Proc. R. Soc. B (2006) 273, 471–478 doi:10.1098/rspb.2005.3342 Published online 29 November 2005
The human brain is a detector of chemosensorily transmitted HLA-class I-similarity in same- and opposite-sex relations Bettina M. Pause1,*, Kerstin Krauel2, Claudia Schrader1, Bernfried Sojka1, Eckhard Westphal3, Wolfgang Mu¨ller-Ruchholtz3 and Roman Ferstl1 1
Institute of Psychology, Christian-Albrechts-University Kiel, Olshausenstrasse 62, 24098 Kiel, Germany 2 Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Otto-von-Guericke-University Magdeburg, Emanuel-Larisch-Weg 17-19, 39112 Magdeburg, Germany 3 Institute of Immunology, Christian-Albrechts-University Kiel, Michaelisstrasse 5, 24105 Kiel, Germany
Studies on subjective body odour ratings suggest that humans exhibit preferences for human leucocyte antigen (HLA)-dissimilar persons. However, with regard to the extreme polymorphism of the HLA gene loci, the behavioural impact of the proposed HLA-related attracting signals seems to be minimal. Furthermore, the role of HLA-related chemosignals in same- and opposite-sex relations in humans has not been specified so far. Here, we investigate subjective preferences and brain evoked responses to body odours in males and females as a function of HLA similarity between odour donor and smeller. We show that pre-attentive processing of body odours of HLA-similar donors is faster and that late evaluative processing of these chemosignals activates more neuronal resources than the processing of body odours of HLA-dissimilar donors. In same-sex smelling conditions, HLA-associated brain responses show a different local distribution in male (frontal) and female subjects (parietal). The electrophysiological results are supported by significant correlations between the odour ratings and the amplitudes of the brain potentials. We conclude that odours of HLA-similar persons function as important social warning signals in inter- and intrasexual human relations. Such HLA-related chemosignals may contribute to female and male mate choice as well as to male competitive behaviour. Keywords: human leucocyte antigen; body odour; event-related potential; HLA-related chemosignals
1. INTRODUCTION Several experimental studies have demonstrated that the individual body odour in many vertebrates is associated with their allelic profile of the major histocompatibility complex (MHC; reviewed by Singh 2001). Due to these MHC-related chemosignals individuals prefer to mate with a conspecific that differs at the MHC (reviewed by Penn 2002). However, while most studies indicate female mate choice (Potts et al. 1991), some also point to the possibility of male mate choice (Yamazaki et al. 1976; Olsson et al. 2003). As female mice prefer MHC-similar females as nest mates (Manning et al. 1992), kin recognition also seems to be mediated by the MHC type. MHC-associated chemosignals have been proposed to affect sexual selection in order to preserve MHC polymorphism. The extreme polymorphism in MHC genes is assumed to have an evolutionary advantage in most vertebrates (e.g. heterozygote and rare allele advantage, inbreeding avoidance; reviewed by Brown & Eklund 1994 and Penn & Potts 1999). However, while sexual selection is traditionally considered to include inter- and intrasexual components (Moshkin et al. 2000; Moore et al. 2001; Olsson et al. 2003), it has not been shown whether the MHC also contributes to male competitive behaviour.
In humans, the MHC is referred to as human leucocyte antigen (HLA). Subjective rating studies, using T-shirts as the odour source, indicate that odours of people with a low (Wedekind & Fu¨ri 1997) or intermediate (Jacob et al. 2002) number of HLA allele matches are preferred, compared to odours of people with a high number of HLA matches. In investigating same- and opposite-sex relations between odour donors and perceivers, the strongest correlation between HLA-dissimilarity and odour pleasantness was unexpectedly observed in the intra-sexual male condition (Wedekind & Fu¨ri 1997). Generally, the behavioural consequences of these HLArelated odour preferences in humans are considered to be related to mate choice (Ober et al. 1997), and to the degree of acquaintance between unrelated people (Eggert et al. 1999a). Again, the strongest HLA effects on the degree of acquaintance were found in male-to-male relations. In order to understand how HLA-related chemosignals influence human information processing, the present study employed chemosensory event-related potentials (CSERPs; Kobal & Hummel 1988) as an objective indicator of the neuronal activity associated with the perception of HLA-related chemosignals. CSERPs are the averaged epochs of the electroencephalogram (EEG) that occur time-locked to the chemosensory stimulus. Latency and amplitude of the CSERP components can provide information as to whether differences between HLArelated chemosignals are already reflected in early,
* Author and address for correspondence: Institute of Experimental Psychology, Heinrich-Heine University, Du¨sseldorf Universita¨tsstraße l, 40225 Du¨sseldorf, Germany (
[email protected]). Received 8 August 2005 Accepted 21 September 2005
471
q 2005 The Royal Society
472 B. M. Pause and others
Human brain and HLA-associated chemosignals
exogenous components (N1, w400 ms post-stimulus) or only influence late, endogenous components (P3, w1000 ms post-stimulus; see Pause & Krauel 2000). Exogenous potentials such as the early negative deflection (N1) reflect mainly pre-attentive processing of external stimulus characteristics (quality, intensity; Pause et al. 1996, 1997). Endogenous potentials such as the late positive deflection (P3) occur in response to rare (unexpected) stimuli and increase in amplitude with the subjective meaning of a stimulus, e.g. when the rare stimulus has to be responded to or is of subjective importance to the perceiver. These components can be elicited in course of the well established oddball paradigm (Donchin & Coles 1988). Within the oddball paradigm, rare stimuli are interspersed among frequent standard stimuli. In the current paradigm, the rare odour stimuli either served as the target stimulus, subjects had to detect and respond to, or served as the distractor stimulus, in order to study evaluative processes independent of task demands. Summarizing, the aim of the present study was to investigate whether human electrical brain responses to body odour depend on the HLA class-I compatibility between sender and perceiver. In order to investigate HLA effects in same- and opposite-sex relations, CSERPs of male and female subjects in response to odours of male and female donors were analysed. In separating early from late CSERP components, it was addressed whether HLAassociated chemosignals are processed pre-attentively and/or consciously.
