Working Memory in ADHD: Prefrontal/Parietal ... - Semantic Scholar

18 downloads 50 Views 40KB Size Report
Oxford University Press. Publishers, Oxford and New York. [2]. Levy, F. (1991). The dopamine theory of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). ANZ J.
Current Drug Targets, 2001, 2, 347-352

347

Working Memory in ADHD: Prefrontal/Parietal Connections F. Levy1* and M. Farrow2 1

School of Psychiatry, University of New South Wales, Sydney, Australia; 2Brain Sciences Institute, Swinburne University of Technology, Melbourne, Australia Abstract: Current theories of dopaminergic and noradrenergic mechanisms, which are thought to be of importance in the regulation of attention are reviewed. A biphasic model of dopaminergic function is described, in which tonic dopamine exerts a suppressive influence on subcortical dopamine systems by altering tonic/phasic dopaminergic relationships. Noradrenergic mechanisms are of importance in modulating sensory processing at the prefrontal cortical level. The work of Silberstein and colleagues utilizing Steady-State Visually Evoked Potential, during the course of an A-X Continuous Performance Task enables examination of the spatial distribution and dynamics of electrical brain activity during the task. The maintenance of activation in the interval between A and X provides a measure of working memory, thought to be related to prefrontal-parietal activation, which is facilitated by administration of methylphenidate to children with ADHD, suggesting that working memory may be a core deficiency in children with ADHD. While tonic dopamine activity in vental striatum/accumbens gates inhibitory activity, dorsolateral prefrontal-parietal connections allow maintenance of working memory required for goal completion.

INTRODUCTION The investigation of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) in children has made advances in neurophysiological and genetic areas over the last ten years, but despite these advances the biological mechanisms involved in disturbances of attention are still unclear. A recent important text edited by Solanto, Arnsten, and Castellanos [1] discusses the basic and clinical neuroscience of stimulant drugs and ADHD, and draws attention to dopaminergic and noradrenergic influences, thought to be of importance in the action of stimulant drugs. The present review, outlines current theories, reviews work by the present authors, who used the Continuous Performance Test, as well as suggesting possible drug targets.

pallidus result in net amplification via thalamic excitatory fibers, which feedback to cortical output neurons, while an indirect pathway from the lateral globus pallidus and substantia nigra increased tonic cortical inhibition. Castellanos proposed that presynaptic effects might predominate in D2-rich subcortical regions, where presynaptic receptors are abundant, producing decreased synaptic dopamine and post-synaptic effects might predominate in D4-rich cortical regions, which lack presynaptic receptors, producing increased synaptic dopamine [5].

Castellanos [4] extended the above theory to suggest that neuronal signals which travel directly to the medial globus

Swanson et al [6] reviewed current genetic studies of dopamine genes and ADHD. They point out that family, twin and adoption studies have documented a strong genetic basis for ADHD. Two candidate genes, the dopamine transporter (DAT) and dopamine receptor D4 (DRD4) were investigated. While these studies provide converging evidence for the association of dopamine genes and ADHD (three published studies of the DAT1 gene [7-9] and four of five published studies of the DRD4 7-repeat allele [10-14,] found positive associations). A recent Swanson et al study [15] did not replicate the association of reaction time measures with the 7-repeat DRD4.allele. Swanson et al [6] point out that while both candidate dopamine genes (DAT1 and DRD4) have been reported to be associated with ADHD in different samples, the association has not been strong (i.e. a relative risk of about 1.5-2), suggesting that ADHD may have multiple causes. They postulate that the 7-R allele of the DRD4 gene may produce a receptor that is subsensitive to dopamine, while the I0-R allele of the DAT1 gene may be associated with a dopamine transporter, which is abnormally efficient at the re-uptake process.

*Address correspondence to this author at the Avoca Clinic, Prince of Wales Hospital, Sydney, NSW 2031, Australia

Single photon emission computed tomography studies in adults have shown that methylphenidate lowers increased striatal DAT availability in adults suffering from ADHD

DOPAMINE TRANSMITTERS Levy [2] discussed the implications of clinical, animal and neuroanatomical studies of differential isomer and dosage effects of central nervous system stimulant medications for the dopamine theory of attention deficit disorder. Central to the theory were animal studies by Glowinski et al [3], which demonstrated that mesocorticoprefrontal DA neurons lacked autoreceptors and presynaptic receptors, unlike the basal ganglia where dopaminergic activity is controlled by autoreceptors. These differences were thought to explain differential effects of stimulant medications at low compared with high dose levels.

