APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY
Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 15: S49±S73 (2001) DOI: 10.1002/acp.834
Dissociation, Memory and Suggestibility in Adults and Children MITCHELL L. EISEN1* and STEVEN J. LYNN2 1
California State University, Los Angeles, USA State University of New York, Binghamton, USA
2
SUMMARY The present article reviews research and theory related to dissociation, memory, and suggestibility in adults and children. We examine various manifestations of dissociation (e.g. trait versus state accounts of dissociation, pathological versus non-pathological dissociation), the different ways that memory and suggestibility are de®ned and measured across studies, and the extent to which dissociation is related to suggestibility as a function of the de®nition and measurement of relevant constructs. Finally, we examine the relationship between dissociation and children's resistance to misinformation, provide a critical review of the strengths and limitations of the extant literature, and suggest directions for future research and theoretical developments in the ®eld. Copyright # 2001 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Controversy surrounding the validity of children's allegations of abuse and legal cases related to false, repressed, and recovered memories have focused considerable attention on children's memory of traumatic events (Lindsay, 1998). This wave of interest in trauma and memory has fueled considerable speculation regarding the role that dissociation plays in how adults and children process traumatic experiences (see Putnam, 1997 for a review). Early on in the false memory debate, dissociation emerged as a widely accepted explanatory mechanism for reported memory lapses and distortions in cases of child sexual abuse (see Brown et al., 1998 for a review). Clinical theories of trauma, dissociation, and memory have been driven in large part by anecdotal reports and case studies of traumatized adults (Spiegel, 1991) and children (Terr, 1991) who reported dissociative experiences that adversely affected their memory for personally experienced traumatic events. Clinical theories have garnered support from research that revealed gaps in adults' recall of traumatic and/or abusive events experienced in childhood (Williams, 1994), as well as more general or non-speci®c gaps in the autobiographical memory accounts of adults who report a history of childhood abuse (Edwards and Fivush, 1998). However, controversy continues to surround the question of whether these memory gaps are speci®c to, or enhanced by, traumatic experiences and dissociative mechanisms or, are instead part and parcel of `normal' memory (Loftus, 1993; Shobe and Kihlstrom, 1997). Another area of controversy is whether memories that are purportedly dissociated or walled off from normal conscious introspection are potentially retrievable in unadulterated form and content or are, instead, subject to the vagaries of memory decay and distortion that Correspondence to: Mitchell L. Eisen, Department of Psychology, California State University, Los Angeles, CA 90032, USA. E-mail:
[email protected]
Copyright # 2001 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
S50
M. L. Eisen and S. J. Lynn
characterize mundane processing of information. Controversies aside, the study of dissociation has fascinating implications for understanding the nature of attention and memory. The present article describes what is currently known about the interrelationship of dissociation, memory, and suggestibility in adults and children and provides a useful framework to guide future research and theory development. In the course of our discussion we will: (a) present a number of different conceptualizations of dissociation and dissociative phenomena, (b) distinguish pathological and non-pathological dissociation as well as situational versus trait accounts of dissociation, (c) explore a variety of ways that dissociation can potentially affect event memory and resistance to misinformation, and (d) examine the relationship between dissociation and memory errors in the context of a variety of different false memory paradigms. DISSOCIATION: DEFINITIONS AND DEVELOPMENT Contemporary conceptualizations of dissociation vary greatly. The construct of dissociation has been used to describe a wide range of experiences and symptoms (see Cardena, 1994). The DSM-IV de®nes dissociation as a `disruption in the usually integrated functions of consciousness, memory, identity, or perception of the environment (American Psychiatric Association, 1994, p. 477). Other commonly cited de®nitions include: `The lack of the normal integration of thoughts, feelings, and experiences into the stream of consciousness and memory' (Bernstein and Putnam, 1986, p. 727), `a compartmentalization of experience' (van der Kolk et al., 1996, p. 306), and a form of cognitive avoidance (Carlson, 1997). Central to each of these conceptualizations is the idea that dissociation involves a degree of separation of thoughts or memories from conscious awareness and/or discontinuity in the ¯ow of consciousness. Although a common de®nitional core can be identi®ed, the literature in this area is complex and can be dif®cult to assimilate insofar as the construct of dissociation has been invoked to describe both a pervasive trait that can be observed and measured across time and settings, as well as a `peritraumatic' or situationally bound response to extreme stress or trauma (a so-called `dissociative state') that can occur