2. MATERIAL AND METHODS (a) Subjects Forty subjects reporting no history of neurological or endocrine diseases, or diseases related to the upper respiratory tract participated in the study. Due to missing EEG data, two subjects were excluded from further analysis (S12 and S33, see table 1). The final subject sample (mean ageZ26.9, s.d.Z4.7, rangeZ20–41, no group differences (ANOVA): F(3,34)Z1.27, pO0.20) included three smokers (no group differences (Craddock-Flood): c23Z3.77, pO0.20), and 29 dextrals, six sinistrals and three ambidexters (no group differences (Craddock-Flood): c26Z4.53, pO0.20). Thirtysix subjects described themselves as heterosexual and two (one male) as bisexual. A screening for olfactory sensitivity (mixture of citral, eugenol, linalool, menthol and isoamyl acetate) at the beginning of the experiment revealed no group differences (Craddock-Flood: c26Z2.73, pO0.20). All female subjects were investigated at the beginning of their menstrual cycle (mean cycle dayZ3.5, s.d.Z1.8) and 11 of them used oral contraceptives (no group differences (Craddock-Flood): c21Z0.38, pO0.20). The subjects participated voluntarily in the study, gave written, informed consent and were paid for participation. (b) Body odours and odour presentation Odour samples (axillary hair) from 61 donors were collected (table 1). As some donated for more than one experimental session, the number of odour donors for each experimental group varied between 18 and 24. The final donor sample had a mean age of 27.7 years (s.d.Z5.7; no group differences (ANOVA): F(3,82)Z1.01, pO0.20) and seven of them described themselves as regular smokers (no group Proc. R. Soc. B (2006)
differences (Craddock-Flood): c23Z0.167, pO0.20). The donating subjects declared that they did not suffer from any endocrine disorder and were asked to refrain from using deodorants and to wash their armpits exclusively with water 2 days before the axillary hair was collected. Furthermore, they were requested to refrain from eating onions, garlic or asparagus and had to keep nutrition and hygiene diaries. All subjects cut their axillary hair in the morning, immediately after waking up. The females were additionally asked to cut their hair in the follicular phase of the menstrual cycle, within a few days after the end of menstruation (mean cycle dayZ 6.1, s.d.Z1.8). Axillary hair samples were stored at K20 8C until use (mean storage timeZ48 days, s.d.Z42). All donors were paid for their cooperation. For the EEG sessions, the axillary hairs were placed in odour chambers (meanZ33.3 mg hairs in each chamber, s.d.Z9.6) of a constant-flow (2!3-) olfactometer (Burghart Company, Wedel, Germany). Odours were presented birhinally by independent airstreams (100 ml sK1) for 600 ms each with an interstimulus interval of 18–22 s (meanZ20 s). During stimulus presentation, subjects performed the velopharyngeal closure technique (Pause et al. 1999a). Thereby, the soft palate closes the connection between the oral and the nasal cavity, and because the subjects are breathing through their mouth, the odour presentations do not interfere with the nasal breathing cycle. (c) Design In our study, all subjects and odour donors (donation of samples of axillary hair) were serologically tissue-typed for their A- and B-loci (HLA-class I). Three odour donors were individually assigned to each of 40 (20 males) subjects (table 1). Whereas two of the donors shared none of the detected HLA class I alleles with the perceiving subject (standard stimulus and deviant stimulus 1, condition: HLAdissimilar), one of them shared at least three alleles (deviant stimulus 2, condition: HLA-similar). As the subjects perceived the foreign body odours always in the context of their own body odour, it might have been more difficult to detect of the deviant stimulus 2 (HLA-similar). In order to obtain a similar context odour for the deviant stimulus 1 (HLA-dissimilar), the standard stimulus was chosen to be similar to the deviant stimulus 1. A three-stimulus oddball design was used to differentiate HLA effects on automatic (pre-attentive) and on controlled (conscious) stimulus processing. Within the oddball paradigm, the standard odours were presented with an occurrence probability of 60% and the two deviant stimuli with a probability of 20% each. During four blocks of 50 trials, the deviant stimuli were alternatively presented as targets or distractors. The subjects’ task was to detect the target odour by lifting their right index finger (response initiation by a 160 Hz tone, 3–4 s after odour onset). However, they were not informed about the occurrence of the distractor odour. At the beginning of each 50-trial block, standard and target odours were introduced to the subjects. They were asked whether they disliked or liked the odour and whether they could imagine having a partner with such a body odour (seven-point scale for each rating: K3 to C3). Both ratings were highly correlated (rZC0.86, p!0.001). However, further analyses of the subjective data were exclusively based on the ratings for partner preferences, because they were closer to a normal distribution than the ratings for general odour valence (Kurtosis analysis).
