

74

protein Bassoon or acute destruction of the ribbon by fluorophore assisted light

75

inactivation (FALI). These manipulations resulted in smaller vesicle pool size, calcium

76

channel mislocalization, increased auditory thresholds, asynchrony in neural responses

77

and reductions in both tonic and phasic responses (Snellman, Mehta et al. 2011, Jing,

78

Rutherford et al. 2013). However, neither of those manipulations were selective for the

79

ribbon and so might be manifestations of accessory protein denaturing.

80

Recently, manipulation of the gene encoding CtBP2/RIBEYE allowed for deletion of

81

RIBEYE by removing the A-domain of ctbp2 to eliminate ribbons in mice, enabling a

82

more precise examination of ribbon function. This mouse will be referred to as KO and

83

the littermate controls as WT throughout the manuscript. Elimination of ribbons in the

84

retina reduced phasic and tonic transmission and rendered release sites sensitive to the

85

slow calcium buffer EGTA (Maxeiner, Luo et al. 2016). Here we present analysis of 4

86

cochlear synaptic anatomy, hair cell and afferent fiber physiology and hearing function

87

in the RIBEYE knockout mouse. Results are distinct from those in retina and from a

88

RIBEYE deletion in zebrafish (Lv, Stewart et al. 2016) , suggesting a more subtle

89

function for the ribbon than previously assumed.

90

Results:

91

IHC-Spiral Ganglia Neuron (SGN) synapses are formed and maintained despite

92

absence of the ribbon: The inner ear structure of the KO was assessed first for the

93

presence of synaptic ribbons and synapses (Maxeiner, Luo et al. 2016). Validating loss

94

of hair cell ribbons was accomplished using immunohistochemistry for the RIBEYE B-

95

domain (CtBP2). Anti-CtBP2 labelled the ribbons in wildtype (WT) but not in the KO

96

animals (postnatal day 21, P21)(Fig. 1A). Nuclear CtBP2 was still observed in both WT

97

and KO (not shown) because the KO is specific to the RIBEYE A-domain. The nuclear

98

staining served as a usful control for antibody function.

99

Using antibodies to Homer for postsynaptic labeling and Bassoon for presynaptic

100

labeling (Fig. 1B,C) we quantified the number of puncta and whether puncta were

101

juxtaposed as synaptic pairs (Fig. 1I,J). Bassoon and Homer puncta were similar in

102

number between WT and KO (Bassoon: 40 ± 15 in WT, vs. 43 ± 12 in KO, p = 0.76;

103

Homer: 17 ± 3 for both WT and KO; WT: n = 20 mice, KO: n = 19 mice, p = 0.64). The

104

number of paired Homer and Bassoon puncta (Fig. 1D) were similarly comparable:

105

14 ± 2 (n = 20) for WT and 13 ± 2 (n = 19, p = 0.18) for KO IHCs, suggesting that

106

synapses were formed and maintained in comparable numbers regardless of the

107

presence of ribbons. The higher number of Bassoon puncta likely reflects its localization

108

at efferent synapses. Figure 1- Supplemental Fig. 1 presents a lower magnification 5

109

image that illustrates the multiple roles of Bassoon at the auditory periphery. Bassoon is

110

a major component of the presynaptic elements at the auditory periphery including the

111

efferent fibers and the hair cell afferent synapses. Thus it is not surprising that paired

112

Bassoon puncta are much less than total puncta because Bassoon serves multiple

113

synapses. Similarly, when interpreting Bassoon KO data it is not surprising that its

114

phenotype may be more dramatic than simply the RIBEYE effect alone due to number

115

of synapses involved and schaffolding function of Bassoon that leads to structural

116

changes at the synapse.

117

RIBEYE KO in zebrafish revealed a non osmiophilic structure resembling a ribbon that

118

maintained organization of synaptic vesicles in the absence of the RIBEYE protein in

119

hair cells of the lateral line neuromasts (Lv, Stewart et al. 2016). We examined whether

120

such a structure existed in the mammalian cochlea in the IHCs of the KO using

121

transmission electron microscopy (TEM) and electron-tomography. Representative

122

examples of conventional TEM (Fig. 1E, 70 nm sections) and tomographic

123

reconstructions (Fig. 1F,G at 10 nm tomographic sections) are presented. The KO

124

presynaptic synapses consistently showed a small osmiophilic density in contact with

125

the pre-synaptic membrane at the center of the synapse that resembled the arciform

126

density seen at ribbon synapses in the retina. This density may be the Bassoon

127

enriched linker between the membrane and the ribbon. Interestingly, this density is not

128

observed in the Bassoon KO mouse (Frank et al., 2010). Tomographic reconstructions,

129

(Fig. 1G, right) clearly show the presynaptic density, which is smaller than the ribbon

130

and is present also at ribbon synapses in WT IHCs (Fig. 1G, left). We found no spatial

131

organization of vesicles near active zones in KO IHCs, suggestive of the ‘ghost’ ribbons

6

132

observed in the zebrafish lateral-line hair cells (Lv, Stewart et al. 2016). Thus, in the KO

133

mouse, IHCs lack synaptic ribbons and have no obvious compensatory structures. We

134

surmise that the differences observed in the zebrafish model are a function of the KO

135

being incomplete or there being a compensation mechanism in zebrafish, not found in

136

mammal.

137

KO IHCs may upregulate a similar protein to replace RIBEYE loss. CtBP1 is a likely

138

candidate because it is similar to the RIBEYE-B domain, is expressed in hair cells and

139

localizes to the synaptic region (Kantardzhieva, Liberman et al. 2013). Antibody labeling

140

(Fig. 1H,K,L) demonstrates the presence of CtBP1 in both WT and KO animals which

141

overlapped with Homer in WT. A reduction in puncta number was identified in the KO

142

animal due to loss of localization at the afferent synapses (Fig. 1K,L). Thus, the CtBP1

143

localization at the synapse requires the presence of RIBEYE, and CtBP1 does not

144

substitute for RIBEYE in the KO animal.

145

Synaptic vesicles are normal in size but fewer in number:

Vesicle diameters

146

(distance between the outer edges of the bilayer in each vesicle) were measured in the

147

reconstructed tomograms. We measured in both X and Y directions with no differences

148

observed. In the WT the average vesicle diameter was 33.6 ± 3 nm (n = 21) and in the

149

KO it was 33.8 ± 3 nm (n = 21).

150

Synaptic vesicles were conspicuously fewer in number the KO. Vesicles were counted

151

in volumes around the center of the reconstructed synapses (Fig. 1G), where one

152

boundary was the presynaptic membrane. The volumes had XY dimensions of 453 ± 6

153

nm for WT (n = 3) and 569 ± 117 nm for KO (n = 11), and Z dimensions of 88 ± 3 and

154

95 ± 10 nm for WT and KO, respectively. Total vesicle numbers were 20 ± 2 for WT and 7

155

7 ± 3 for KO, significantly greater for WT compared with KO synapses (p < 0.001, Welch

156

correction for unequal variance). Our TEM data is qualitatively similar to that reported in

157

the companion paper and supports the conclusion that ribbons are not present in the

158

KO.

159

Auditory nerve activity is reduced in absence of synaptic ribbons: Previous

160

investigation of ribbonless synapses in the KO mouse focused on the retina and did not

161

assess function at the systems level (Maxeiner et al., 2016). To determine how hearing

162

was affected, we measured ABRs and distortion product otoacoustic emissions

163

(DPOAEs). DPOAEs are a direct measure of outer hair cell function and cochlear

164

amplification. DPOAEs are not expected to be affected by absence of RIBEYE and

165

serve as a control for developmental acquisition of mature cochlear mechanics. DPOAE

166

threshold measurements were not different across frequencies from 4 - 46 kHz (Fig.

167

2A). Data are presented in decibels (dB) as referenced to sound pressure level (SPL)

168

with lower values indicating enhanced sensitivity in the mid-cochlea. Normal DPOAEs

169

indicate that cochlear amplification was not effected mechanically by the loss of outer

170

hair cell ribbons, suggesting that sound-driven activation of IHCs was intact, and that

171

any differences in auditory nerve activity should result from changes at the IHC afferent

172

synapses.

173

Wave 1 of the ABR is a direct measurement of the synchronized firing of SGNs at

174

sound onset, triggered by synaptic output from cochlear IHCs. The subsequent waves

175

of the ABR reflect the ensuing activation of brainstem nuclei in the ascending auditory

176

pathway (Zheng, Henderson et al. 1999). ABR thresholds were elevated by

177

approximately 10 dB at all probe frequencies in KO mice (Fig. 2B), except for 48 kHz 8

178

where hearing threholds are already relatively high. The variance in our control data

179

ranged from 8-12dB depending on frequency; this dictated the sample size needed to

180

detect a 5% change as statistically different. ABR threshold was identified as the

181

stimulus intensity required to evoke a voltage response 5x the RMS noise floor for the

182

measurement. The companion paper found a similar elevation in threshold though it did

183

not reach statistical significance. The difference is likely due to sampling size where we

184

powered our sample sized based on variance found in control measurements. The ABR

185

waveform (Fig. 2C: 23 kHz, 60 dB) shows a large reduction in wave 1 amplitude with no

186

effect on the subsequent peaks. This was surprising and suggests considerable

187

plasticity in central processing of data translated from cochlea periphery. KO mice had

188

smaller wave 1 peak amplitudes, for all sound levels that evoked a response (Fig. 2D).

189

When wave 1 growth functions were plotted relative to the maximal response amplitude

190

within genotype, the amplitude versus level function for KO scaled up to overlap with

191

WT (Fig. 2E). Considering wave 1 amplitude as a fraction of the maximal amplitude, the

192

overlap of the normalized wave 1 growth curves suggested that loss of maximum output

193

could produce an elevation of threshold in the ABR measurement that does not

194

necessarily reflect a sound detection limit or a change in sensitivity of the auditory

195

system. Indeed, normal ABR amplitudes beyond wave 1 indicate compensation in the

196

cochlear nucleus. Future work should determine if KO mice have increased behavioral

197

thresholds.

198

In mice lacking Bassoon function, where IHCs lacked most of their membrane-anchored

199

ribbons, there was an increase in wave 1 latency (Khimich, Nouvian et al. 2005) and in

200

first-spike latency in recordings of SGNs in vivo (Buran, Strenzke et al. 2010). ABR

9

201

wave 1 latencies were not different between WT and KO at any stimulus intensity (Fig.