independent of pre-existing personality or constitutional factors. Additionally, dissociation has been conceptualized in terms of pathological and non-pathological variants. We begin our review with a consideration of the literature on pathological versus non-pathological dissociation and trait versus state accounts of this construct. Non-pathological dissociation Dissociative phenomena are widely believed to be a normal aspect of everyday life (see Lynn and Rhue, 1994). In order to better understand pathological dissociation, it is ®rst necessary to describe non-pathological dissociation that does not necessarily re¯ect serious psychopathology. According to Putnam (1995), a child's dissociativity can be de®ned as ` . . . a function of the frequency and duration with which the individual spontaneously enters into a dissociative state of consciousness in naturally occurring situations' (p. 585). Non-pathological dissociation encompasses experiences in which attention is deeply absorbed or focused (e.g. watching a beautiful sunset), and there is a loss of awareness or degradation of monitoring of internal states and external activities. Copyright # 2001 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 15: S49±S73 (2001)
Dissociation and suggestibility
S51
Two pervasive traits associated with nonpathological dissociation in adults and children are absorption (i.e. the ability to set ordinary reality aside temporarily while engaging in fantasy: Tellegen and Atkinson, 1974), and fantasy proneness (i.e., longstanding history of profound imaginative involvements: Wilson and Barber, 1978, 1983; Lynn and Rhue, 1988). Fantasy and imaginal activities are thought to have a dissociative dimension involving a loss of awareness of external events, problems in reality monitoring, the feeling of being on `automatic pilot', and vivid daydreaming. In support of the link between dissociation and imaginal activities, are ®ndings that dissociation, fantasy proneness, and imaginative involvement are at least moderately intercorrelated. Green et al. (presentation to the annual meeting of the American Psychological Association, San Francisco, CA, 1991) administered a measure of fantasy proneness to 1249 college students, along with four of the most widely used measures of dissociation and found that the measures of fantasy proneness and dissociation correlated in the range of 0.47 to 0.63. Segal and Lynn (1992±1993) also found evidence for moderate correlations (r 0.40±0.44) between two measures of dissociation and scales of daydreaming and poor attentional control. Finally, Rhue et al. (1996) found that a measure of childhood dissociation was correlated at 0.54 with a childhood fantasy inventory and 0.39 with a measure of fantasy proneness adapted for use with children. Pathological dissociation In contrast to relatively benign dissociation associated with escapist fantasy and daydreaming, for example, pathological dissociation is thought to have serious long-term personal repercussions (Putnam, 1985). Pathological dissociation is characterized by disturbing symptoms that include depersonalization, amnesia, identity confusion, and derealization. These symptoms can be so severe and disabling that they can lead to a diagnosis of a dissociative disorder (Putnam, 1997; Steinberg, 1994). Pathological dissociation has consistently been linked to a history of trauma (Lilienfeld et al., 1999; Waller et al., 1996). The idea that there is an intimate connection between trauma and dissociation can be traced to the turn of the twentieth century when Janet (1919/1925) proposed that dissociation involves a defensive segregation of memories, or what he called `disaggregated memories'. Janet observed that when individuals are overwhelmed by traumatic events that clash with existing schemas of safety, for example, they are often unable to incorporate the events into their narrative memory, and subsequently avoid thinking about the trauma-related information altogether. Janet dubbed this process `phobia of memory' (Janet, 1919/1925) and contended that the continued avoidance of trauma-related cues insured that the highly charged memories would be splitoff from ordinary consciousness, leading to dramatic pathological symptoms such as amnesias and ``multiple personalities'' in extreme cases (see van der Hart and Freidman, 1989 for a review of Janet's theory). Over the past two decades, investigators have documented a strong association between dissociation and a variety of traumatic experiences. These experiences include severe childhood sexual and physical abuse (Briere, 1992; Chu and Dill, 1990; Putnam, 1989), witnessing a violent death during childhood (Putnam et al., 1986), combat exposure (Brende, presentation at the Conferance for Multiple Personality Disorders and Dissociative States, Chicago, IL, 1986), and rape in adolescence or adulthood (Coons and Milstein, 1986). However, it should be noted that speci®cation of the precise role of trauma in engendering dissociation and other manifestations of psychopathology is limited by the Copyright # 2001 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 15: S49±S73 (2001)
S52
M. L. Eisen and S. J. Lynn