1389-4501/01 $28.00+.00

© 2001 Bentham Science Publishers Ltd.

348

Current Drug Targets, 2001, Vol. 2, No. 4

[16]. Use of radioactive isotopes is not possible in children, but Volkow et al [17] have shown that plasma concentrations of 6 ng/ml of d-methylphenidate blocked an average 50% of DAT. Since plasma levels in excess of 6 ng/ml occur in ADHD children receiving typical methylphenidate doses of .3 to .6 mg/kg, this suggests that greater than 50% DAT blockade may be required for therapeutic efficacy. Seeman and Madras [18] have reviewed mechanisms of action of anti-hyperactivity medications (methylphenidate and dexamphetamine). They proposed that clinically relevant doses of stimulants might increase extra cellular background levels of dopamine above that of action potential of released dopamine. This results in a relatively reduced amplitude of impulse-associated dopamine, less activation of post-synaptic dopamine receptors, and thus less psychomotor activity. A similar biphasic model, described by Grace [19], in relation to dopaminergic mechanisms, is thought to be of importance in ADHD. Grace posits that when DA is released into the striatal synaptic cleft, in response to an action potential, it is rapidly removed from the synapse, by a highly efficient reuptake system. On the other hand, tonic DA level is thought to be mediated by stimulation of pre-synaptic heteroreceptors on DA terminals by corticostriatal glutamergic projections. Tonic DA exerts a suppressive influence on subcortical DA systems, and the response of ADHD children to stimulants may be achieved by altering tonic/phasic DA relationships to achieve more optimal regulatory levels. Grace has hypothesised that the hyperactivity and impulsivity of ADHD result from abnormally low tonic DA activity within the ventral striatum/nucleus accumbens, leading to abnormally high phasic DA responses. Tonic DA was proposed to be regulated, in part by frontal and cortical afferents to the accumbens. According to Grace [20] the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, which projects to the striatum, is also the cortical area believed to be responsible for the proper function of working memory, as discussed by Goldman-Rakic [21]. NORADRENERGIC TRANSMITTERS A role for noradrenergic transmitters was suggested in a review by Pliszka et al [22]. The authors presented a multistage hypothesis, which emphasised the interaction of norephinephrine, epinephrine and dopamine in the modulation of attention and impulse control. Positron emission tomographic research suggested a posterior attention orienting system modulated by noradrenaline (NE). This system which normally ‘primes’ the posterior cortex to external stimuli was thought to be dysregulated in ADHD, possibly via deficits in the locus coeruleus (LC). Nor-adrenergic mechanisms have been further clarified by the work of Arnsten, 1998 [23] and Arnsten and colleagues [24]. They described research in rodents and primates, which indicated the NE has an important influence on spatial working memory and attentional functions of the prefrontal cortex (PFC). They have shown beneficial effects of the alpha 2 NE agonist guanfacine on cognitive tasks, such as delayed response, delayed alternation and delayed match