202

2F), suggesting a milder phenotype in the KO as compared to Bassoon.

203

As the ABR measurements are averaged responses of 260 stimulus repetitions, a

204

possible explanation for the reduced ABR wave 1 amplitude in KO is that the response

205

reduces over time. If the ribbon is providing a means of maintaining vesicles near the

206

synapse, the concomitant loss of vesicles at ribbonless synapses might reduce the

207

ongoing response to repetitive stimulation. Comparison of ABR amplitudes where the

208

number of repeats was varied from 260 to 1000 showed no difference in amplitudes, nor

209

did repeated measurements meant to evoke depletion.Thus, rundown alone is unlikely

210

to account for the decrease in ABR wave 1.

211

Characterization of molecular synaptic components indicates an increase in distribution: To begin identifying cellular

212

postsynaptic glutamate receptor

213

mechanisms that might underlie the measured reduction in ABR wave 1 amplitude we

214

investigated the distribution of presynaptic Ca2+ channels and postsynaptic glutamate

215

receptors as indicators of functional synapses. Figure 3 presents immunohistochemical

216

localizations of presynaptic ribbons with anti-CtBP2, presynaptic voltage-gated Ca2+

217

channels with anti-CaV1.3, and postsynaptic AMPA-type glutamate receptors with anti-

218

GluA3 in P35 mice organ of corti. There was no difference in the number of puncta

219

between WT and KO for CaV1.3 (20 ± 6, n = 9 vs. 18 ± 5, n = 13, p = 0.51) or GluA3 (15

220

± 3, n = 9 vs. 15 ± 2, n = 13, p = 0.43) (Fig. 4A,B). There was also no difference in the

221

number of paired synaptic puncta between genotypes, with values of 14 ± 2 (n = 9) for

222

WT and 13 ± 2 (n = 13) for KO (p = 0.13). Thus, the presence of the ribbon did not

223

influence the number of apparently functional synapses per IHC. 10

224

Careful inspection of GluA3 immunoreactivity showed that the glutamate receptor

225

puncta were larger on average in the KO (Fig. 4D). Puncta volumes were measured for

226

presynaptic markers CaV1.3 and Bassoon as well as postsynaptic markers Homer and

227

GluA3 (Fig. 4C). Like GluA3, Homer puncta were significantly larger (KS-test,

228

p ≤ 0.0001) on average in the KO (Fig. 4D). The volumes of CaV1.3 and Bassoon

229

distributions were very similar among genotypes however the volume of Bassoon was

230

significantly higher in KO. Most WT and KO synapses exhibited the typical elongated

231

stripe-like appearance of CaV1.3 puncta. However, we noted a tendency for KO

232

synapses to have CaV1.3 clusters that were fragmented into smaller puncta (Fig. 3C,

233

arrow; see also Companion Paper). These anatomical data support the hypothesis that

234

the ribbon is not a major regulator of synapse formation or stability; however, it does

235

perhaps suggest that postsynaptic receptors are sensitive to activity and the increased

236

plaque size might reflect a compensation for reduced activity.

237

Basolateral IHC properties: We compared the properties of the IHC voltage-gated

238

Ca2+ current (Fig. 5), which drives exocytosis. We first blocked outward K+ currents

239

using Cs+ and tetraethylammonium (TEA) in the internal solution to isolate Ca2+ currents

240

in WT and KO IHCs (Fig. 5A,B). Current-voltage plots showed no difference in voltage

241

of half-activation (V1/2) or maximum current amplitude (Fig.5C-E). Half activation

242

voltages were -34 ± 0.3 mV (n = 18, WT) and -33.4 ± 0.3 mV (n = 26, KO) for animals

243

P10-P13. The companion paper found small changes in voltage dependent properties

244

of the calcium channels at older ages and in higher external calcium; differences that

245

might suggest compensatory effects of ribbon loss or later specializations being ribbon

246

dependent. Examples of current elicited with K+ as the major internal cation and without

11

247

TEA are presented in Fig. 5F,G. Recordings were made at P20-24. No differences were

248

observed in maximal current or in V1/2 (Fig. 5H). Zero-current potentials monitored in

249

current-clamp mode (Fig. 5I) were not different, with means of -63 ± 10 mV (n = 5, WT)

250

and -65 ± 8 mV (n = 8, KO).

251

Release is slower and smaller for naturalistic stimuli in absence of the ribbon: We

252

measured presynaptic exocytosis with the two-sine wave method for tracking vesicular

253

fusion by monitoring real time changes in cellular membrane capacitance (Cm) from

254

IHCs (Santos-Sacchi 2004, Schnee, Castellano-Munoz et al. 2011). The two-sine

255

method allows continuous monitoring of Cm during the depolarizing stimulus as

256

compared to a before and after monitoring of capacitance changes, which is particularly

257

helpful during smaller, longer depolarizations (Schnee, Santos-Sacchi et al. 2011).

258

Typically, small depolarizations lead to relatively slow, linear increases in Cm during

259

depolarization. In contrast, larger depolarizations induce an abrupt departure from

260

linearity termed the ‘superlinear’ response (Schnee et al. 2011). Both types of

261

responses were present in recordings from IHCs of WT and KO mice alike (Fig. 6A,B).

262

From Cm measurements, we estimated the sizes of synaptic vesicle pools, their rates of

263

release and the Ca2+-dependence of release. In response to strong depolarizations, we

264

found no difference in maximal release between genotypes for either the linear or

265

superlinear release components (Fig. 6C). The linear component was 52 ± 24 fF (n =

266

15, WT) compared to 42 ± 22 fF (n = 15, KO); and the superlinear component of release

267

was 223 ± 79 fF (n = 17, WT) compared to 253 ± 116 fF (n = 17, KO). Likewise, no

268

differences in release rates (Fig. 6D) were observed for the linear component: 101 ± 72

269

fF/s (n = 15, WT) compared to 75 ± 50 fF/s (n = 14, KO) or for the superlinear 12

270

component: 256 ± 164 fF/s (n = 17, WT) and 253 ± 117 fF/s (n = 18, KO). In addition,

271

the Ca2+-dependence of release for the linear component was not different (0.9 ± 0.03

272

fF/pC (n = 37, WT) as compared to 0.93 ± 0.03 fF/pC (n = 34, KO). The voltage at which

273

the superlinear component of release first appears was also not different between mice

274

(Fig. 6E). Fitting the data with a simple Gaussian function gave values of -40 ± 9 mV for

275

WT and -41 ± 3 mV for KO for the center values. However the width of the functions

276

were different, with values of 7.6 ± 2 for WT compared to 14.3 ± 3 for KO (values p
-

299

39 mV) resulted in no difference in release latency. The time constants of endocytosis

300

were not significantly different (6.0 ± 4s, n = 13, WT and 7 ± 5 s, n = 13, KO). Together

301

these data suggest that fewer vesicles were available for release at ribbonless

302

synapses, but that with stronger depolarizations or longer time courses of depolarization

303

the recruitment of additional vesicles masked this immediate difference in release

304

latency and amplitude.

305

The ribbon may be a barrier to Ca2+ diffusion, creating a restricted space for sharp Ca2+

306

nanodomains beneath the ribbon (Roberts 1994, Graydon, Cho et al. 2011).

307

Intracellular Ca2+ buffering can alter release properties depending on the relative

308

distribution of Ca2+ channels with release sites. In retina the KO increases buffering

309

efficacy (Maxeiner et al. 2016), suggesting looser coupling between Ca2+ channels and

310

release sites. To investigate the effects of ribbon absence on stimulus-secretion

311

coupling in p11 to p13 IHCs, we compared between genotypes the effects on release

312

properties of changing concentration of the slow Ca2+ buffer EGTA (Fig. 7). Examples of

313

release obtained with either 0.1 or 5 mM EGTA are presented in Fig. 7A (control

314

experiments all use 1 mM EGTA). Although increasing buffer concentration reduced

315

release and lowering buffer increased release, the relative changes were comparable

14

316

between WT and KO mice for release measured at 1 sec (Fig. 7B). Data previously

317

demonstrated that increasing Ca2+ buffering increased the time to superlinear release

318

(Schnee, Santos-Sacchi et al. 2011). Fig. 7C demonstrates that this effect is retained in

319

the KO and that there are no major differences between WT and KO modulation by

320

Ca2+ buffering. These results contrast with findings from retinal bipolar cells and in the

321

zebrafish KO (Maxeiner, Luo et al. 2016); see Discussion). Similarly, release rates

322

measured for steps to -45 mV were not significantly different for the initial release

323

components (Fig. 7D). We further investigated the first component of release, typically

324

thought to represent vesicles in early release pools, not requiring trafficking. Comparing

325

release at two voltages again showed that the buffer greatly altered release properties

326

but not in a differential way between genotypes. However, it should be noted that the

327

difference in early release observed in Fig. 6 with 1 mM EGTA was lost with 0.1 mM

328

EGTA. Thus, it is possible that a sensitivity exists that is below a clear detection limit for

329

our capacitance measurements.

330

EPSCs are more heterogeneous in size and kinetics in the absence of the ribbon:

331

Cm measurements are a powerful tool for investigating whole-cell exocytosis but they

332

lack sensitivity (1 fF reflects fusion of ~20 vesicles), they reflect information from all

333

synapses and they can be quite variable. To obtain a more refined view of activity from

334

individual synapses we recorded postsynaptically, from the afferent fiber bouton. Hair

335

cells bathed in artificial perilymph lack the standing inward current through

336

mechanoelectric transduction channels in hair bundles, causing the resting membrane

337

potential to be relatively hyperpolarized (Farris, Wells et al. 2006, Johnson, Kennedy et

338

al. 2012). To depolarize hair cells and increase the rate of vesicle fusion, we applied a

15

339

high potassium (40 mM K+) solution to depolarize the hair cell. K+ application to the hair

340

cell in current-clamp mode depolarized hair cells by an average of 32 ± 5 mV (n = 8;

341

Fig. 8A). There was no difference in depolarization between genotype thus allowing

342

conclusions regarding postsynaptic responses to be ascribed to presynaptic effects.