fact that many of the studies in this area have relied on retrospective reports and have not examined the role of other potential contributing factors such as the nature of the family relationships in which abuse or violence is rooted. A dimensional versus typological perspectives on the trait of dissociation Until recently, it was widely believed that non-pathological and pathological dissociation represented ends of a single continuum of a normally distributed trait in the population (see Sandberg and Lynn, 1992). In fact, most research instruments designed to assess the trait of dissociation were originally based on a dimensional model of dissociation (DES: Bernstein and Putnam, 1986; Adolescent Dissociative Experiences Scale, A-DES: Armstrong et al., 1997; Child Dissociative Checklist, CDC: Putnam et al., 1993; Children's Perceptual Alteration Scale, CPAS: Evers-Szostak and Sanders, 1992; Trauma Symptom Inventory, TSI: Briere, 1995). In contrast with the view of dissociation representing a single dimension, Waller et al.'s (1998) analyses of data from a multi-site study provided support for the proposition that there may be two distinct ways that dissociation presents as a measurable trait. Their study revealed that certain patients experience symptoms that are reported by only a small minority of the general population. Speci®cally, a subset of eight DES items (The DES-T subscale of the DES) re¯ects a pathological form of dissociation that is taxonic in nature, rather than continuous. This typological perspective is consistent with the existence of two distinct types of dissociative tendencies: Pathological and non-pathological dissociation. THE DEVELOPMENT OF DISSOCIATION IN CHILDREN A common view of pathological dissociation is that it is initially an adaptive strategy that becomes habitual at some point, leading to conditioned dissociative reactions that prevent adequate processing and integration of information related to highly stressful or traumatic events (Koopman et al., 1994; Lynn and Rhue, 1994; Speigel, 1991). Perry et al. (1995) contend that enduring trauma engenders information processing and memory de®cits. According to Perry's evolutionary/neurobiological perspective, a child's typical initial response to overwhelming threat is to freeze. When suf®ciently terrorized, Perry proposes that the freezing response can evolve into a complete dissociation, and the virtual cessation of cognitive processing. Perry and his colleagues add that continued trauma can sensitize a child to the hyper arousal, leading to a pattern of dissociative responding under stress. Perry's conceptualization is based on the idea that a child's brain changes in a use dependent manner in response to patterns of neural activation cued by experience (Perry, 1994, presentation at the 41st Annual Meeting of the Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, New York, 1994). Perry proposes that speci®c patterns of neural activation associated with acute responses to trauma (e.g. dissociation) are likely to be consolidated on the neural level and predispose the child to develop dissociative disorders over time. Perry's position is consistent with a growing consensus in the area of developmental cognitive neuroscience that early repetitive or chronic stress can lead to long-term changes in known neural mechanisms related to memory, which, in turn, are associated with dissociation (see Nelson and Carver, 1998 for a review). Studies that can be considered supportive of Perry's viewpoint have reported the following results: Copyright # 2001 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 15: S49±S73 (2001)
Dissociation and suggestibility
S53
(1) The younger children are when they ®rst experience trauma, the more likely they are to develop dissociative disorders (see Putnam, 1997). (2) Severe traumas are associated with greater levels of dissociation in children (Putnam, 1997). (3) Increased levels of dissociation in adulthood are associated with self-reports of abuse and trauma in childhood (Bremner et al., 1994; Carlson and Rosser-Hogan, 1993; Chu and Dill, 1990; Marmer et al., 1994; Putnam and Trickett, 1997). (4) A history of exposure to extreme stress (such as childhood physical abuse) increases the risk for stress-related symptomology when re-exposed to traumatic stress later in life (see Bremner et al., 1994, for a review). (5) Prolonged exposure to trauma can engender more routine or automatized patterns of dissociative responding (DiTomasso and Routh, 1992). Perry's view is compatible with the idea that dissociation degrades information processing and impairs memory. The most commonly identi®ed memory-related symptom of pathological dissociation in children is amnesia for a traumatic event (Putnam, 1997). However, more general compartmentalization of thoughts, feelings, and aspects of the self, which is purported to occur in response to traumatic experiences, has been argued to create pervasive gaps in autobiographical memory, and episodes of `time loss' (Putnam, 1997). According to Putnam, discontinuities in memory associated with pathological dissociation render a person vulnerable to plausible suggestions that ®ll gaps in autobiographical memories. Putnam also notes that highly dissociative children (and adults) are prone to chronic feelings of depersonalization and derealization that lend a dream-like quality to experiences and autobiographical memories. Accordingly, dissociative propensities would be expected to engender dif®culties in source monitoring and uncertainty regarding recollections in general, and problems with reality monitoring of historical facts and life events in particular (see Putnam 1995, 1997 for reviews). In short, pathological dissociation would be expected to impair memory, rather than rendering it impervious to alteration over time. Diathesis-stress Some children may be more vulnerable than others to the