Levy and Farrow

to sample with repeated stimuli. Arnsten et al [25] proposed that post-synaptic alpha 2A receptor stimulation inhibits irrelevant and distracting sensory processing through effects on pyramidal cells that project to (posterior) sensory association cortices. When a selective alpha, adrenergic agonist was infused into the PFC spatial working memory was impaired, suggesting an opposing role between alpha 1 and alpha 2 agonists. Solanto, Arnsten and Castellanos [1] have edited an important text on Stimulant Drugs and ADHD. Solanto et al [26] reviewed the critical role of the prefrontal cortex (PFC) in regulating attention, inhibiting inappropriate or impulsive behaviours and using working memory to plan, organise and guide behaviour effectively. Research in rodents and monkeys indicates that both dopamine and norepinephrine have profound effects on the cognitive functioning of the PFC. Low levels of dopamine D1 and norepinephrine alpha-2 receptor stimulation are essential for PFC function, while very high levels of dopamine D1 and alpha-1 receptor stimulation impair PFC function. Beneficial effects of guanfacine (α-2A adrenergic agonist) on cognitive performance, have been described by Arnsten [27] and Jakala et al [28]. Solanto et al [26] also discuss subcortical dysfunction at basal ganglia, cerebellar and locus coeruleus (LC) levels. The ventral striatum (nucleus accumbens) (as described by Grace) [19] plays a critical role in individuals with ADHD, where low tonic dopamine levels allow proportionately elevated phasic DA release. Berridge [29] reviewed the role of the LC. Recordings from the LC in monkeys show that during alert waking the LC exhibits moderate baseline firing. In contrast, the phasic responses of the LC to target stimuli were most pronounced during an optimal alert state, and diminished during either drowsy or hyperaroused states. Solanto [25] suggests that the PFC may regulate appropriate phasic response of the LC. Recent imaging studies [30] indicate that the cerebellum is activated during alert states, and cerebellar dysfunction may be involved in the hyperactivity observed in ADHD [31]. PREFRONTAL-PARIETAL INTERACTION While the above hypotheses suggest important roles for both anterior (dopaminergic) and posterior (noradrenergic) attention systems in ADHD, there have been few, if any direct demonstrations of such reciprocal relationships in ADHD children. However, recent work reviewed below [32], utilizing the Continuous Performance Task, suggests prefrontal-parietal interactions in ADHD children. Levy and Hobbes [33] described the use of an agenormalised, Continuous Performance Test (X version) which distinguished a group of 15 hyperkinetic children from 83 normal school children. Levy and Hobbes [34] utilised the CPT to examine the effect of methylphenidate on task performance after a prior treatment with haloperidol. Methylphenidate was shown to diminish attention deficits in

Working Memory in ADHD: Prefrontal/Parietal Connections

a group of 12 boys, diagnosed with attention deficit disorder with hyperactivity, but when preceded by haloperidol, this effect was blocked in all the vigilance subtests. The findings were thought to imply a primarily dopaminergic action of methylphenidate in attention-disordered children. Levy and Hobbes [35] utilized data from the 1988 study to examine the effect of methylphenidate over the course of the Continuous Performance Task (Blocks 1 and 2). They showed a significant drug vs time interaction over the course of the CPT. This effect was antagonised by haloperidol. The study was the first to show a significant effect of methylphenidate (and of haloperidol in opposite direction) during the second half of the CPT task. Methylphenidate was thought to allow increased activation over the course of the task. Event Related Potential (ERP) studies of ADHD have also been used to examine activation processes in ADHD children. The most commonly examined componenent is the P300, that is a late positive wave with a latency of 300 to 800 ms [36]. The amplitude of the P300 is influenced by stimulus probality or relevance, and the latency is influenced by cognitive, perceptual or memory load [36]. It is thought to reflect allocation of attention and stimulus evaluation processes, as well as updating of internal representations and working memory [37]. Most studies in ADHD children have found the amplitude of the P300 to be reduced in children with ADHD [36-38]. Silberstein et al [32] and Farrow et al (in preparation) have utilized a Steady-State Visually Evoked Potential (SSVEP), which measures cortical electrical activity over the course of an A-X Continuous Performance Task (children are asked to press a button when they see an X proceeded by an A). The technique, also known as Steady-State Probe Topography (SSPT), enables examination of disturbances in the spatial distribution and dynamics of brain electrical activity. (Reductions in SSVEP amplitude have previously been associated with increased cortical activation [39,40]). The investigators found that compared to the mean amplitude during a reference task, control subjects demonstrated SSVEP amplitude reductions, during the A-X interval. Transient amplitude reductions occurred in frontal regions, while right parietal and occipital amplitude reductions were sustained throughout the 3.5 second A-X interval. In contrast, ADHD subjects demonstrated much smaller frontal amplitude reductions and increased parieto-occipital amplitude (ie less activation) suggesting they failed to increase cortical activation according to task demands. The largest group differences occurred at the disappearance of the A, when controls showed extensive activation, particularly in the parieto-occipital region, while ADHD subjects showed reduced activation in the parieto-occipital region. The disappearance of the A also coincided with large latency reductions in controls at frontal and temporal sites, while in ADHD subjects there were latency increases. A further study (Farrow et al, in preparation) examined changes in SSVEP following methylphenidate administration in 60 boys with ADHD (mean age 10 years 1 month), recently diagnosed according to DSM–IV criteria