343

Afferent boutons of type I auditory nerve fibers were recorded ex vivo at ages of P17-

344

P21. Due to the approach vector of the recording pipette, recordings were made from

345

boutons located on the side of the IHC facing the ganglion (modiolar face) rather than

346

the pillar cell side. The start of the high K+ response was defined when the mean EPSC

347

rate exceeded the rate in normal external solution by 2 standard deviations. Examples

348

of postsynaptic responses to K+ applications are provided in Figure 8B-D. Two types of

349

responses were observed, sustained responses (Fig. 8B,C) and transient responses

350

(Fig. 8D). WT responses were mainly of the sustained type (7 of 8) while the KO

351

responses had a smaller proportion of sustained responses (7 of 11). EPSC rates are

352

plotted for transient (Fig. 8E) and sustained responses (Fig. 8F) for both WT and KO.

353

EPSC rates in 40 mM K+ perilymph were quantified over one second windows at 100

354

ms intervals and the means are displayed as thicker lines (Fig. 8F). The peak release

355

rates varied considerably across fiber populations (0-120 s-1) and there was a tendency

356

for the KO neurons to reach a lower peak EPSC rate on average: 57 ± 30 s-1, (n = 11,

357

WT) compared with 44 ± 31 s-1, (n = 18, KO) (Fig. 8G).

358

The time to peak EPSC rate was less variable than absolute rates (Fig. 8H). The KO

359

had one outlier that steadily increased throughout the K+ application, removal of this

360

point from the statistical analysis resulted in the KO reaching peak EPSC rate sooner (9

361

± 10 sec, n = 10) than WT (21 ± 12 sec, n = 9; p = 0.037). Observing the sustained 16

362

release group shows that both WT and KO reduced EPSC rate over time but that the

363

KO reduced more completely than WT (Fig. 8G). 8/11 KO reduced to less than 5% of

364

peak as compared to 3/8 WT. Integrating the rates over time (Fig. 8I) confirmed an

365

increase in total EPSCs in WT (1.8 ± 1.1 x 103) compared to KO (0.81 ± 0.66 x 103, p =

366

0.021).

367

EPSC amplitude distributions were generated for each recorded fiber. Frequency

368

histograms of EPSC amplitudes could be fit with either a double Gaussian function or a

369

Gaussian and gamma function where the smallest peak was always fit by a Gaussian

370

function (e.g. Fig. 8J,K). The area under the smaller peak was calculated and plotted as

371

a percentage of the total in Fig. 8L, where the KO showed a statistically larger (p =

372

0.034) percentage of small events with 5.5 ± 5.6% (n = 12) as compared to 1 ± 0.7% (n

373

= 7). The increase in number of small events might suggest less coordination in release

374

because there are fewer vesicles filling release sites. It might also suggest that smaller

375

responses are visible because of an increased in receptor plaque size as well. Only

376

fibers with more than 100 EPSCs were included for this analysis. No difference was

377

found in the proportion of simple events for each unit, where a simple event is described

378

as having a single rise and decay phase (Fig. 8M). We also compared the median

379

amplitudes (Fig. 8N). Although there was a tendency for EPSCs in the KO to be greater

380

in amplitude 212 ± 46 pA (n = 13) compared to 173 ± 36 pA (n = 7), the values were not

381

statistically different. A difference here would support the argument of a postsynaptic

382

compensatory increase in glutamate receptor plaque size. The coefficient of variation

383

(CV) for the amplitude distributions were different on average (p = 0.017; Fig. 8O). The

384

control values are similar to those obtained by (Li, Keen et al. 2009, Grant, Yi et al.

17

385

2010). Recordings with a larger proportion of smaller events had larger CV. When small

386

events were removed from the histogram, the CVs were not statistically different

387

between KO and WT. The postsynaptic responses in KO also had a greater proportion

388

of EPSCs > 350 pA (Fig. 8J,K, 9D): 1.3 ± 1.3% in WT compared to 8.0 ± 10% for KO

389

EPSCs (t-test for unequal variances, p = 0.04). The change in amplitude was not

390

accompanied by a change in kinetics. The increased amplitude might reflect the

391

increase in postsynaptic glutamate receptor plaque (Fig. 3,4).

392

Examples of EPSCs are presented in Fig. 9A to show the variation in amplitudes and to

393

identify a smaller population of EPSCs with slower kinetics (see inset). As the ribbon is

394

proposed to be involved with multivesicular release (MVR), and the kinetic similarities

395

between large and small EPSCs are a signature of MVR, we investigated the decay of

396

EPSCs in auditory nerve fiber recordings from WT and KO animals. Fitting the EPSC

397

decay with single exponentials provided time constants (Fig. 9B). Calculating the CVs of

398

EPSC decay time constants (Fig. 9C) revealed more variability in KO but no significant

399

difference compared to WT. The skewness (Fig. 9F) of the decay time constant

400

distributions was significantly larger for the KO (0.46 ± 0.2, n = 13) compared to WT

401

(0.29 ± 0.07, n=7; p = 0.04); the KO mean EPSC decay time was greater than the

402

median decay time. As a population 0.44% of WT EPSCs were < 100 pA in amplitude

403

with decay time constants > 1 ms while 1.9% of KO events fell in that category. Plotting

404

the decay time constant against amplitude for each EPSC in all fibers (Fig. 9D) (WT: n =

405

7 recordings, 15,564 EPSCs; KO: n = 13 recordings, 17,210 EPSCs) identifies a

406

population of small slow EPSCs in the KO that are present in greater proportion than in

407

WT (upper left quadrant). The mean EPSC decay time constants plotted as a function of

18

408

EPSC amplitude for each recording (bin of 10 pA) illustrates that all the KO fibers had a

409

proportion of EPSCs with slow time courses while the WT have only 2 fibers with these

410

slower events (Fig. 9E). An example of the smaller, slower EPSC is provided in the

411

inset of Fig. 9A. The distributions of EPSC durations for all EPSCs < 100 pA (Fig. 9G,H)

412

demonstrates the shift toward slower longer duration EPSCs in KO fibers (p > 0.001,

413

using the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test). The cumulative probability functions of EPSC

414

duration for all fibers by genotype (thick red and blue lines) illustrates the larger

415

proportion of slow EPSCs in the KO fibers (Fig. 9H). These data may also support the

416

argument for a larger postsynaptic glutamate receptor plaque, where diffusion of

417

glutamate, coupled with the lower glutamate concentration might serve to slow the

418

decay kinetics.

419

Discussion:

420

The KO mouse provides an unprecedented opportunity to investigate the functional

421

relevance of synaptic ribbons in driving information transfer at the primary auditory

422

synapse. Synapse formation and stability appeared normal based on synapse counts

423

and protein localization, despite complete absence of the ribbon (Figs. 1, 3-4). No

424

evidence was found for partial or defective ribbons structures, nor did we detect

425

compensation by other proteins in the absence of RIBEYE (Fig. 1). Also, cochlear

426

mechanics appeared normal as assessed by DPOAE (Fig. 2). Thus, this mouse

427

provides an excellent model to investigate the ribbon’s role in synaptic transmission.

428

Whole animal function: ABR responses reported a 10 dB threshold shift and about a

429

50% reduction in ABR amplitude with no significant effect on the shape of the

430

waveforms or on the delay (Fig. 2). These data agree qualitatively with the companion 19

431

paper data, the lack of significance likely being a function of the sample size. The minor

432

auditory deficit may not be expected to produce a change in behavioral sound detection

433

threshold as the ensuing ABR peaks, representing brainstem nuclei, were unaffected.

434

Alternatively, central compensation might occur that mask peripheral deficits. These

435

data suggest that the ribbon is not essential for setting hearing threshold, and that its

436

role in auditory function is more nuanced than for example otoferlin which when absent

437

or altered leads to deafness with threshold elevations of more than 100 dB (Roux,

438

Safieddine et al. 2006, Pangrsic, Lasarow et al. 2010). Further work is needed to better

439

understand ribbon function at the systems level. Additional testing to better investigate

440

auditory processing such as sound localization experiments may provide additional

441

insight into how the ribbon might serve systems level function.

442

Synapse formation: Synapse formation is surprisingly normal with appropriate number

443

of synapses and synaptic markers present (Figs. 1,4). Little change in Ca2+ channel

444

distribution or biophysics were observed in the KO (Fig. 5) relative to Bassoon

445

disruption, suggesting that Ca2+ channel distribution or regulation does not depend

446

critically on the ribbon (Frank, Rutherford et al. 2010). Our companion paper identifies

447

minor changes in Ca2+ channel distribution and voltage sensitivity at later ages; whether

448

these are a function of ribbon absence or activity dependent modulation remains to be

449

explored. The lack of effect on calcium channel properties and distributions at early

450

ages supports the hypothesis of a compensatory mechanism occurring later during

451

synapse maturation. Given the reports of late development changes in ribbon size and

452

glutamate receptor plaque sizes, activity driven shaping of the synapses is a reasonable

453

expectation (Fernandez, Jeffers et al. 2015). Postsynaptically, both Homer and the

20

454

glutamate receptors had larger volumes in the KO animal than in WT (Fig. 4). This

455

change also may be an indication of activity dependent synaptic shaping where the

456

reduced activity in the KO animal results in a postsynaptic up regulation of receptors.

457

The increase in the population of larger amplitude EPSC’s in KO animals further

458

supports this conclusion (Figs 8I,J, 9D).

459

Comparison to other systems: In zebrafish a frameshift mutation was used to

460

eliminate ribeye from ribbon synapses. This work revealed ghost like ribbons where

461

vesicles were organized about invisible ribbon-like structures (Lv C et al.,2016). Neither

462

our investigations, nor the companion paper nor the original work investigating the

463

retina (Maxeiner et al., 2015) found comparable ghosts in this mouse. We would

464

conclude that either the frame shift approach did not account for all ribeye or there is a

465

strong compensatory mechanism in zebrafish that is not found or is much weaker in

466

mammal. We investigated CTBP1 as a potential compensatory molecule but as Fig. 1

467

shows there is no labeling near synapses in the KO mouse. The presence of ribbons in

468

photoreceptors of zebrafish support the idea that there are fundamental differences

469

between zebrafish and mammalian systems. Additionally, work in zebrafish suggests a

470

tight reciprocal relationship between calcium channel numbers, calcium current

471

amplitude and ribbon size (Lv, Stewart et al. 2016). As no changes in calcium channel

472

distribution or numbers were observed in the KO animal despite the loss of the ribbon,

473

this relationship does not appear to exist in mammal or is greatly reduced, again

474

supporting fundamental differences between model organisms.