adverse affects of extreme stress or repeated trauma. A diathesis-stress model of dissociation rests on the assumption that traumatic events alone are not necessarily suf®cient to elicit chronic dissociative reactions. Rather, traumatic events, in combination with other factors such as innate biological differences (Putnam, 1995), can constitute a diathesis that predisposes some children to develop dissociative disorders. In support of extreme fantasizing as one such diathesis, Rauschenberger and Lynn (1995, `Fantasy proneness, negative affect, and dissociation; unpublished manuscript,' 2000) found that a history of fantasy proneness (e.g., intense and often prolonged fantasizing) during childhood placed persons at risk for signi®cant dissociative pathology. Rauschenberger and Lynn (1995) found that 17% of the fantasy prone persons exhibited signs of serious psychopathology. In their second study (Rauschenberger and Lynn, unpublished manuscript, 2000), 33% of fantasy prone persons were also diagnosed as having a dissociative disorder (Dissociative Disorder Not Otherwise Speci®ed) based on an individually administered structured interview. Lynn and his colleagues speculate that if escapist imaginal activities become habitual ways of coping with a variety of stressful Copyright # 2001 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 15: S49±S73 (2001)
S54
M. L. Eisen and S. J. Lynn
circumstances, by way of stimulus generalization, the individual is likely to experience memory problems such as `missing time' and even more profound amnesic episodes. However, it is extremely dif®cult to predict which children who are highly imaginative daydreamers will develop pathological dissociative traits or symptoms over time. Attachment has been proposed as another diatheses for pathological dissociation. Barach (1991) hypothesized that severe disruptions in attachment stemming from an unresponsive primary caretaker elicit dissociative responses in some children. Liotti (1991) maintained that disorganized attachments lead to an increased vulnerability to develop dissociative disorders, a contention supported by research (Ogawa et al., 1997) indicating that disorganized patterns of attachment in infancy are related to dissociation scores in late adolescence. In a prospective study, Carlson (1998) also found that disorganized patterns of attachment in early childhood were related to scores on a measure of dissociation in adolescence. TRAIT VERSUS PERITRAUMATIC DISSOCIATION As noted earlier, dissociation is described not only as a pervasive personality trait, but also as a situationally bound response to an external event that disrupts cognitive processing. At a theoretical level, potential memory problems have been associated with a general propensity to dissociate. However, as alluded to above, memory problems have also been noted in conjunction with stress-related responding that can purportedly induce a dissociative `state' that compromises the encoding of the details of a traumatic event as it unfolds. Marmer et al. (1996) coined the term peritraumatic dissociation to describe a dissociative response that occurs during or in the aftermath of a traumatic or highly stressful event. Peritraumatic dissociation can encompass the following: (a) poor memory for a speci®c event, (b) vague or fragmented recollections, (c) complete amnesia for the experience, and (d) other signs or symptoms of dissociation including derealization, depersonalization, and confusion about the sequence of events. A growing body of research with combat veterans (Marmer et al., 1994), victims of natural disasters (Koopman et al., 1994), bystanders to human violence (Classen et al., 1998), and survivors of motor vehicle accidents (Harvey and Bryant, 1998) indicates that peritraumatic dissociation is a signi®cant risk factor for the development of posttraumatic symptomatology. Theoretically, peritraumatic dissociation can be evidenced in any person confronted with a suf®ciently stressful stimulus, regardless of the individual's preexisting cognitive or personality functioning. Further, this dissociative response may be entirely unique to a particular situation, resulting, for example, in an individual not attending to, or `blocking out' information delimited to a speci®c moment in time. Accordingly, a peritraumatic dissociative response may or may not affect other elements of information processing, and may or may not lead to further complications down-the-line. Although support for peritraumatic dissociation is limited by its primary reliance on correlational data based on retrospective reports of peoples' responses to traumatic experiences, we believe this construct is useful in terms of identifying a number of potential memory problems associated with individuals' discrete responses to highly stressful and/or traumatic events. In the remainder of the article we will argue that regardless of whether dissociation is conceptualized as a more or less discrete response to a traumatic event, or as a relatively stable trait, dissociation can result in memory problems and increased suggestibility. This Copyright # 2001 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 15: S49±S73 (2001)
Dissociation and suggestibility
S55
view differs from accounts of dissociation that imply that memories are somehow segregated yet imperishable and unaffected by ordinary processes of memory distortion and decay. The discussion that follows will focus on the link between dissociation, impaired memory, and increased suggestibility in children.