Current Drug Targets, 2001, Vol. 2, No. 4 349

(Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of the American Psychiatric Association – 4th edition). SSVEP amplitude and latency during the A-X interval, before methylphenidate, and 90 minutes after administration of 0.3 mg/kg of methylphenidate was examined. After methylphenidate, there was increased activation predominantly in frontal and occipital regions, and reduced parieto-occipital latency, at the disappearance of A, in the A-X interval. The most sustained and significant changes occurred in the right parietal region.There were also transient reductions in frontal latency, particularly after the disappearance of the A. The results are thought to suggest that right prefrontal and right parietal processes, which are involved in performance of the CPTAX task in healthy control children, are deficient in ADHD children, and are enhanced by methylphenidate in ADHD children. In particular, these processes may be involved in the maintenance of activation in the interval between A and X, allowing a ‘working’ retention of the parameters necessary for correct task performance. Goldman-Rakic [21,41] described the role of the primate pre-frontal cortex in spatial cognition, from the point of view of connectivity with major neurological centers. The prefrontal cortex in primate studies was found to be integral to delayed response tasks, which required behaviour to be guided by representations of discrimination stimuli, rather than those stimuli. Thus, the work of Goldman-Rakic in primates and the above SSVEP findings suggest that prefrontal-parietal reverberating reciprocal circuits maintain the visuo-spatial representation required for delayed response and for CPT performance. The activation of cortical circuits is maintained by subcortical tone acting directly and indirectly [3, 42]. Gray et al [42] elaborated a model describing intercorrelations between basal ganglia and accumbens, which monitor switching from one step to to the next of a motor program. Goldman-Rakic et al [43] have described the anatomical overlap of different monoaminergic receptors in the same cortical strata, suggesting that there may be families of receptors linked by localization on common targets. This would provide the anatomical basis for subcortical influences on prefrontal/parietal systems. On the other hand, the complimentary laminar distribution of D1 vs D2, 5-HT1 vs 5HT2, and alpha vs beta adrenergic receptors, raises the possibility that different subtypes within a given class, may have distinctive actions in the cortex, by virtue of their localisation on different cells, or possibly on different portions of the same cell. For example, Wilson et al [44] have shown that the prefrontal cortex is segregated into object and spatial domains with separate connections. Denny and Rapport [45] have described a model, which posits that working memory plays a pivotal role in the organisation of behaviour. Organised responding is functionally dependant on the capacity of working memory to (a) generate and maintain representations of input stimuli (b) search memory traces for matches and (c) access and maintain representations of behavioural responses appropriate to input stimuli. Failure of working memory leads not only to disorganised behaviour but also motivates children to redirect their attention to other stimuli in the environment (stimulation seeking), giving rise to frequent

350

Current Drug Targets, 2001, Vol. 2, No. 4

rapid shifts in activity. Working memory is viewed as a causal cognitive process that is the direct consequence of one or more neurobiological substrates. The present review suggests that prefrontal-parietal activation is an important component of working memory in CPT tasks, shown to be impaired in ADHD children, and suggests that CPT models should be useful in the investigation of drug targets for ADHD. While the inhibitory role of prefrontal/striatal circuits have been described by Castellanos [4] and others, the importance of prefrontal/parietal pathways for working memory has not been emphasised (see Castellanos [46, pp 244] for path diagram). While the inhibitory role of prefrontal/striatal circuits have been described by Castellanos and others, the importance of prefrontal/parietal pathways for working memory has not been emphasised. Thus, while the tonic dopaminergic activity within the ventral striatum/accumbens described by Grace [20] gates the inhibitory activity of the prefrontal cortex, the dorsolateral prefrontal/parietal connections allow maintenance of working memory required for goal completion. ABBREVIATIONS

Levy and Farrow

[5]

Swanson, J.M. and Castellanos, F. X. (1997). Biological basis of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: Neuroanatomy, genetics and pathophysiology NIH Consensus Conference on ADHD, NIH, Rockville, Md.

[6]

Swanson, J. M.; Flodman, P.; Kennedy, J; Spence, M.A.; Moyzis, R.; Schuck, S.; Murias, M.; Moriarty, J.; Barr, C.; Smith,; Posner, M. (2000). Dopamine genes and ADHD. Neurosci. and Biobehav. Rev., 24, 21-25.