475

Mechanism of ABR amplitude reduction: The decrease in ABR amplitude is likely a

476

function of the reduced frequency of EPSCs leading to fewer action potentials 21

477

generated (Fig. 8F). In addition, the increase in proportion of EPSCs with lower

478

amplitudes and longer durations (Fig. 9H) is expected to increase the likelihood of spike

479

generation failure (Rutherford, Chapochnikov et al. 2012). As larger EPSCs are better

480

phase-locked than smaller EPSCs (Li, Cho et al. 2014) and present works finds a 5-

481

10% increase in smaller EPSCs one might predict a lower vector strength and reduced

482

phase locking. Our companion paper in fact does observe these differences in single

483

unit firing. Similarly the increased population of smaller EPSCs is reminiscent of

484

observations in young animals when compared to older animals and so might reflect a

485

developmental delay in the KO animal. Fewer fibers fire together at sound onset,

486

reducing auditory nerve synchrony and wave 1 amplitude (Fig 2C,D). Measurements of

487

exocytosis from IHCs in response to small depolarizations, clearly showed a reduction

488

in release and an increase in time to release in the KO (Fig. 6E,F). This suggests a

489

decrease in vesicles available at the synapse for immediate release (supported by TEM

490

counts). The reduction in available vesicles for release as well as the population of

491

reduced amplitude EPSCs might also predict a spike timing deficit as described in the

492

companion paper. However, the more rapid response in WT was masked by stronger

493

stimulation (Fig. 6F), indicating that vesicle replenishment remained robust despite the

494

absence of the ribbon-associated synaptic vesicle pool. Given the robust nature of

495

vesicle trafficking, the primary role for the ribbon maybe in ensuring a population of

496

vesicles is present for the timing of onset responses, whereas vesicle trafficking can

497

serve to replenish vesicle populations during sustained release. A similar result was

498

reported by (Maxeiner, Luo et al., 2016) in retina.

22

499

Postsynaptic recordings suggest that the KO reaches peak release sooner than WT and

500

to a similar peak rate, implying that vesicle numbers are comparable for immediate

501

release. In contrast, TEM suggests fewer vesicles available at the synapse and the

502

small increase in population of small EPSCs also suggests a reduction in filled release

503

sites. Thus, it is possible that the ribbon acts as a barrier for vesicle interaction with the

504

release site while simultaneously serving to increase the total vesicles number

505

available. Interestingly, this idea was first suggested by (Furukawa and Matsuura 1978)

506

and later supported by (Schnee, Castellano-Munoz et al. 2013) as a possible

507

mechanism for explaining neural adaptation. However, the companion paper suggests

508

modifications at the synapse that might also be in accord with the postsynaptic

509

measurements here as well, i.e. more vesicles at the membrane. If there is an increase

510

in presynaptic density area and more vesicles per active zone, it might be predicted that

511

faster immediate release occurs but sustainability might be reduced, EPSC data also

512

supports this idea. To resolve these possibililities we need a better understanding of the

513

molecular underpinnings of the active zone and number of available release sites.

514

Synaptic vesicle replenishment for sustained release: Our data would argue against

515

a role for the ribbon in vesicle formation and replenishment. Total exocytosis, from both

516

the linear and superlinear components, was not reduced in the KO, implying a similar

517

total number of vesicles that could be recruited for release (Figs. 6A,B and 7A). In

518

contrast to zebrafish lateral line (Lv, Stewart et al. 2016), we measured no change in

519

vesicle size between KO and WT, indirectly supporting our view that the ribbon is not

520

regulating or involved in vesicle formation, a result corroborated by the companion

23

521

paper. Thus, no change in vesicle size, or maximal release supports the conclusion that

522

the ribbon is not involved in vesicle formation.

523

Ca2+ influx and coupling to exocytosis: The ribbon may act as a barrier to Ca2+

524

diffusion, creating a higher level of local [Ca2+] to synchronize vesicle fusion of multiple

525

vesicles (Graydon, Cho et al. 2011). In this case, the ribbon’s presence would reduce

526

the sensitivity of exocytosis to exogenous Ca2+ buffers (by elevating concentrations of

527

Ca2+). Likewise in retina from KO mice, EGTA reduced spontaneous release in the KO

528

but not in WT (Maxeiner, Luo et al., 2016). In contrast, our data shows that buffering

529

efficacy was not altered in the KO (Fig. 7), suggesting the ribbon does not play a major

530

role in regulation of intracellular Ca2+ concentration at IHC active zones. The similar

531

response between WT and KO might indicate a small effect though at a level below a

532

clear detection limit for capacitance measurements. It is possible that the geometric

533

differences and synaptic architecture between retina and cochlea alter the sensitivity to

534

Ca2+ buffers or that the effect noted in retina is indirect, for example EGTA-AM might

535

alter the presynaptic cell’s resting potential. Thus, our results are not fundamentally

536

different from (Maxeiner, Luo et al., 2016), both supporting the idea that the ribbon

537

serves to consolidate vesicles near to release sites as suggested by (von Gersdorff,

538

Vardi et al. 1996).

539

Ribbon and Multiquantal release: The underlying mechanism for multiquantal release

540

remains an open question but might be driven by simultaneous multiquantal or

541

multivesicular release (Glowatzki and Fuchs 2002). The majority of EPSCs measured in

542

the KO remain large and uniformly fast in onset and decay times and the EPSC

543

amplitude is not reduced suggesting that the ribbon does not play a pivotal role in 24

544

multivesicular release. Thus, mechanisms that involve sequential fusion of vesicles

545

along the ribbon and then release or rapid sequential vesicle release that piggyback

546

onto a release site from the ribbon are unlikely to be valid as these should be

547

dramatically altered by the loss of the ribbon. One remaining possibility is the

548

synchronous release of vesicles from multiple release sites. Although the ribbon is not

549

required for this mechanism, it may indirectly modulate this mechanism by influencing

550

the number of docked vesicles available for release. The increase in number of smaller

551

slower EPSCs likely arising from fewer docked vesicles available for synchronous

552

release may support a ribbon role; however, the small relative contribution of this

553

vesicle pool indicates a minor role. The phenomenon of large EPSC amplitude

554

variability may still be explained by a univesicular mechanism including a dynamic

555

fusion pore, which presumably would not require a ribbon (Chapochnikov, Takago et al.

556

2014). Where identified, the slow footlike response described by (Chapochnikov,

557

Takago et al. 2014) was not different between genotypes.

558

Comparison to Bassoon KO: In mice, the effects of KO are subtle compared with

559

disruption of Bassoon protein. Loss-of-function mutation in Bassoon led to a reduction in

560

the number of IHC ribbons associated with presynaptic elements, ABR threshold

561

elevation of 30 dB, longer ABR wave 1 latency and first-spike latency, and fewer Ca2+

562

channels at active zones (Khimich, Nouvian et al. 2005, Buran, Strenzke et al. 2010,

563

Frank, Rutherford et al. 2010, Jing, Rutherford et al. 2013). Thus, the effects of loss of

564

Bassoon protein (and membrane-anchored ribbons) were greater than loss of RIBEYE

565

protein, consistent with a more critical role for Bassoon in scaffolding synaptic proteins.

566

Also given Bassoon’s presence at efferent synaptic endings in the organ of Corti, it is

25

567

not surprising that the functional loss of Bassoon has a greater phenotype than that of

568

RIBEYE (Supplemental Fig. 1).

569

Summary: A central tenet for ribbon function is maintaining a large pool of vesicles

570

near the synapse. The ribbon may serve as a conveyor belt to both maintain and to

571

traffic vesicles to release sites (Parsons 1994, Nouvian, Beutner et al. 2006). Our data

572

suggests the ribbon increases the time a vesicle remains in the synaptic region. Robust

573

vesicle trafficking and replenishment either via recycling or transporting new vesicles to

574

the synaptic zone provides vesicles to the synaptic zone, but the time vesicles remain

575

near to release sites may be shortened in the absence of the ribbon serving to maintain

576

them close to the synapses. A model for this basic idea was proposed (Graydon, Zhang

577

et al. 2014) that reproduced data from bipolar cell synapses. The addition of a more

578

robust trafficking mechanism may allow this model to also reproduce hair cell synapses.

579

The increase in variance, whether measured as CV, skewness or simply standard

580

deviation in measured parameters such as time to release, release rate and EPSC

581

amplitude was greater for KO. The increased variance may simply reflect a nonuniform

582

population of vesicles near the synapse in the absence of the ribbon. Thus, the ribbon

583

may serve as a biological buffer, maintaining a uniform population of vesicles near

584

release sites.

585

Materials and Methods

586

Animals

587

Mice of both sexes from a mixed genetic background (C57bl6, 129/Ola) were bred from

588

heterozygous breeding pairs and genotyped by PCR for the knockout of the A-domain

589

in Ctbp2 (Maxeiner et al., 2016) while wild-type littermates served as controls. The 26

590

Administrative Panel on Laboratory Animal Care at Stanford University (APLAC

591

#14345) and Animal Care and use Committee and Washington University in St. Louis

592

approved all animal procedures. Experiments were performed blinded to genotype.

593

Auditory measurements:

594

distortion products of optoacoustic emissions (DPOAEs) in mice of postnatal day 21 as

595

previously described (Oghalai, 2004). Mice were anesthetized by administering

596

100 mg/kg ketamine and 10 mg/kg xylazine intraperitoneally and their body temperature

597

held constantly at 37°C (FHC, DC-temperature controller and heating pad) until they

598

fully recovered.

599

Sound stimuli were generated digitally by a self-written software (MATLAB 2009b,

600

MathWorks), controlling a custom-built acoustic system using a digital-to-analog

601

converter (National Instruments, NI BNC-2090A), a sound amplifier (TDT, SA1), and

602

two high frequency speakers (TDT, MF1). The speakers were connected to an ear bar

603

and calibrated in the ear canal prior to each experiment using a probe-tip microphone

604

(microphone type 4182, NEXUS conditioning amplifier, Brüel and Kjær).

605

Auditory Brainstem Responses (ABRs) were recorded by placing three needle

606

electrodes subcutaneously at the vertex, below the left ear, and a ground electrode

607

close to the tail. The signals were amplified 10,000 times using a biological amplifier

608

(Warner Instruments, DP-311) digitized at 10 kHz, and digitally band-pass filtered from

609

300 to 3,000 Hz. The stimulus for eliciting the ABR was a 5 ms sine wave tone pip with

610

cos2 envelope rise and fall times of 0.5 ms. The repetition time was 50 ms, and 260

611

trials were averaged.