Human brain and HLA-associated chemosignals
B. M. Pause and others
473
Table 1. Design and specification of HLA class I types. donors (D)
sex of subject
sex of donor
female
same sex
opposite sex
male
same sex
opposite sex
subjects (S)
standard stimulus HLA-dissimilar
deviant stimulus 1 HLA-dissimilar
deviant stimulus 2 HLA-similar
no.
HLA-type
no.
HLA-type
no.
HLA-type
no.
HLA-type
S1 S2 S3 S4 S5 S6 S7 S8 S9 S10 S11 S12 S13 S14 S15 S16 S17 S18 S19 S20 S21 S22 S23 S24 S25 S26 S27 S28 S29 S30 S31 S32 S33 S34 S35 S36 S37 S38 S39 S40
A2,3 B7,62 A2,2 B44,62 A1,25 B8,18 A1,25 B18,57 A2,24 B7,62 A3,24 B35,61 A2,26 B37,44 A1,1 B8,8 A3,11B 35,35 A1,25B 18,57 A1,2 B8,60 A2,3 B7,7 A3,3 B18,35 A1,24 B8,35 A2,3 B7,60 A1,1 B8,8 A2,3 B35,60 A1,11 B35,44 A3,3 B7,44 A1,24 B8,44 A24,25 B18,62 A1,1 B8,35 A2,3 B27,62 A1,11 B8,35 A2,2 B60,60 A2,2 B27,51 A1,3 B7,8 A2,2 B7,7 A2,3 B13,35 A3,3 B7,7 A 25,33 B7,18 A 1,1 B35,57 A3,3 B35,44 A1,2 B8,60 A2,3 B7,7 A1, 24 B8,51 A2,3 B7,60 A2,24 B7,62 A24,25 B18,18 A1,3 B8,60
D1 D4 D7 D10 D13 D15 D17 D19 D21 D22 D24 D27 D26 D31 D28 D36 D33 D32 D41 D42 D26 D35 D38 D32 D24 D41 D36 D28 D46 D47 D10 D50 D53 D56 D9 D59 D60 D13 D62 D16
A1,24 B8,35 A3,11 B35,47 A2,11 B35,39 A3,24 B35,51 A1,25 B18,57 A2,29 B44,44 A1,24 B8,58 A2,26 B7,44 A2,2 B44,62 A2,3 B7,7 A3,3 B35,44 A1,1 B35,57 A1,2 B8,60 A2,2 B60,60 A1,1 B8,35 A2,30 B27,51 A1,24 B8,51 A2,2 B7,60 A1,24 B8,62 A2,3 B35,51 A1,2 B8,60 A2,3 B7,60 A1,24 B7,8 A2,2 B7,60 A3,3 B35,44 A1,24 B8,62 A2,30 B27,51 A1,1 B8,35 A24,25 B18,62 A1,2 B8,51 A3,24 B35,51 A2,24 B39,44 A2,11 B50,60 A3,3 B18,35 A1,25 B18,57 A2,25 B18,62 A1,24 B8,27 A1,25 B 18,57 A1,2 B8,57 A2,29 B44,56
D2 D5 D8 D11 D12 D16 D1 D18 D6 D23 D25 D28 D30 D32 D34 D37 D38 D40 D33 D39 D43 D29 D33 D31 D25 D38 D37 D34 D44 D26 D11 D51 D54 D57 D12 D49 D1 D9 D63 D15
A1,24 B8,44 A3,11 B35,35 A2,11 B18,35 A3,24 B35,61 A1,25 B8,18 A2,29 B44,56 A1,24 B8,35 A2,26 B7,44 A2,2 B44,62 A2,3 B7,62 A3,3 B27,35 A1,1 B8,35 A1,2 B37,60 A2,2 B7,60 A1,11 B8,35 A2,2 B27,51 A1,24 B7,8 A2,2 B7,7 A1,24 B8,51 A2,3 B13,35 A1,3 B8,60 A 2,3 B7,7 A1,24 B8,51 A2,2 B60,60 A3,3 B27,35 A1,24 B7,8 A2,2 B27,51 A1,11 B8,35 A24,25 B18,18 A1,2 B8,60 A3,24 B35,61 A2,24 B27,44 A2,11 B38,60 A3,30 B18,35 A1,25 B8,18 A2,25 B7,18 A1,24 B8,35 A1,25 B18,57 A1,2 B8,57 A2,29 B44,44
D3 D6 D9 D12 D14 D10 D18 D20 D4 D9 D26 D29 D25 D33 D35 D28 D39 D34 D24 D38 D44 D34 D45 D28 D32 D36 D43 D29 D42 D48 D49 D52 D55 D58 D22 D1 D3 D61 D64 D65
A2,3 B7,60 A2,2 B44,62 A1,25 B18,57 A1,25 B8,18 A2,24 B7,62 A3,24 B35,51 A2,26 B7,44 A1,1 B8,8 A3,11 B35,47 A1,25 B18,57 A1,2 B8,60 A2,3 B7,7 A3,3 B27,35 A1,24 B8,51 A2,3 B7,60 A1,1 B8,35 A2,3 B13,35 A1,11 B8,35 A3,3 B35,44 A1,24 B7,8 A24,25 B18,18 A1,11 B8,35 A2,3 B62,62 A1,1 B8,35 A2,2 B7,60 A2,30 B27,51 A1,3 B8,60 A2,3 B7,7 A2,3 B35,51 A3,3 B7,7 A2,25 B7,18 A1,1 B8,57 A3,3 B7,44 A1,2 B8,60 A2,3 B7,7 A1,24 B8,35 A2,3 B7,60 A2,24 B7,62 A24,25 B18,51 A1,3 B7,60
(d) EEG recording and analysis The EEG was recorded unipolarly from seven electrode positions (F3, Fz, F4, Cz, P3, Pz, P4) in reference to linked mastoids (bandpass: 0.016–30 Hz) and grounded at the position Oz. The recording time was 6 s for each trial, including 1 s baseline (sample rateZ128 Hz). Eye movements were corrected off-line (Elbert et al. 1985). For peak detection (maximum amplitudes) the bandpass was set to 0.053–4.7 Hz. Within the averaged potential, four peaks (N1, P2, P3-1, P3-2) were detected in relation to the averaged baseline (Pause et al. 1996). However, only those peaks which represented early exogenous or late endogenous stimulus processing were considered for further analysis. Therefore, CSERPs in response to the detected and rejected standard odours were compared with the CSERPs in response to the detected and rejected target odours (three-way ANOVA: Proc. R. Soc. B (2006)
detection (correct rejection, false alarms, hits, misses), anterior/posterior and hemisphere (left, central, right)). As the standard stimuli exclusively belonged to HLA-dissimilar donors, these analyses were carried out for odour stimuli from HLA-dissimilar odour donors only. Exogenous potentials were considered not to vary with the subjective stimulus significance, whereas endogenous potentials should be larger in response to subjectively meaningful (detected) stimuli. The main statistical analysis was performed by means of a six-way ANOVA, including the factors HLA (similar/ dissimilar), odour donor (same sex, opposite sex), subject (female, male), detection (hits, correctly rejected distractors), anterior/posterior and hemisphere. In case of significant interactions including the HLA factor, further analyses were carried out by isolating simple HLA effects within selected interactions, according to Levine (1991). By analysing the nested effects within selected interactions, multiple testing of