PERITRAUMATIC DISSOCIATION, MEMORY, AND SUGGESTIBILITY Studies with adults (Christiansen and Loftus, 1987, 1990; Christiansen and Nilsson, 1984; see Shobe and Kihlstrom, 1997) and children (Goodman et al., 1986; Peterson and Bell, 1996) indicate that recall of traumatic experiences is not generally compromised relative to non-traumatic events (Byrne et al., this issue), and that the central details of highly stressful experiences are remembered better than peripheral elements of the experience. However, we will examine four scenarios in which a dissociative response could potentially impair children's memory for a traumatic event. Dissociation prevents adequate encoding of the event If a child consciously or unconsciously engages in cognitive avoidance and distraction tactics (i.e. dissociation) that de¯ect attention from a traumatic event as it unfolds, then she would fail to adequately encode the details of the experience into declarative memory. If a memory was never encoded as a coherent narrative, then how could it be retrieved and accurately reported at a later date? It could be argued that it is impossible to create an accurate memory under such conditions. Van der Kolk et al. (1996) suggest another possibility: that dissociated imprints of memories are retrieved as sensory fragments that have little or no linguistic component and can be woven together to construct a coherent, accurate narrative memory of the traumatic experience. However, this possibility strains credulity in that the reconstruction of events from memory fragments, feelings, and intuition is considered by many experts to be one of the most common antecedents of false memory formation (see Loftus, 1997 for a review). Memory inaccuracy looms as an even greater possibility when the basic limits on children's ability to encode, organize, and retrieve, complex information apart from the context in which the event occurred are considered. It is true that even very young children (3±5 years of age) can provide accurate reports of novel life events after considerable delays of months, or even years (see Fivush, 1993 for a review). However, it is also the case that younger children have less conscious control over their memory (Kail, 1990) and often need assistance from adults to retrieve memories out of context and report them in a narrative form (see Fivush, 1998 for a review). Even healthy non-dissociative young children are highly dependent on adults' assistance to retrieve and jointly reconstruct decontextualized memories of routine nonstressful events (Fivush, 1993). When children are asked to recall the details of traumatic events, they might need considerable help from adults to just make sense of the experience, let alone retrieve and reconstruct the details of the event. If a child dissociates in the face of trauma and is limited to encoding decontextualized fragments and feelings, as van der Kolk et al. (1996) contend, then the process of jointly reconstructing a coherent narrative should be all the more problematic, rendering the child more vulnerable to the effects of misleading information and other suggestive in¯uences. Copyright # 2001 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 15: S49±S73 (2001)
S56
M. L. Eisen and S. J. Lynn
Normal forgetting should take place when a memory is isolated and never rehearsed Case studies (Hewitt, 1994; see Fivush, 1998, and Reviere, 1996 for reviews) reveal that even severe traumas experienced by very young children can be associated with the retention of clear memory fragments. However, if dissociation at the point of encoding involves the compartmentalization and subsequent avoidance of memories, then rehearsal of memories should be effectively prevented. It is widely accepted that some form of rehearsal is necessary for memories to be stored and maintained over time. Fivush and Shwarzmueller (1995) contend that preventing the rehearsal of memories related to childhood trauma though mechanisms such as dissociation or repression should lead to normal forgetting over long periods of time. Therefore, even when isolated elements of the event are encoded and retained in declarative memory, the details of these memory accounts should fade over time, like `ordinary' memories, rendering recall of an intact and accurate narrative memory increasingly less probable. If `dissociated' memories fade like ordinary memories, it would undermine the contention that dissociated memories can be somehow recoveredÐoften many years after the eventÐby techniques such as hypnosis (see Spiegel, 1995), unsullied by the usual vagaries of memory. Dissociation results in defensive distortions in memory In an effort to come to terms with ordinary non-traumatic events, it is generally accepted that children and adults tend to generate somewhat inaccurate accounts of experiences to better ®t their sense of self and view of the