We measured auditory brainstem responses (ABRs),

27

612

At each frequency, the peak to peak voltages of ABR signals at stimulus intensities

613

ranging from 10 to 80 dB sound pressure levels (SPL) were measured in 10 dB steps

614

and fitted and interpolated to find threshold five standard deviations above the noise

615

floor. For statistical purposes we defined threshold as 80 dB SPL, if no ABR was

616

detected.

617

Distortion Products of Otoacoustic Emission (DPOAE): For stimulation of DPOAEs

618

two sine wave tones of equal intensity (l1=l2) and 1-second duration were presented to

619

the ear. The tones ranged from 20 to 80 dB SPL attenuated in 10 dB increments and

620

the two frequencies (f2 = 1.2f1) ranged from f2 with 4 to 46 kHz. The acoustic signal was

621

detected by the microphone in the ear bar was sampled at 200 kHz and the magnitude

622

of the cubic distortion product (2f1-f2) determined by FFT. Averaging 20 adjacent

623

frequency bins surrounding the distortion product frequency determined the noise floor.

624

DPOAE thresholds were reached when the signal was greater than three standard

625

deviations above the noise floor.

626

Tissue Preparation, Staining and Confocal Microscopy: For staining of pre- and

627

postsynaptic markers we used antibodies against Homer (Synaptic Systems, 160003,

628

1:800 dilution), Bassoon (AbCam, ab82958, 1:500 dilution), CtBP1 (BD-Biosciences,

629

612042, 1:600 dilution), CtBP2/RIBEYE (Santa Cruz Biotechnology, sc-5966, 1:300

630

dilution; BD Transduction Lab, 612044, 1:400), CaV1.3 (Alomone Labs, ACC-005, 1:75),

631

GluA3 (Santa Cruz Biotechnology, sc-7612, 1:200 dilution). Cochlea of P22 or P35 mice

632

were isolated in HBSS solution or artificial perilymph, a small hole was introduced at the

633

apex, cochlea were gently perfused through round and oval windows with freshly

634

prepared 4 % (w/v) PFA (Electron Microscopy Sciences) in PBS until the fixative

28

635

became visible at the apex. Organs were incubated in the same fixative solution for 30

636

min at room temperature. Whole mount organs of Corti were micro dissected, the

637

tectorial membrane removed, the tissue permeabilized for 30 min in 0.5 % (v/v)

638

Triton X-100/PBS, blocked for 2 h in 4 % (w/v) BSA/PBS at room temperature. Staining

639

with all primary antibodies was performed overnight at 4°C in the same blocking buffer.

640

For staining with antibodies against GluA2 and CaV1.3 the protocol was slightly varied:

641

fixation was 20 min at 4°C, followed by decalcification in 10 % EDTA (Sigma) for 2 h,

642

permeabilization and blocking was carried out in a blocking buffer containing 16%

643

donkey serum and 0.3 % Triton X-100. After washing all species appropriate secondary

644

antibodies (Alexa Fluor 488, 546 or 555, 647: 1:600 dilution in 4 % (w/v) BSA/PBS)

645

were applied for 1 h at room temperature. After washing, the organs of Corti were

646

mounted hair bundles facing upward in ProLong Gold© Antifade (Life Technologies).

647

Samples were imaged using confocal microscopy (LSM700 or LSM880, Zeiss) on 1.4

648

NA oil immersion objectives for confocal imaging, and 1.2 NA water immersion objective

649

for Airyscan imaging. The optimal voxel size was adjusted to the green channel

650

(confocal x/y/z: 100/100/350 nm; Airyscan x/y/z: 33/33/222 nm). RIBEYE WT and KO

651

samples were prepared in parallel and images were collected with identical settings.

652

All images were taken in the apical region, between 200-1000 µm from the apex, of the

653

organ of Corti.

654

Image analysis

655

For

656

colocalization, we used the Imaris software package (Version 8.4, Bitplane). Puncta

657

were initially detected using the spot algorithm allowing for different spot sizes and

three-dimensional

analysis

of

synaptic

29

punctum

volume,

intensity

and

658

elongated point spread function in z-direction (initial seed size x-y/z: CtBP1, Ctbp2,

659

Homer, 0.44/1.13 µm; Bassoon 0.37/1.13 µm; GluA3 0.7/1.4 µm; CaV1.3 0.56/1.4 µm),

660

followed by co-localizing the spots with a threshold lower than 1. All counts were

661

normalized to the number of Inner hair cells in a region of interest.

662

To estimate the size of synaptic puncta we used the surface algorithm in Imaris. For all

663

stainings, the surface detail was set to 0.15 µm, background subtraction to 0.562 µm,

664

and touching object size was 0.750 µm (Bassoon puncta were split at 0.36 µm).

665

TEM and Tomography

666

Temporal bones of P21 mice (WT: n = 5, KO: n = 5) were isolated in HBSS, a small

667

hole was introduced at the apex of the cochlea, gently perfused through round and oval

668

windows with freshly prepared 4 % (w/v) PFA,

669

Microscopy Sciences) in a buffer containing (in mM) 135 NaCl, 2.4 KCl, 2.4 CaCl2, 4.8

670

MgCl2, 30 HEPES, pH 7.4 until the fixative became visible at the apex. Temporal bones

671

were soaked for 4 hours in the same buffer and shipped in PBS. Cochleae were

672

microdissected out from fixed adult mouse temporal bones and slowly equilibrated with

673

30% glycerol as cryoprotection. Tissues were then plunge frozen in liquid ethane

674

at -180C using a Leica Biosystems grid plunger. Frozen samples were freeze

675

substituted using Leica Biosystems AFS in 1.5% uranyl acetate in absolute methanol

676

at -90C for 2 days and infiltrated with HM20 Lowicryl resin (Electron Microscopy

677

Sciences) over 2 days at -45C. Polymerization of the resin was carried out under UV

678

light for 3 days at temperatures between -45C and 0C. Ultrathin sections at 100 nm

679

were cut using a Leica ultramicrotome and collected onto hexagonal 300-mesh Cu grids

680

(Electron Microscopy Sciences). 30

2 % (w/v) Glutaraldehyde (Electron

681

Samples were imaged using a 200kV JEOL 2100 with a Gatan Orius 832 CCD camera.

682

Single images were acquired using DigitalMicrograph (Gatan). Tilt series were acquired

683

using SerialEM (Mastronarde 2005) at 1 tilt increment from -60 to +60 and

684

tomographic reconstruction of tilt series and segmentation of tomograms was done with

685

IMOD software suite (Kremer, Mastronarde et al. 1996). Fiji was used to generate

686

projection images from tomograms and for cropping, rotating and adjusting the

687

brightness and contrast of images.

688

Hair cell recordings:

689

IHCs from the apical coil of the organ of Corti were dissected from p10-13 Ctbp2

690

littermates of either sex, placed on a plastic coverslip and secured with two strands of

691

dental floss. Cells were visualized with a water immersion lens (100x) attached to a

692

BX51 Olympus upright microscope. Patch-clamp recordings were obtained at room

693

temperature (21-23 C) in an external solution containing in (mM) NaCl 140, KCl, 5.4,

694

CaCl2, 2.8, MgCl2 1, 4-(2-hydroxyethyl)-1-piperazineethanesulfonic acid (HEPES) 10, D-

695

glucose 6, creatine, ascorbate, Na pyruvate 2, pH 7.4 adjusted with NaOH. 100 nM

696

apamin was included to block the sK K+ channel. Cesium at 3 mM was used externally

697

to reduce inward leak current. Pipette solution contained (mM) CsCl 90, TEA,

698

tetraethylammonium chloride 13, 3 NaATP, MgCl2 4, creatine phosphate 5, ethylene

699

glycol-bis(β-aminoethyl ether)-N,N,N',N'-tetraacetic acid (EGTA), HEPES 10, pH

700

adjusted with CsOH to 7.2.

701

Two-Sine Method

702

Hair cells were voltage-clamped with an Axon Multiclamp 700B (Axon Instruments-

703

Molecular Devices, Sunnyvale, CA.) and data recorded at 200-300 kHz with a National 31

704

Instrument USB-6356 D/A acquisition board and JClamp software (SciSoft). Vesicle

705

fusion was determined by monitoring membrane capacitance as a correlate of surface

706

area change. Capacitance was measured with a dual sinusoidal, FFT-based method

707

(Santos-Sacchi 2004). In this RC analysis method, two voltage frequencies, f1 and f2

708

(twice the f1 frequency) are summed and the real and imaginary components of the

709

current response are used to determine the magnitudes of the three model components.

710

The time resolution of the Cm measurement is the period of f1, which we varied from

711

0.16 to 0.32 ms, corresponding to 6250 to 3125 Hz. Stimulation protocols were

712

performed after 10 minutes in the whole-cell mode to allow Ca2+ current run up and full

713

exchange of the internal solution into the cell (Schnee, Castellano-Munoz et al. 2011). A

714

dual sinusoid of 40 mV amplitude was delivered superimposed to the desired voltage

715

step. The mean IHC Cm was 10 ± 1 pF N = 25 and uncompensated series resistance of

716

10 ± 3 M. All voltages were corrected for a 4 mV junction potential.

717

Postsynaptic Recordings

718

Whole-cell voltage-clamp recordings were made at terminal endings of SGNs from P17-

719

21 animals (Grant, Yi et al. 2010). Cochlea were acutely dissected from the temporal

720

bone in chilled (~ 4°C) standard extracellular solution containing 1 mM curare. The

721

apical turn was excised after removing stria vascularis and tectorial membrane, and

722

placed under two dental floss fibers on a recording chamber filled with the standard

723

extracellular solution at room temperature. Recording pipettes were made from 1.5 mm,

724

thick-wall borosilicate glass (WPI). Pipettes were pulled using a multistep horizontal

725

puller (Sutter) and fire polished while applying a pressure to the back end of pipettes to

726

an impedance of 6-8 MΩ (Johnson, Forge et al. 2008). When whole-cell recording was 32

727

achieved from a bouton, 5 µM TTX was perfused into the chamber, and EPSCs were

728

recorded in the standard solution (5.8 mM K+) for at least 3 min, then in 40 mM KCl for

729

1-5 min (depending on the duration of synaptic activity on each nerve).