[7]

Cook, E.H.; Stein, M.A.; Krasowski, M.D.; Cox, N.J.; Olken, D.M.; Keiffer, J.E. (1995). Association of attentiondeficit disorder and the dopamine transporter gene. Am. J. of Hum. Genet., 56, 993-998.

[8]

Gill, M.; Daly G; Heron, S.; Hawi, Z.; Fitzgerald, M. (1997). Confirmation of association between attention deficit disorder and the dopamine transporter polymorphism. Mol. Psychiatry, 2, 311-313.

[9]

Waldman, I.D.; Rowe, D.C.; Abramowitz, A.; Kozel, S.T.; Mohr, J.H.; Sherman, S.L.; Cleveland, H.H.; Sanders, M.L.; Gard, J.M.C.; Stever,C. (1998). Association and linkage of the dopamine transporter gene and attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder in children: heterogeneity owing to diagnostic subtype and severity. Am. J. Human Genet., 63.

[10]

Swanson, J. M.; Sunahara, G. A.; Kennedy, J.L.; Regino, R.; Fineberg, E.; Wigal, T.; Lerner, M.; Williams, L.; La Hoste, G. J.; Wigal, S. (1998). Association of the dopamine receptor D4 (DRD4) gene with a refined phenotype of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): A familybased approach. Mol. Psychiat., 3, 38-41.

[11]

La Hoste, G. J.; Swanson, J. M.; Wigal, S. B.; Glabe, C.; Wigal, T.; King, N.; Kennedy, J.L. (1996). Dopamine D4 receptor gene polymorphism is associated with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Mol. Psychiat., 1, 121-124.

[12]

Rowe, M. I.; Stever, C.; Giedinghaven, L. N.; Gard, J. M. C.; Cleveland, H.H.; Terris, S. T.; Mohr, J.H.; Sherman, S.; Abramowitz, A.; Waldman, I.D. (1998). Dopamine DRD4 receptor polymorphism and attention deficit disorder. Mol. Psychiat., 3, 419-426.

[13]

Smalley, S. L.; Bailey, J. N. Palmer, C. G.; Cantwell, D. P.; McGough, J. J.; Del’Homme, M. A.; Asamow, J. R.; Woodward, J. A.; Ramsey, C.; Nelson, S. F. (1998). Mol. Psychiat., 3, 427-430.

[14]

Castellanos, F. X.; Lau, E.; Tayebi, N.; Lee, P.; Long, R.E.; Giedd, J. N.; Sharp, W.; Marsh, W. L.; Walter, J. M.; Hamburger, S. D.; Ginns, E. L.; Rapoport, J. L.; Sidransky, E. (1998). Lack of an association between a dopamine-4 receptor polymorphism and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: genetic and brain morphometric analyses. Mol. Psychiat., 3, 431-434.

[15]

Swanson, J.; Oosterlaan, J.; Murias, M.; Schuck, S.; Flodman, P.; Spence, M.A. et al. (2000). Attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder children with a 7-repeat allele of the dopamine receptor D4 gene have extreme behavior but normal performance on critical neuropsychological tests of attention. PNAS, 97(9), 4754-4759.

[16]

Krause, K-H.; Dresel, S.H.; Krause, J.; Kung, H.F.; Tatsch, K. (2000). Increased striatal dopamine transporter in adult patients with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder; effects of methylphenidate as measured by single photon emission computed tomography. Neurosci. Let., 285, 107-110.

ADHD = Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder DA

= Dopamine

DAT

= Dopamine transporter

DRD4 = Dopamine 4 receptor gene NE

= Noradrenaline

LC

= Locus coeruleus

CPT

= Continuous Performance Task

ERP

= Event Related Potential

SSVEP = Steady State Visually Evoked Potential SSPT

= Steady State Probe Tomography

REFERENCES [1]

Solanto, M.V., Arnsten, A.F.T., Castellanos, F.K. (2001). Stimulant Drugs and ADHD: Basic and Clinical Neuroscience. Oxford University Press. Publishers, Oxford and New York.

[2]

Levy, F. (1991). The dopamine theory of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). ANZ J. Psychiat., 25, 277283.

[3]

Glowinski, J., Tassin, J.P., Thierry, A.M. (1984). The mesocortico-prefrontal dopaminergic neurons. TINS, (Nov.) 415-418.