474 B. M. Pause and others (a) –5
Human brain and HLA-associated chemosignals
(µV)
(b)
(µV)
(d )
0
5
10 (c) –5
0
5
10 0
0.5
1.0 (s)
1.5
2.0
0
0.5
1.0 (s)
1.5
2.0
Figure 1. Grand averages across all subjects in response to body odours from HLA-similar donors (bold lines) and HLAdissimilar donors (thin lines). (a) Females smelling females. (b) Females smelling males. (c) Males smelling males. (d ) Males smelling females. All recordings from Pz; at time point 0 the odorous flow was activated. single comparisons is avoided. As the standard odours did not vary in terms of HLA similarity, the main analysis was carried out for the deviant odours only.
3. RESULTS (a) Detection performance Across all conditions, 50.3% of the targets were correctly detected, and 74.6% of the standards and 52.6% of the distractors were correctly rejected. The subjects’ detection performance was independent of HLA-related differences. (b) CSERPs in response to detected and undetected odour stimuli The amplitude of the N1 was generally larger in response to rare (hits: 2.01 mV, misses: 1.84 mV) than to frequent stimuli (correct rejections: 1.17 mV, false alarms: 1.10 mV; F(3,845)Z8.50, pZ0.004). However, the N1 was unaffected by the subjective task relevance of the odours. On the contrary, the amplitude of the P3-2 did vary with the subjective stimulus meaning (F(3,845)Z5.61, pZ0.019): it was larger in response to detected odours (hits: 4.17 mV, false alarms: 4.27 mV) than to undetected odours (correct rejections: 3.55 mV, misses: 3.10 mV). Additionally, the P3-2 showed a parietal maximum (F(1,845)Z219.44, p!0.001; anterior: 2.05 mV, posterior: 5.5 mV) and was largest above the midline (F(2,845)Z22.50, p!0.001; left: 3.04 mV, central: 4.86 mV, right: 3.42 mV). As the P2 and the P3-1 were neither affected by the level of detection nor by the probability of stimulus occurrence, they were not considered in further analyses. Proc. R. Soc. B (2006)
(c) CSERPs in response to odours of HLA-similar and HLA-dissimilar donors The CSERP analyses show that across all subjects body odours of HLA-similar donors were processed faster than those of HLA-dissimilar donors (Main effect of HLAsimilarity on N1 peak-latency: F(1,34)Z7.45, pZ0.010, powerZ0.755; HLA-similar: 384.5 ms, HLA-dissimilar: 407.7 ms). However, the speed of late, evaluative odour processing (P3-2) was not affected by the degree of HLA similarity between subject and odour donor. Additionally, HLA-related differences between odour donors and perceivers affected the strength of the neuronal brain response during late, evaluative stimulus processing (see figure 1a–d ). Odours of HLA-similar persons evoked larger P3-2 potentials than odours of HLA-dissimilar donors (HLA!anterior/posterior!hemisphere: F(2,68)Z 7.52, pZ0.010, powerZ0.935). Analyses of the simple main effects revealed that HLA-related differences were most prominent above medial posterior scalp areas (Pz; F(1,37)Z7.58, pZ0.009, powerZ0.765). The local distribution of the HLA-effects on the P3-2 potential was modulated by the sex of the odour donors and of the subjects. Generally, in same-sex smelling conditions (men smelling male odour or females smelling female odour), potentials evoked by odours of HLAsimilar donors were larger than the cortical evoked responses to odours of HLA-dissimilar donors at medial parietal electrodes (HLA!odour donor!anterior/ posterior!hemisphere: F(2,68)Z5.03, pZ0.032, powerZ0.800; nested effect for Pz: F(1,37)Z8.29, pZ0.007, powerZ0.801). However, analysing female
Human brain and HLA-associated chemosignals HLA-similar 13
B. M. Pause and others
475
HLA-dissimilar
(µV)
12 11 10 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 0
anterior posterior anterior posterior same sex
anterior posterior anterior posterior
opposite sex
same sex
opposite sex male subjects
female subjects
Figure 2. P3-2 amplitudes (means and standard deviations) in response to HLA-similar and dissimilar odour donors: separation for anterior and posterior electrode positions and for the sex of the subjects and of the odour donors (HLA!odour donor! subject!anterior/posterior; pZ0.008).