730

external solution contained (in mM): 5.8 KCl, 140 NaCl, 0.9 MgCl2, 1.3 CaCl2, 0.7

731

NaH2PO4, 5.6 D-glucose, 10 HEPES, 2 Ascorbate, 2 Na-Pyruvate, 2 Creatine

732

Monohydrate, Osmolarity was 305 mosmls, pH = 7.4 (NaOH). Intracellular solutions

733

contained (in mM): 135 KCl, 3.5 MgCl2, 0.1 CaCl2, 5.0 EGTA, 5.0 HEPES, 2.5 Na2-ATP,

734

pH = 7.2 (KOH), osm ~290 mosmls.

735

Data Analysis:

736

Data were collected blinded to genotype for the experimentalist. Genotype was revealed

737

during analysis phase to ensure appropriate n values were obtained between groups.

738

Although gender was noted, no gender differences were observed between groups and

739

so data is presented combined. Data analysis was largely performed using unpaired

740

two-tailed t-tests unless otherwise stated. When non normal distributions were

741

observed, as in the EPSC histograms we selected different comparators as described

742

with the data. Similarly when large n values would bias comparisons, such as with

743

analyzing immunohistochemistry data, we moved to alternative more appropriate tests

744

as described in the text. Data is presented as mean ± standard deviation unless

745

otherwise stated. Sample size is presented in legends or in text. For physiology

746

experiments sample size reflects cell numbers which also indicates animal numbers as

747

we did not record from more than one cell in a preparation. For immunohistochemistry,

748

data is presented for animal numbers as well as image, cell and puncta numbers.

33

Standard

749

The voltage against capacitance plots were arbitrarily fit with an exponential equation of

750

the form: Y = Y0 + A1 exp(-x(x-x0)/t) where: Y0 is the baseline change in capacitance and is

751

near 0, A1 is the asymptote for the maximal change in capacitance, x0 is the voltage

752

where changes in capacitance measurements were first noted and t reflects the voltage-

753

dependence of the release (Cm/Vm). The equation for the Gaussian fit used in Fig.

754

6E is: Y = Y0 + (A/(w*√(π/2)))*exp(-2*((x-xc)/w)^2) where Y0 is baseline, typically 0, A is the

755

area under the curve, w is the width and xc is the peak of the Gaussian.

756

Acknowledgments

757

This work was supported by National Institutes of Health, National Institute on Deafness

758

and Other Communication Disorders (NIDCD), Grants R01DC014712 to M.A.R.; M.A.R.

759

was also supported by an International Project Grant from Action on Hearing Loss.

760

NIDCD; F32 4F32 DC013721 grant from NIDCD to MN, Intramural Research Program

761

(Z01-DC000002) and NIDCD Advanced Imaging Core ZIC DC000081) to W.S. and

762

B.K.: RO1DC009913 to AJR, core grant P30 44992. We thank Autefeh Sajjadi for her

763

help with genotyping animals and basic husbandry. We thank Bill Roberts for comments

764

on the manuscript.

765

The authors declare no competing financial interests

766

References

767

Buran, B. N., N. Strenzke, A. Neef, E. D. Gundelfinger, T. Moser and M. C. Liberman

768

(2010). "Onset coding is degraded in auditory nerve fibers from mutant mice lacking

769

synaptic ribbons." J Neurosci 30(22): 7587-7597.

770

34

771

Chapochnikov, N. M., H. Takago, C. H. Huang, T. Pangrsic, D. Khimich, J. Neef, E.

772

Auge, F. Gottfert, S. W. Hell, C. Wichmann, F. Wolf and T. Moser (2014). "Uniquantal

773

release through a dynamic fusion pore is a candidate mechanism of hair cell

774

exocytosis." Neuron 83(6): 1389-1403.

775 776

dick, o. (2001). "localization of the presynaptic cytomatrix protein piccolo at ribbon and

777

conventional synapses in the rat retina: comparison with basson." J Comp Neurol 439:

778

10.

779 780

Farris, H. E., G. B. Wells and A. J. Ricci (2006). "Steady-state adaptation of

781

mechanotransduction modulates the resting potential of auditory hair cells, providing an

782

assay for endolymph [Ca2+]." J Neurosci 26(48): 12526-12536.

783

Fernandez, K. A., P. W. Jeffers, K. Lall, M. C. Liberman and S. G. Kujawa (2015).

784

"Aging after noise exposure: acceleration of cochlear synaptopathy in "recovered" ears."

785

J Neurosci 35(19): 7509-7520.

786 787

Frank, T., M. A. Rutherford, N. Strenzke, A. Neef, T. Pangrsic, D. Khimich, A. Fejtova,

788

E. D. Gundelfinger, M. C. Liberman, B. Harke, K. E. Bryan, A. Lee, A. Egner, D. Riedel

789

and T. Moser (2010). "Bassoon and the synaptic ribbon organize Ca(2)+ channels and

790

vesicles to add release sites and promote refilling." Neuron 68(4): 724-738.

791 792

Fuchs, P. A. (2005). "Time and intensity coding at the hair cell's ribbon synapse." J

793

Physiol 566(Pt 1): 7-12.

35

794 795

Furukawa, T. and S. Matsuura (1978). "Adaptive rundown of excitatory post-synaptic

796

potentials at synapses between hair cells and eight nerve fibres in the goldfish." J

797

Physiol 276: 193-209.

798 799

Glowatzki, E. and P. A. Fuchs (2002). "Transmitter release at the hair cell ribbon

800

synapse." Nat Neurosci 5(2): 147-154.

801 802

Grant, L., E. Yi and E. Glowatzki (2010). "Two modes of release shape the postsynaptic

803

response at the inner hair cell ribbon synapse." J Neurosci 30(12): 4210-4220.

804

Graydon, C. W., S. Cho, G. L. Li, B. Kachar and H. von Gersdorff (2011). "Sharp

805

Ca(2)(+) nanodomains beneath the ribbon promote highly synchronous multivesicular

806

release at hair cell synapses." J Neurosci 31(46): 16637-16650.

807 808

Graydon, C. W., J. Zhang, N. W. Oesch, A. A. Sousa, R. D. Leapman and J. S.

809

Diamond (2014). "Passive diffusion as a mechanism underlying ribbon synapse vesicle

810

release and resupply." J Neurosci 34(27): 8948-8962.

811 812

Jing, Z., M. A. Rutherford, H. Takago, T. Frank, A. Fejtova, D. Khimich, T. Moser and N.

813

Strenzke (2013). "Disruption of the presynaptic cytomatrix protein bassoon degrades

814

ribbon anchorage, multiquantal release, and sound encoding at the hair cell afferent

815

synapse." J Neurosci 33(10): 4456-4467.

816

36

817

Johnson, S. L., A. Forge, M. Knipper, S. Munkner and W. Marcotti (2008). "Tonotopic

818

variation in the calcium dependence of neurotransmitter release and vesicle pool

819

replenishment at mammalian auditory ribbon synapses." J Neurosci 28(30): 7670-7678.

820 821

Johnson, S. L., H. J. Kennedy, M. C. Holley, R. Fettiplace and W. Marcotti (2012). "The

822

resting transducer current drives spontaneous activity in prehearing mammalian

823

cochlear inner hair cells." J Neurosci 32(31): 10479-10483.

824 825

Kantardzhieva, A., M. C. Liberman and W. F. Sewell (2013). "Quantitative analysis of

826

ribbons, vesicles, and cisterns at the cat inner hair cell synapse: correlations with

827

spontaneous rate." J Comp Neurol 521(14): 3260-3271.

828 829

Kantardzhieva, A., M. Peppi, W. S. Lane and W. F. Sewell (2012). "Protein composition

830

of immunoprecipitated synaptic ribbons." J Proteome Res 11(2): 1163-1174.

831 832

Khimich, D., R. Nouvian, R. Pujol, S. tom Dieck, A. Egner, E. D. Gundelfinger and T.

833

Moser (2005). "Hair cell synaptic ribbons are essential for synchronous auditory

834

signalling." Nature 434(7035): 889-894.

835 836

Kremer, J. R., D. N. Mastronarde and J. R. McIntosh (1996). "Computer visualization of

837

three-dimensional image data using IMOD." J Struct Biol 116(1): 71-76.

838

37

839

Li, G. L., S. Cho and H. von Gersdorff (2014). "Phase-locking precision is enhanced by

840

multiquantal release at an auditory hair cell ribbon synapse." Neuron 83(6): 1404-1417.

841 842

Li, G. L., E. Keen, D. Andor-Ardo, A. J. Hudspeth and H. von Gersdorff (2009). "The

843

unitary event underlying multiquantal EPSCs at a hair cell's ribbon synapse." J Neurosci

844

29(23): 7558-7568.

845 846

Lv, C., W. J. Stewart, O. Akanyeti, C. Frederick, J. Zhu, J. Santos-Sacchi, L. Sheets, J.

847

C. Liao and D. Zenisek (2016). "Synaptic Ribbons Require Ribeye for Electron Density,

848

Proper Synaptic Localization, and Recruitment of Calcium Channels." Cell Rep 15(12):

849

2784-2795.

850 851

Mastronarde, D. N. (2005). "Automated electron microscope tomography using robust

852

prediction of specimen movements." J Struct Biol 152(1): 36-51.

853 854

Matthews, G. and P. Fuchs (2010). "The diverse roles of ribbon synapses in sensory

855

neurotransmission." Nat Rev Neurosci 11(12): 812-822.

856 857

Maxeiner, S., F. Luo, A. Tan, F. Schmitz and T. C. Sudhof (2016). "How to make a

858

synaptic ribbon: RIBEYE deletion abolishes ribbons in retinal synapses and disrupts

859

neurotransmitter release." Embo j 35(10): 1098-1114.

860

38

861

Nouvian, R., D. Beutner, T. D. Parsons and T. Moser (2006). "Structure and function of

862

the hair cell ribbon synapse." J Membr Biol 209(2-3): 153-165.

863 864

Pangrsic, T., L. Lasarow, K. Reuter, H. Takago, M. Schwander, D. Riedel, T. Frank, L.

865

M. Tarantino, J. S. Bailey, N. Strenzke, N. Brose, U. Muller, E. Reisinger and T. Moser

866

(2010). "Hearing requires otoferlin-dependent efficient replenishment of synaptic

867

vesicles in hair cells." Nat Neurosci 13(7): 869-876.

868 869

Parsons, T. D. (1994). "Calcium-triggered exocytosis and endocytosis in an isolated

870

presynaptic cell: capacitance measurements in sacucular hair cells." Neuron 13: 8.