[4]

Castellanos, F. X. (1997). Toward a pathophysiology of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. Clinical Paediatrics, 36, (July), 381-393.

Working Memory in ADHD: Prefrontal/Parietal Connections

[17]

Volkow, N.D.; Wang, G. J.; Gatley, S.J.; Fowler, J.S.; Logan, J.; Ding, Y.S.; Hitzemann, R.; Pappas, N. (1998). Dopamine transporter occupancies in the human brain induced by therapeutic doses of oral methylphenidate. Am. J. Psychiatry, 155, 1325-31.

[18]

Seeman, P. and Madras, B.K. (1999). Anti-hyperactivity medication: Methylphenidate and amphetamine, Mol. Psychiat., 3, 386-396.

[19]

Grace, A.A. (1995). The tonic/phasic model of dopamine system regulation; its relevance for understanding how stimulant abuse can alter basal ganglia function. Drug and Alc. Depend., 37, 111-729

[20]

Grace, A.A. (2001). Psychostimulant actions on dopamine and limbic system function: Relevance to the pathophysiology and treatment of ADHD in, Stimulant Drugs and ADH: Basic and Clinical Neuroscience (Solanto, M.V. Arnsten, A..F.T.,Castellanos, F.X. Eds). Oxford University Press, Publishers, Oxford and New York pp. 134-157.

[21]

[22]

[23]

A..F.T.,Castellanos, F.X. Eds). Stimulant Drugs and AD HD: Basic and Clinical Neuroscience. Publishers, Oxford University Press. Oxford and New York. pp. 158-184. [30]

Castellanos, F. X. (2001). Neuroimaging studies of ADHD. In, (Solanto, M.V. Arnsten, A..F.T.,Castellanos, F.X. Eds) Stimulant Drugs and ADHD: Basic and Clinical Neuroscience. Publishers, Oxford University Press. Oxford and New York. pp. 243-258.

[31]

Castellanos, F.X.; Giedd, J.N.; Marsh, W.L.; Hamburger, S.D.; Vaituzis, A.C.; Dickstein, D.P. et al (1996). Quantitative brain magnetic resonance imaging in attentiondeficit/hyperactivity disorder. Arch. Gen. Psychiat., 53, 607-616.

[32]

Silberstein, R.B.; Farrow, M.; Levy, F.; Pipingas, A.; Hay, D.A.; Jarman, J.C. (1998). Functional brain electrical activity mapping in boys with AttentionDeficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. Arch. Gen. Psychiat., 55, 1105-1112.

[33]

Goldman-Rakic, P.S. (1996). The prefrontal landscape implications of functional architecture for understanding human mentation and the central executive. Philos Trans R. Soc. Land. B. Biol. Sci., 35, 1445-1453.

Levy, F. and Hobbes, G. (1981). The diagnosis of Attention Deficit Disorder (Hyperkinesis) in children. J. Am. Acad. Child Psychiatry, 20, 376-384.

[34]

Plizka, S.R.; McCracken, JT.; Maas, J.W.; (1996). Catecholamines in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: current perspectives. J. Amer. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry, 35, 264-272.

Levy, F and Hobbes G. (1988). The action of stimulant medication in Attention Deficit Disorder with hyperactivity: Dopaminergc, noradrenergic or both? J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry, 27(6), 802-805.

[35]

Arnsten A.F.T. (1998). Catecholamine modulation of prefrontal cortical cognitive function. Trends in Cog. Sci., 2, 436-447.

Levy, F. and Hobbes, G. (1996). Does haloperidol block methylphenidate? Motivation or attention?. Psychopharmacol., 126, 70-74.

[36]

Klorman, R.; Brumaghim, J.T.; Borgstedts, A.D.; Salzman, L.F. (1991). How event-related potentials help understand the effects of stimulants on attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. In (eds, Jennings, J.R., Ackles, P.K., Coles, M.G.H.) Advances in Psychophysiology , Vol. 4. Publishers, London: Jessica Kingsley, pp. 107-153.

[37]

Tannock, R. (1998). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Advances in cognitive, neurobiological, and genetic research. J. Child Psychol. Psychiat., 39, 65-99.

[38]

Levy, F and Ward, P. (1995). Neurometrics, dynamic brain imaging and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. J. Paediatr. Child Health, 31, 279-283.