and male subjects separately (HLA!odour donor! subject!anterior/posterior: F(1,34)Z8.08, pZ0.008, powerZ0.789) it could be found that males show larger P3-2 amplitudes in response to odours of HLA-similar males above frontal scalp areas (F(1,37)Z7.90, pZ0.008, powerZ0.782), whereas females show larger potentials to odours of HLA-similar females above parietal scalp areas (F(1,37)Z5.88, pZ0.020, powerZ0.656; see figure 2). (d) Subjective ratings Group mean differences of subjective rating data indicate that subjects tended to reject body odours of donors with a similar HLA-type more than those with a dissimilar HLAtype (table 2). Moreover, correlations (Pearson) between the ratings for the potential partner preference and the P3-2 amplitude (mV; table 2) revealed significant results: high correlations were solely observed when subjects smelled odours from HLA-similar persons of the same sex ( p!0.05). In females, the P3-2 amplitude increased, the more negatively the body odours were evaluated. In males, the P3-2 amplitude was larger, the more positively the body odours were judged. 4. DISCUSSION First, the CSERP results as well as the correlation analyses of the subjective ratings reveal that odours of HLA-similar persons convey significant information, whereas odours of HLA-dissimilar persons are perceived as less relevant. Second, both analyses point to the conclusion that males and females process HLA-related chemosignals of the same sex differently. The CSERP results are related to two components, which reflect exogenous (N1) and endogenous (P3-2) stimulus processing (Donchin & Coles 1988). The N1 component did not vary with the subjective task relevance but was larger in response to the rare than to the frequent stimuli. This result is in accordance with the observation that the chemosensory N1 amplitude is prone to show Proc. R. Soc. B (2006)
Table 2. Mean ratings for the potential partner preferences and correlations with the P3-2 amplitude. (Note: Ratings varied between K3 (least preference) and C3 (highest preference). Correlations were performed across all six electrodes (F3, Fz, F4, P3, Pz, P4). Level of significance: *p!0.05.) design subject
donor
HLA
rating (mean)
correlation (Pearson)
females
same sex
males
opposite sex same sex
similar dissimilar similar dissimilar similar dissimilar similar dissimilar
K0.05 C0.25 C0.22 C0.33 K0.70 K0.35 K0.11 K0.11
K0.67* K0.35 K0.30 K0.20 C0.75* C0.02 K0.01 C0.24
opposite sex
strong (stimulus-specific) effects of habituation (see Pause 2002). On the contrary, the P3-2 component was larger in response to detected than to undetected odours and, additionally, showed a parietal maximum. This result is in line with a number of studies which demonstrate that the chemosensory P3-2 component represents the features of the traditional P3 component (reviewed by Pause & Krauel 2000). As the chemosensory P2 might be related to exogenous stimulus processing and the P3-1 to novelty detection (Pause et al. 1996), their functional significance could not be fully explained with the present data set and they were not considered for the HLA analyses. In general, and independent of the sex of the odour donor and smeller, CSERPs were larger (P3-2) and appeared with a shorter latency (N1) when the subjects processed body odours from HLA similar donors. We suggest that the HLA effect on the N1 latency is related to the precise neuronal timing within the primary (Spors & Grinvald 2002) and secondary (Lorig 1999)
476 B. M. Pause and others
Human brain and HLA-associated chemosignals
olfactory cortex, which is important for pre-attentive odour quality discrimination. Since the P3-2 is related to cognitive-emotional stimulus evaluation, our results indicate that more attentional resources are activated in response to body odours of HLA-similar donors than to odours from HLA-dissimilar sources. However, one could argue that the P3-2 in response to HLA-similar donors was larger, because the deviant stimulus 2 (HLA similar) was more different than the deviant stimulus 1 (HLA-dissimilar) from the standard stimulus (HLA-dissimilar, see table 1). However, this interpretation would indicate that HLA-related body odour differences between two people could be detected in general, without relating them to one’s own HLA-type. As so far, all evidence in humans points solely to the ability of humans to detect HLA-related body odours in relation to their own HLA-type (Ober et al. 1997; Wedekind & Fu¨ri 1997; Jacob et al. 2002) it is argued strongly that the HLA-related differences in brain activity, reported here, are due to the immunogenetic relatedness between the odour donor and the perceiver. Moreover, the N1 effect, which is independent of the oddball design, also refers to the fact that HLA-similarity is processed and is thus in line with the interpretation of the P3-2 effect stated herein. The fact that information about genetic similarity has a processing advantage in speed as well as in recruitment of neuronal resources is in line with findings on chemosensory and visual self-perception. As the processing of one’s own body odour is faster than the processing of non-self chemical signals (Pause et al. 1999b) and as the perception of one’s own face activates a distinct and unique neuronal network, including limbic and prefrontal brain areas (Kircher et al. 2001), the existence of a self-detecting neuronal assembly within the human brain seems to be likely. It is proposed that the detection of genetic similarity might be associated with this self-detection system, which, however, might become functional during a sensitive period in ontogenesis (see Yamazaki et al. 1988). It is further concluded that the behavioural impact of chemosensory signals related to HLA-similarity is stronger than of signals related to HLA-dissimilarity. As the HLA loci are the most polymorphic loci in the human genome (Parham & Ohta 