871

Roberts, W. M. (1994). "Localization of calcium signals by a mobile calcium buffer in

872

frog saccular hair cells." J Neurosci 14(5 Pt 2): 3246-3262.

873 874

Roux, I., S. Safieddine, R. Nouvian, M. Grati, M. C. Simmler, A. Bahloul, I. Perfettini, M.

875

Le Gall, P. Rostaing, G. Hamard, A. Triller, P. Avan, T. Moser and C. Petit (2006).

876

"Otoferlin, defective in a human deafness form, is essential for exocytosis at the

877

auditory ribbon synapse." Cell 127(2): 277-289.

878 879

Rudolph, S., M. C. Tsai, H. von Gersdorff and J. I. Wadiche (2015). "The ubiquitous

880

nature of multivesicular release." Trends Neurosci 38(7): 428-438.

881

39

882

Rutherford, M. A., N. M. Chapochnikov and T. Moser (2012). "Spike encoding of

883

neurotransmitter release timing by spiral ganglion neurons of the cochlea." J Neurosci

884

32(14): 4773-4789.

885 886

Santos-Sacchi, J. (2004). "Determination of cell capacitance using the exact empirical

887

solution of partial differential Y/partial differential Cm and its phase angle." Biophys J

888

87(1): 714-727.

889 890

Schmitz, F., A. Konigstorfer and T. C. Sudhof (2000). "RIBEYE, a component of

891

synaptic ribbons: a protein's journey through evolution provides insight into synaptic

892

ribbon function." Neuron 28(3): 857-872.

893 894

Schnee, M. E., M. Castellano-Munoz, J. H. Kong, J. Santos-Sacchi and A. J. Ricci

895

(2011). "Tracking vesicle fusion from hair cell ribbon synapses using a high frequency,

896

dual sine wave stimulus paradigm." Commun Integr Biol 4(6): 785-787.

897 898

Schnee, M. E., M. Castellano-Munoz and A. J. Ricci (2013). "Response properties from

899

turtle auditory hair cell afferent fibers suggest spike generation is driven by

900

synchronized release both between and within synapses." J Neurophysiol 110(1): 204-

901

220.

902

40

903

Schnee, M. E., J. Santos-Sacchi, M. Castellano-Munoz, J. H. Kong and A. J. Ricci

904

(2011). "Calcium-dependent synaptic vesicle trafficking underlies indefatigable release

905

at the hair cell afferent fiber synapse." Neuron 70(2): 326-338.

906 907

Snellman, J., B. Mehta, N. Babai, T. M. Bartoletti, W. Akmentin, A. Francis, G.

908

Matthews, W. Thoreson and D. Zenisek (2011). "Acute destruction of the synaptic

909

ribbon reveals a role for the ribbon in vesicle priming." Nat Neurosci 14(9): 1135-1141.

910 911

Uthaiah, R. C. and A. J. Hudspeth (2010). "Molecular anatomy of the hair cell's ribbon

912

synapse." J Neurosci 30(37): 12387-12399.

913 914

von Gersdorff, H., E. Vardi, G. Matthews and P. Sterling (1996). "Evidence that vesicles

915

on the synaptic ribbon of retinal bipolar neurons can be rapidly released." Neuron 16(6):

916

1221-1227.

917 918

Zanazzi, G. and G. Matthews (2009). "The molecular architecture of ribbon presynaptic

919

terminals." Mol Neurobiol 39(2): 130-148.

920 921

Zenisek, D., N. K. Horst, C. Merrifield, P. Sterling and G. Matthews (2004). "Visualizing

922

synaptic ribbons in the living cell." J Neurosci 24(44): 9752-9759.

923

41

924

Zheng, X. Y., D. Henderson, S. L. McFadden, D. L. Ding and R. J. Salvi (1999).

925

"Auditory nerve fiber responses following chronic cochlear de-efferentation." J Comp

926

Neurol 406(1): 72-86.

927

928

Figure Legends

929

Figure 1. Number of synapses stays unaffected in ribbon less organs of corti.

930

A-D Max intensity projections of confocal z-sections of inner hair cells (IHCs) in apical

931

cochlea turns of P22 KO and wildtype littermates, immunolabeled for CtBP2/RIBEYE

932

(A), Homer (B), Bassoon (C) and Overlay (D). Virtually no punctated extra nuclear

933

CtBP2 staining can be seen in KO mice that is colocalized with other synaptic proteins,

934

while Bassoon and Homer appear generally unaffected. (E) 100 nm thick TEM sections

935

of pre- and postsynaptic densities in WT and KO mice at P28 confirm the absence of

936

electron dense ribbon structures in KO mice. (For E-G) Black arrows point to ribbon

937

structure, white arrows point to synaptic vesicles and white arrowheads point to the

938

remaining density resembling the arciform density. (F) Single slice from a 10 nm thick

939

tomographic section. (G) Reconstruction of complete synapses from data obtained used

940

to create E,F. Presynaptic density in cyan and postsynaptic density in green.Tethered

941

synaptic vesicles modeled in yellow, electron dense structure attributed to synaptic

942

ribbons in magenta. (H) Confocal max intensity projection of IHCs immunolabeled for

943

CtBP1 (green) and Homer (magenta). (I) Quantification of RIBEYE, Homer and

944

Bassoon puncta in the organ of corti normalized to the number of IHCs in analyzed

945

regions of interest, showing complete loss of synaptic RIBEYE staining in KO. The

42

946

amount of Homer and Bassoon staining stays unaffected (p > 0.05). WT in blue, n = 20

947

mice, quantified puncta: RIBEYE = 3796, Bassoon = 10215, Homer = 4340; KO in red,

948

n = 19 mice, n puncta: RIBEYE = 33, Bassoon = 10825, Homer = 4201. (J) Paired

949

synaptic puncta per IHC is lost in absence of RIBEYE staining (p ≤ 0.001) between WT

950

and KO. The number of synapses defined by colocated postsynaptic Homer and

951

presynaptic Bassoon puncta (p = 0.18) is unaffected in KO. (K) The number of extra

952

nuclear CtBP1 puncta is greatly reduced (p = 0.003) in KO mice. (WT in blue, n = 9

953

mice, n puncta: CtBP1 = 19929, Homer 4940; KO in red, n = 7 mice, n puncta: CtBP1 =

954

11960, Homer = 5033). (L) Remaining CtBP1 puncta show significantly reduced

955

(p ≤ 0.001) pairing with postsynaptic Homer staining. Scale Bars: A-D, H: 5 µm; E, F:

956

100 nm. Boxes represent standard deviations of the mean. Significance levels of two

957

tailed unpaired t-tests: n.s., not significant; * p ≤ 0.05; ** p ≤ 0.01; *** p ≤ 0.001.

958

959

Figure 1 Supplemental Figure 1: Immunohisotchemistry from WT (A) and KO (B) for

960

Bassoon (green) CTBP2 (red) and myosin VIIa (Blue). Low power images show

961

Bassoon is present in efferents of outer hair cells as well as presynaptically in IHCs.

962

Bassoon localization is much broader than RIBEYE’s and is minimally effected in the

963

mutant animal.

964

965

Figure 2 Hearing in RIBEYE knockout mice. A Distortion products of otoacoustic

966

emissions (DPOAE) show no significant differences in the tested frequency range (4-

967

46 kHz in half octave increments) between KO (red, n = 30) and WT littermates (blue,

43

968

n = 22), suggesting normal outer hair cell function. (B) Auditory brainstem response

969

(ABR) measurements in the tested frequency range (4-46 kHz in half octave

970

increments) reveal a small (≈ 10 dB SPL), but significant threshold shift in KO (red, n =

971

28) over their WT littermates (blue, n = 22) at P21-P22. (C) Mean ABR waveforms to

972

5 ms tone pips at 23 kHz at 60 dB SPL from KO (red, n = 30) and wild type littermates

973

(blue, n = 24) reveal a decrease in amplitude of the first ABR wave in KO. (D)

974

Quantification of peak to peak amplitude (wave I: p1-n1) differences between KO (red)

975

and WT littermates (blue) at 23 kHz were significant over most sound pressure levels

976

(20 dB SPL: p > 0.05; 30 & 60 dB SPL: p ≤ 0.01; 40-60 dB SPL: p ≤ 0.001) (E)

977

Amplitudes of first peak amplitudes normalized to their respective values at 60 dB SPL.

978

Amplitudes scale with their maximum value in WT and KO. (F) First peak latency (p1) is

979

not significantly altered in KO mice. For wave I amplitude and latency (D-F) analysis the

980

n-number varied between sound pressure levels by a clearly detectable first wave and

981

speaker calibration, n of WT/KO: 20 dB, 11/7; 30 dB, 21/11; 40 dB, 23/22; 50 dB, 23/26;

982

60 dB, 23/28; 70 dB, 5/6. Boxes represent standard deviations of the mean; whiskers

983

and shaded line in (C) represent standard errors of the mean. Significance levels of two

984

tailed unpaired t-tests: n.s., not significant; * p ≤ 0.05; ** p ≤ 0.01; *** p ≤ 0.001.