[39]

Silberstein, R.B. (1995). Steady-state visually evoked potentials, brain resonances, and cognitive processes. In (ed, Nunez, P.L.) Neocortical Dynamics and Human EEG Rhythms. Publishers, New York, NY: Oxford University Press, pp. 272-303.

[40]

Farrow, M.; Silberstein, R.B.; Levy, F.; Pipingas, A.; Wood, K.; Hay, D.A. Jarman, F. C. (1996). Prefrontal and parietal deficits in ADHD suggested by brain electrical activity mapping during children’s performance of the AX CPT. Australian Educat. Dev. Psychol., 13(1), 59-68.

[41]

Goldman-Rakic, P.S. (1987). Circuitry of primate prefrontal cortex and regulation of behaviour by representation memory. In, Handbook of Physiology: The Nervous System. Higher Functions of the Brain. Bethesda, MD: American Physiological Society, Sect.1, Mol. M, pp. 373-417.

[42]

Gray, J.J.; Feldon, J.; Rawlins, J.N.P.; Hemsley, D.R.; Smith, R.D. (1991). . The neuropsychology of schizophrenia. Behav. Brain Sci., 14, 1-84.

[24]

Arnsten, A.; Steere, J.C.; Hunt, R.D.(1996). The contribution of alpha sub2-noradrenergic mechanisms to prefrontal cortical cognitive function: Potential significance for Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder. Arch. Gen. Psychiat., 53, 448-455.

[25]

Arnsten, A.F.T.; Steere, J.C.; Jentsch, D.J.; Li, B.M. (1998). Noradrenergic influences on prefrontal cortical cognitive function: Opposing actions at postjunctional alpha1 versus alpha2- adrenergic receptors. Adv. Pharmacol., 42, 764-767.

[26]

Current Drug Targets, 2001, Vol. 2, No. 4 351

Solanto, M.V.; Arnsten, A. F. T.; Castellanos, F.X. (2001). The neuroscience of stimulant drug action. In (Solanto, M.V. Arnsten, A..F.T.,Castellanos, F.X. Eds). Stimulant Drugs and ADHD: Basic and Clinical Neuroscience. Publishers, Oxford University Press, Oxford and New York. pp. 355-379.

[27]

Arnsten, A.F.T. (2001). Dopamineric and noradrenergic influences on cognitive functions mediated by the prefrontal cortex. In, Stimulant Drugs and ADHD: Basic and Clinical Neuroscience. Publishers, Oxford University Press. Oxford and New York. pp. 185-208.

[28]

Jakala, P.; Rieckkinen, M.; Siervio, J.; Koivisto, E.; Kejonen, K.; Vanhanen,M. et al. (1999). Guanfacine, but not clonidine, improves planning and working memory performance in humans. Neuropsychopharmacol., 20, 460470.

[29]

Berridge, C.W. (2001). Arousal and Attention- related actions on the locus coerulus – noradrenergic system: Potential target in the therapeutic actions of amphetaminelike stimulants. In, (Solanto, M.V. Arnsten,

352

Current Drug Targets, 2001, Vol. 2, No. 4

[43]

Goldman-Rakic, P.S.; Lidow, M.S.; Gallager, D.W. (1990). Overlap of dopaminergic, adrenergic and serotonergic receptors and complimentarity of their subtypes in primate prefrontal cortex. J. Neurosci., 10(7), 2125-2138.

[44]

Wilson, F.A.W.; Goldman-Rakic, P.S. (1993). Dissociation of object and spatial processing domains in primate prefrontal cortex. Science, 260, 1955-1958.

[45]

Denny, C.B. and Rapport, M.D. (2001). Cognitive pharmacology of stimulants in children with ADHD. In,

Levy and Farrow

(Solanto, M.V. Arnsten, A..F.T.,Castellanos, F.X. Eds) Stimulant Drugs and ADHD: Basic and Clinical Neuroscience. Publishers, Oxford University Press. Oxford and New York. pp. 283-302. [46]

Castellanos, F.X. (2001). Neuroimaging studies of ADHD. In, (Solanto, M.V. Arnsten, A..F.T.,Castellanos, F.X. Eds) Stimulant Drugs and ADHD: Basic and Clinical Neuroscience. Publishers, Oxford University Press. Oxford and New York. pp. 243-258.

Suggest Documents