1996), the probability of meeting unrelated individuals with a dissimilar HLAtype is extremely high. Therefore, the development of a preference for potential partners with a dissimilar HLA type might be related to other factors than to chemosensory cues, whereas the rejection of potential partners with a similar or identical HLA type might be most effectively determined by the rarely occurring chemosignals of self. Furthermore, it has been proposed that MHC-regulated inbreeding avoidance might lead to higher fitness benefits than MHC-heterozygosity (Penn 2002). Accordingly, inbreeding avoidance could be successfully achieved if MHC similarity is transmitted as an avoidance behaviour activating signal. In same sex conditions, the specific brain areas involved in the processing of HLA-related body odours are different in male and female perceivers. In males, large HLArelated potential differences occur above anterior scalp areas and in females above parietal scalp areas. The prefrontal cortex is assumed to play a key-role in the integration of valenced information (Davidson 2002; Anderson et al. 2003), while activity in the parietal cortex Proc. R. Soc. B (2006)
seems to be related to emotion-related arousal (Nitschke et al. 2000). It is, therefore, postulated that males process chemosensory signals from HLA-similar males primarily as hedonic information, while females process according signals primarily as arousing information. In line with these considerations, the most negative ratings were given by males responding to same sex odours, whereas in females, the hedonic ratings were less pronounced (table 2; group mean differences might not have reached the significance level, because the number of subjects in our electrophysiological study was much smaller than in studies designed to investigate subjective ratings). Thereby, the positive sign of the correlation in males indicates that emotionally positive odours of HLA-similar males were perceived as unexpected (average ratings are negative) and correspondingly elicited larger potentials. In contrast, females might have responded with larger potentials to odours of other HLA-similar females with an unexpected negative valence (inverse correlation; average ratings are indifferent). As the CSERPs of females and males, responding to HLA-related body odour signals of opposite sex donors, showed similar features, it remains possible that not only females but also males are involved in human mate selection (Yamazaki et al. 1976; Potts et al. 1991; Olsson et al. 2003). The effect that females also responded to HLA-similarity in same sex conditions, might be equivalent to the effects of kin recognition in female mice, demonstrating that females nest communally and appear to nest with MHC-similar females when siblings are unavailable (Manning et al. 1992). However, the HLAeffect on the chemosensory male–male communication raises the intriguing possibility that competitive behaviour in males may be modulated by HLA-associated chemosignals. So far, it is known that competition in male behaviour and sperm production can be initiated through chemosensory signals of conspecifics (Rich & Hurst 1998; DelBarco-Trillo & Ferkin 2004), and that male competitive behaviour can facilitate female mate choice (Lenington et al. 1992; Candolin 1999). Furthermore, the reduced fitness in inbred mice is related to the reduced survivorship of males in competitive conditions (Meagher et al. 2000). Thus, male competitive behaviour could have evolved secondary to female mate choice (Moore et al. 2001) on the basis of the same MHC-related regulatory mechanisms. The limitations of our study are related to the unknown relative contribution of the HLA-A and -B loci to the described effects. While so far no study in humans allows us to decide which of the different HLA genes contributes most to the individual body odour, in rats it has been shown that class I as well as class II regions of the MHC are responsible for the individuality of body odours (Brown et al. 1989). However, due to the fact that class I genes are expressed on the surface of all nucleated somatic cells, and that class II genes have a much more restricted expression pattern, most animal research on MHC-related body odours has focused on MHC-class I genes (see Penn 2002). In humans, the HLA-C region is the phylogenetically youngest among the classical class I genes (Kelley et al. 2005), and might, therefore, contribute less to chemosensory communication than the A- and B-region. We and others (Wedekind & Fu¨ri 1997) have,
Human brain and HLA-associated chemosignals therefore, focused the research on HLA-related chemoperception on the A- and B-region. While HLA-related body odours can be differentiated by rodents (Ferstl et al. 1990) and via analytic technologies (Eggert et al. 1999b; Montag et al. 2001), so far, the signalling properties of these chemosignals have exclusively been reported in human rating studies (Wedekind & Fu¨ri 1997; Jacob et al. 2002). In contrast to the latter, our CSERP results show that body odours from HLA-similar sources have a processing advantage and thus may convey more significant information than those from HLAdissimilar donors. Therefore, in humans, HLA-related signals seem to be associated to a negative selection bias in mating behaviour. Moreover, HLA-associated odour signals from same-sex persons are processed differently in males and females, pointing to different behavioural functions in male-to-male (competition) and female-tofemale (communal behaviour) relations. We thank B. Schaal, A. N. Gilbert and T. Elbert for their helpful comments on earlier drafts of the manuscript and F. Eggert for fruitful discussions throughout the experiment. Special thanks to B. Gottsmann, A. Krischer and K. Rogalski for their technical help during data acquisition. The study was supported by a grant from the Volkswagen foundation.