985

986

Figure 3. Ribbonless synapses contain clusters of presynaptic voltage-gated Ca2+

987

channels juxtaposed to postsynaptic glutamate receptors. A-D Max intensity

988

projections of airyscan z-sections of IHCs in apical cochlea turns of P35 KO and WT

989

littermates, immunolabeled for CtBP2/RIBEYE (A), GluA3 (B), CaV1.3 (C) and overlay

990

between calcium channels and glutamate receptors (D). Inner hair cell afferent 44

991

synapses from WT have a synaptic ribbon: a cytoplasmic presynaptic density comprised

992

mainly of RIBEYE protein, recognized by anti-CtBP2 (green). Co-labelled are the

993

closely associated presynaptic voltage-gated Ca2+ channels (CaV1.3, gray) and

994

postsynaptic AMPA receptors (GluA3, magenta). Inner hair cell afferent synapses from

995

KO lacked synaptic ribbons. The AMPA receptors in KO mice exhibited a ring-like

996

morphology, and the CaV1.3 channels clustered in the form of round spots or elongated

997

stripes, both similar to WT synapses. Scale bar: 2 µm

998

Figure 4. Changed size distribution but unaffected synapse number and

999

organization in ribbonless synapses (A) Quantification of RIBEYE, GluA3 and

1000

CaV1.3 puncta in the organ of corti normalized to the number of IHCs in analyzed

1001

regions of interest. The number of GluA3 receptor and CaV1.3 puncta is unaffected (p >

1002

0.05). WT in blue, n = 9 images from 3 mice, 95 cells quantified puncta: RIBEYE =

1003

1456, GluA3 = 1459, CaV1.3 = 1897 KO in red, n = 13 images from 3 mice, 248 cells, n

1004

puncta: RIBEYE = 28, GluA3 = 2149, CaV1.3 = 2681. (B) Colocalization of synaptic

1005

puncta per IHC. In absence of RIBEYE, the number of synapses, defined by

1006

colocalization of postsynaptic GluA3 and presynaptic CaV1.3, is unaffected (p = 0.13) in

1007

KO. (C) Volume of presynaptic CaV1.3, Bassoon and postsynaptic Homer and GluA3 in

1008

WT (blue, n synapses: CaV1.3 = 1370; Bassoon = 1917; Homer = 5219; GluA3 =

1009

1611) and KO (red, n synapses: CaV1.3 = 2132; Bassoon = 1817; Homer = 4990;

1010

GluA3 = 2334) mice. (D) Normalized cumulative distributions of volumes show

1011

increased sizes of postsynaptic GluA3 and Homer immunoreactivity. (C) Boxes

1012

represent standard deviations of the mean. Significance levels of two tailed unpaired t-

1013

tests (A/B) or two sample Kolmogorov-Smirnov test (C): D represents the maximum

45

1014

distance of the Kolmogorov-Smirnov distribution functions. n.s., not significant; * p ≤

1015

0.05; ** p ≤ 0.01; *** p ≤ 0.001; **** p ≤ 0.0001.

1016

Figure 5: No differences were observed in calcium or potassium currents

1017

between KO and WT animals. Ca2+ currents were obtained from mice ranging in age

1018

from P10-P12 for WT (A, blue) and P10-13 KO (B, red), with time course of stimulus

1019

shown above, are comparable in all measured properties. Voltage-current plots,

1020

presented in (C) as mean ± SD show no difference (n provided in figure). Summaries of

1021

peak current (D) and voltage of half activation (V1/2, E) are not statistically different (n=

1022

18 and 26 for WT and KO, p = 0.13 for peak current and 0.326 for V1/2). Potassium

1023

currents, obtained from responses to 50 ms voltage pulses ranging from -124 mV to 16

1024

mV in 10 mV increments, from a holding potential of -84 mV are presented in (F) for WT

1025

and (G) for KO mice at P24. A summary of the half activation voltage as estimated from

1026

a Boltzmann fit to the tail current voltage-current (H) found no statistical difference (n =

1027

6 and 8 for WT and KO, respectively with p = 0.084). Current-clamp measurements

1028

found no difference in resting potential (I) (n = 5 and 8 for WT and KO, respectively, p =

1029

0.69).

1030

1031

Figure 6. Capacitance responses recorded from RIBEYE knockout mice show

1032

little difference between phenotypes. Example records of Cm responses (top),

1033

showing two release components and Ca2+ currents (below) recorded from a WT (blue)

1034

(A) and KO (red) (B) in response to 1s depolarizations from -84 mV to -49 and -44 mV.

1035

Dashed line indicates pre-stimulus baseline Cm. Black solid line indicates the 1st linear

1036

component of release and cyan line the 2nd, superlinear component. (C) Summary plot 46

1037

of 1st and 2nd component magnitudes. No statistical difference was found between

1038

groups. For the linear component n = 15 for WT and 15 for KO, p = 0.23. For 2nd,

1039

superlinear component n = 17 for both groups and p = 0.38. Filled stars indicate mean

1040

for this and all subsequent figures. (D) Summary plot of 1st and 2nd component rates

1041

obtained by linear fits to data points, as indicated by lines in (A,B). No statistical

1042

difference was found between groups where, n = 16 for WT amd 13 for KO, with p =

1043

0.266. For the second component rate, n = 17 for both, with p = 0.085. (D). (E) presents

1044

a population histogram for the voltage at which the superlinear response is first

1045

observed. Fits to Gaussian distributions show no change in the mean value but an

1046

increase in the width of the plot for the KO. (F) Example of delay in onset of capacitance

1047

change with small depolarizations for a KO cell. Plots of Cm verses voltage responses

1048

measured at 0.1 (G), 0.5 seconds (H) 1 sec (I) or 3 sec (J) show a significant difference

1049

for release at 0.1s and -44 mV (p=0.03), error bars represent SEM. Fitting the

1050

relationships with simple exponential functions reveal differences in the shapes of the

1051

curves but not in maximum release for the 0.1 and 0.5 plots but not the 1 and 3 sec time

1052

points. N provided in figure for each group. (K) Shows time to release is significantly

1053

longer for KO compared to WT for depolarizations eliciting less than 50% of the

1054

maximal calcium current; n = 32 for WT and 26 for KO, p = 0.0012.

1055

Figure 7. Changing internal Ca2+ buffering does not distinguish phenotypes. (A)

1056

Example records of Cm changes in response to 1s depolarizations from -49 mV in 0.1

1057

EGTA and -39 mV in 1 and 5 mM EGTA for WT (blue) and KO (red). (B) Plot of the

1058

change in Cm in response to 1s voltage steps of -49, -44 and -29 mV for 0.1 EGTA and

1059

-49,-44,-39 and -34 mV for 5 EGTA, n provided in figure. (C) The time to the 2nd

47

1060

component increases significantly with elevated Ca2+ buffer (p value indicated in figure)

1061

but is similar between phenotypes; for 0.1 EGTA n = 7 for WT and 6 KO , p = 0.93,

1062

steps to -29 mV for WT and KO, for 1 EGTA n = 15 for WT and13 for KO , with p = -

1063

0.13, steps to -36 + 8 mV WT and -37 + 9 mV KO and for 5 EGTA n = 3 for WT and 4

1064

for KO, with p = 0.67, steps to -39 + 5 mV WT and -35 + 2 mV KO. (D) The rate of

1065

release to the -49 mV step declines equally between phenotypes with increasing

1066

[EGTA] for 0.1 EGTA, n = 7 for WT and 6 for KO, p = 0.323, for 1 EGTA n = 20 for WT

1067

and 22 for KO, p = 0.99 and for 5 EGTA n = 7 for WT and 6 for KO, p = 0.235. (E,F)

1068

Provide box plots of the first component of release for depolarizations to -49 mV (E) or -

1069

44 mV (F). Effects of buffers were similar between the two groups.

1070

Figure 8: Subtle differences exist between WT and KO mice postsynaptic

1071

properties. Postsynaptic bouton recordings were obtained from WT or KO in control

1072

and during application of 40 mM K+ to the bath. Hair cell recordings demonstrate similar

1073

depolarizations in WT and KO, suggesting that any differences in response properties

1074

may be synapticaly mediated. (A). Cells rapidly recovered following washout of K+. The

1075

solid line below the trace indicates the duration of the application.

1076

Postsynaptic recordings from afferent boutons in response to 40 mM K+ external

1077

solution application are presented in (B) for WT and (C) for KO with a sustained

1078

response and (D) for KO with a transient response. Recordings were made in the

1079

presence of 5 µM tetrodotoxin (TTX). Frequency responses were generated using a 1

1080

second window sliding at 100 ms intervals (E,F). Populations were defined as transient

1081

or sustained by whether firing persisted after 20 seconds. Number of each response

1082

type is presented as fraction in each panel. Maximum frequency responses are 48

1083

presented in box plot format with mean value shown as start (G); n = 10 for WT and 18

1084

for KO, p = 0.313. The time to peak is presented in (H) where onset of K+ application

1085

response was defined as the time point when the frequency exceeded the spontaneous

1086

frequency +3x SD for two overlapping time windows (200 ms). The time to peak was

1087

statistically different when the one outlier was removed (time point at 181 sec); n = 8 for

1088

WT and 10 for KO. Time to peak only includes fibers whose peak frequency reached 30

1089

Hz because those lower had too few points to make an accurate measure. (I)

1090

Integrating the frequency response over 50 seconds shows that WT release in a more

1091

sustainable manner than the KO, n = 8 for WT and 10 for KO, p = 0.021. Representative

1092

probability density functions for EPSC amplitudes are presented for WT number of

1093

events = 6583 WT and 1551 KO in (J) and KO in (K). Solid lines represent fits to either

1094

double Gaussian functions or Gaussian and gamma functions (smaller peak always fit

1095

with Gaussian function). The curve fitting the small events were integrated to measure

1096

the % of events under the small peak (shown next to each example). Only nerves that

1097

had at least 100 events are included to ensure robustness of the fitting. (L) presents a

1098

box plot of the WT and KO percent of area contributed by the small peak. Comparisons

1099

using a t-test for unequal variance (Welch test) suggest a significant, p = 0.032, n = 7

1100

for WT and 13 for KO. (M) presents a box plot comparing the percentages of simple

1101

events, defined as single rise and decay times plots are not significantly different, n = 7

1102

for WT and 13 for KO, p = 0.3. (N) provides box plots of the median amplitude, where

1103

no significant difference was observed, n = 7 for WT and 13 for KO for each, p = 0.105,

1104

while (O) are box plots of the coefficient of variation for the distributions in (N). Plots

1105

were different, p = 0.017, when equal variance was not assumed.

49

1106

Figure 9: KO synapses have a larger population of small slow EPSCs. 7 WT

1107

including 15,564 events and 13 KO fibers including 17210 events were analyzed. (A)

1108

shows examples of EPSCs that range in amplitude and kinetics. The inset highlights

1109

one of the small slower EPSCs identified in KO fibers. Median decay times (B) and the

1110

Coefficient of Variation for the distribution in decay times (C) are presented as box plots.

1111

Neither population show statistical significance. (D) plots the skewness of the

1112

distributions of decay times and demonstrates that the KO decay time is significantly

1113

greater (away from 0), suggesting that the mean is larger than median, largely because

1114

they have a long tail of large decay time. (E), plots the EPSC decay time constant for

1115

every fiber (each fiber a different color) for both WT and KO. (F), summarizes the data

1116

from C as average decay time constant with EPSCs binned over 10 pA. (G), pooling

1117

fibers for WT and KO recordings allowed for the generation of a probability density

1118

function for event duration. These distributions are significantly different by KS test

1119

(p