REFERENCES Anderson, A. K., Christoff, K., Stappen, I., Panitz, D., Ghahremani, D. G., Glover, G., Gabrieli, J. D. E. & Sobel, N. 2003 Dissociated neural representations of intensity and valence in human olfaction. Nat. Neurosci. 6, 196–202. (doi:10.1038/nn1001) Brown, J. L. & Eklund, A. 1994 Kin recognition and the major histocompatibility complex: an integrative review. Am. Nat. 143, 435–461. (doi:10.1086/285612) Brown, R. E., Roser, B. & Singh, P. B. 1989 Class I and class II regions of the major histocompatibility complex both contribute to individual odors in congenic inbred strains of rats. Behav. Genet. 19, 659–674. (doi:10.1007/ BF01066029) Candolin, U. 1999 Male–male competition facilitates female choice in sticklebacks. Proc. R. Soc. B 266, 785–789. (doi:10.1098/rspb.1999.0706) Davidson, R. J. 2002 Anxiety and affective style: role of prefrontal cortex and amygdala. Biol. Psychiatry 51, 68–80. (doi:10.1016/S0006-3223(01)01328-2) DelBarco, J. & Ferkin, M. H. 2004 Male mammals respond to a risk of sperm competition conveyed by odours of conspecific males. Nature 431, 446–449. (doi:10.1038/ nature02845) Donchin, E. & Coles, M. G. H. 1988 Is the P300 component a manifestation of context updating? Behav. Brain Sci. 11, 357–374. Eggert, F., Ferstl, R. & Mu¨ller-Ruchholtz, W. 1999a MHC and olfactory communication in humans. In Advances in chemical signals in vertebrates (ed. R. E. Johnston, D. Mu¨ller-Schwarze & P. W. Sorensen), pp. 181–188. New York: Kluwer Academic Press. Eggert, F., Luszyk, D., Haberkorn, K., Wobst, B., Vostrowsky, O., Westphal, E., Bestmann, H. J., Mu¨llerRuchholtz, W. & Ferstl, R. 1999b The major histocompatibility complex and the chemosensory signaling of individuality in humans. Genetica 104, 265–273. (doi:10. 1023/A:1026431303879) Elbert, T., Lutzenberger, W., Rockstroh, B. & Birbaumer, N. 1985 Removal of ocular artifacts from the EEG—a Proc. R. Soc. B (2006)
B. M. Pause and others
477
biophysical approach. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 60, 455–463. (doi:10.1016/0013-4694(85)91020-X) Ferstl, R., Pause, B., Schu¨ler, M., Luszyk, D., Eggert, F., Westphal, E. & Mu¨ller-Ruchholtz, W. 1990 Immune system signaling to the brain: MHC-specific odors in humans. In Psychiatry: a world perspective (ed. C. N. Stefanis, C. R. Soldatos & A. D. Rabavilas), pp. 751–755. Amsterdam: Elsevier. Jacob, S., McClintock, M. K., Zelano, B. & Ober, C. 2002 Paternally inherited HLA alleles are associated with women’s choice of male odor. Nat. Genet. 30, 175–179. (doi:10.1038/ng830) Kelley, J., Walter, L. & Trowsdale, J. 2005 Comparative genomics of major histocompatibility complexes. Immunogenetics 56, 683–695. (doi:10.1007/s00251-004-0717-7) Kircher, T. T. J. et al. 2001 Recognizing one’s own face. Cognition 78, B1–B15. (doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(00) 00104-9) Kobal, G. & Hummel, C. 1988 Cerebral chemosensory evoked potentials elicited by chemical stimulation of the human olfactory and respiratory nasal mucosa. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 71, 241–250. (doi:10. 1016/0168-5597(88)90023-8) Lenington, S., Coopersmith, C. & Williams, J. 1992 Genetic basis of mating preferences in wild house mice. Am. Zool. 32, 40–47. Levine, G. 1991 A guide to SPSS for analysis of variance. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum. Lorig, T. S. 1999 On the similarity of odor and language perception. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 23, 391–398. (doi:10. 1016/S0149-7634(98)00041-4) Manning, C. J., Wakeland, E. K. & Potts, W. K. 1992 Communal nesting patterns in mice implicate MHC genes in kin recognition. Nature 360, 581–583. (doi:10.1038/ 360581a0) Montag, S., Frank, M., Ulmer, H., Wernet, D., Go¨pel, W. & Rammensee, H.-G. 2001 “Electronic nose” detects major histocompatibility complex-dependent prerenal and postrenal odor components. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 98, 9249–9254. (doi:10.1073/pnas.161266398) Meagher, S., Penn, D. J. & Potts, W. K. 2000 Male–male competition magnifies inbreeding depression in wild house mice. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 97, 3324–3329. (doi:10.1073/pnas.060284797) Moore, A. J., Gowaty, P. A., Wallin, W. G. & Moore, P. J. 2001 Sexual conflict and the evolution of female mate choice and male social dominance. Proc. R. Soc. B 268, 517–523. (doi:10.1098/rspb.2000.1399) Moshkin, M. P., Gerlinskaya, L. A. & Evsikov, V. I. 2000 The role of the immune system in behavioral strategies of reproduction. J. Reprod. Dev. 46, 341–365. (doi:10.1262/ jrd.46.341) Nitschke, J. B., Heller, W. & Miller, G. A. 2000 Anxiety, stress, and cortical brain function. In The neuropsychology of emotion (ed. J. C. Borod), pp. 298–319. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Ober, C., Weitkamp, L. P., Cox, N., Dytch, H., Kostyu, D. & Elias, S. 1997 HLA and mate choice in humans. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 61, 497–504. (doi:10.1017/S0003480097 006532) Olsson, M., Madsen, T., Nordby, J., Wapstra, E., Ujvari, B. & Wittsel, H. 2003 Major histocompatibility complex and mate choice in sand lizards. Proc. R. Soc. B 270(Suppl.), S245–S256. Parham, P. & Ohta, T. 1996 Population biology of antigen presentation by MHC class I molecules. Science 272, 67–74. Pause, B. M. 2002 Human brain activity during the first second after odor presentation. In Olfaction, taste, and
478 B. M. Pause and others
Human brain and HLA-associated chemosignals
cognition (ed. C. Rouby, B. Schaal, D. Dubois, R. Gervais & A. Holley), pp. 309–323. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Pause, B. M. & Krauel, K. 2000 Chemosensory event-related potentials (CSERP) as a key to the psychology of odors. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 36, 105–122. (doi:10.1016/S01678760(99)00105-1) Pause, B. M., Sojka, B., Krauel, K. & Ferstl, R. 1996 The nature of the late positive complex within the olfactory event-related potential (OERP). Psychophysiology 33, 376–384. Pause, B. M., Sojka, B. & Ferstl, R. 1997 Central processing of odor concentration is a temporal phenomenon as revealed by chemosensory event-related potentials (CSERP). Chem. Senses 22, 9–26. Pause, B. M., Krauel, K., Sojka, B. & Ferstl, R. 1999a Is odor processing related to oral breathing? Int. J. Psychophysiol. 32, 251–260. (doi:10.1016/S0167-8760(99)00020-3) Pause, B. M., Krauel, K., Sojka, B. & Ferstl, R. 1999b Body odor evoked potentials: a new method to study the chemosensory perception of self and non-self in humans. Genetica 104, 285–294. (doi:10.1023/A:1026462701154) Penn, D. J. 2002 The scent of genetic compatibility: sexual selection and the major histocompatibility complex. Ethology 108, 1–21. (doi:10.1046/j.1439-0310.2002. 00768.x) Penn, D. J. & Potts, W. K. 1999 The evolution of mating preferences and major histocompatibility genes. Am. Nat. 153, 145–164. (doi:10.1086/303166)
Proc. R. Soc. B (2006)
Potts, W. K., Manning, C. J. & Wakeland, E. K. 1991 Mating patterns in seminatural populations of mice influenced by MHC genotype. Nature 352, 619–621. (doi:10.1038/ 352619a0) Rich, T. J. & Hurst, J. L. 1988 Scent marks as reliable signals of the competitive ability of mates. Anim. Behav. 56, 727–735. (doi:10.1006/anbe.1998.0803) Singh, P. B. 2001 Chemosensation and genetic individuality. Reproduction 121, 529–539. (doi:10.1530/rep.0. 1210529) Spors, H. & Grinvald, A. 2002 Spatio-temporal dynamics of odor representations in the mammalian olfactory bulb. Neuron 34, 301–315. (doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(02) 00644-X) Wedekind, C. & Fu¨ri, S. 1997 Body odour preferences in men and women: do they aim for specific MHC combinations or simply heterozygosity? Proc. R. Soc. B 264, 1471–1479. (doi:10.1098/rspb.1997.0204) Yamazaki, K., Boyse, E. A., Mike, V., Thaler, H. T., Mathieson, B. J., Abbott, J., Boyse, J., Zayas, Z. A. & Thomas, L. 1976 Control of mating preferences in mice by genes in the major histocompatibility complex. J. Exp. Med. 144, 1324–1335. (doi:10.1084/jem.144.5.1324) Yamazaki, K., Beauchamp, G. K., Kupniewski, J., Bard, J., Thomas, L. & Boyse, E. A. 1988 Familial imprinting determines H-2 selective mating preferences. Science 240, 1331–1332. As this paper exceeds the maximum length normally permitted, the authors have agreed to contribute to production costs.