world (Hirt et al., 1999). As experienced events become increasingly incongruent with an individual's schemas of their self and the world they live in, the likelihood of consciously or unconsciously altering the interpretation of events increases. When events involve an unthinkable trauma, the likelihood that children may generate their own inaccurate accounts of what occurred in order to integrate the otherwise unfathomable information into their existing world view increases dramatically (Pynoos et al., 1995). Pynoos et al. (1997) found that when there is an actual violation of the physical or psychological integrity of the child, efforts are made to down-regulate physiological arousal or pain and engage in fantasies that allow the child to feel a physical distancing from what is happening and to even feel it is not happening to him or her, thereby decreasing a sense of active participation. Pynoos notes that (`imagined or intended interventions become inextricably part of the memory network associated with the experience' (p. 278; Pynoos, 1997). Theoretically, peritraumatic dissociative responses that limit what factual information is encoded as the event is experienced should contribute greatly to the malleability of these memory reports. Pynoos's research (see Pynoos, 1997) illustrates that the unfolding of children's memories of traumatic experiences (e.g., sniper attack on school playground) traced within hours, days, weeks, and months after the event represent dramatically altered versions of the experience. Post-trauma elaboration Christiansen (1992a,b) notes that the compelling nature of highly stressful experiences motivates continued mental review of events in order to make sense of what was experienced. Christiansen refers to this operation as post stimulus elaboration, and maintains that this process is fundamental to the consolidation of memories of stressful Copyright # 2001 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 15: S49±S73 (2001)
Dissociation and suggestibility
S57
events. Expanding on Christensen's theory, Eisen and Goodman (1998) introduced the concept of post-trauma elaboration, and suggest that there may be a critical time immediately after the trauma when the memory has yet to be consolidated and the child is most suggestible and vulnerable to the effects of misinformation. Eisen and Goodman assert that a peritraumatic dissociative response that signi®cantly limits what information is encoded, should increase the child's level of uncertainly about what did and did not occur, and therefore heighten their dependency on adult assistance to make sense of what transpired. Eisen and Goodman (1998) propose several possible scenarios as to how post-trauma elaboration might lead to accurate or inaccurate memories for the event. By far, the most positive scenario would be that the child is able to discuss the event with a caring adult and jointly construct a relatively accurate narrative memory compatible with their existing schemas of the world. However, Eisen and Goodman note that the assistance of a caring adult does not guarantee that accurate memory construction will follow. In an effort to buffer the effects of an otherwise horri®c explanation of a traumatic event, a well-meaning adult might try to use the child's confusion and uncertainty to protect them by helping him or her to create a less emotionally toxic yet fundamentally inaccurate interpretation of what occurred. Alternatively, a manipulative adult might take advantage of the child's confusion and deliberately mislead the child in order to cover up improper conduct such as abuse, or to turn the child against other adults, as can occur in a custody dispute. Finally, a misguided or incompetent forensic interviewer might suggest plausible explanations for what happened and, together with the child, jointly construct an inaccurate narrative account that con®rms the interviewer's preconceptions. In summary, peritraumatic dissociative responses to extreme stressors should logically impair a child's ability to recall the event accurately and theoretically make her more vulnerable to suggestive in¯uences when recalling the experience. For ethical reasons, it is impossible to evaluate hypotheses regarding peritraumatic dissociation and suggestibility in the laboratory. However, it is possible to examine suggestibility and dissociation with trait measures of dissociativity, in the laboratory. In the following section we describe how the construct of suggestibility is de®ned and measured in applied memory research and examine the relationship between dissociation and suggestibility in the context of a variety of different research paradigms. SUGGESTIBILITY The term `suggestibility' is applied to an array of phenomena that range from hypnotic responsivity to simple gullibility (see Eysenck and Fureanaux, 1945; Roediger, 1996; Schooler and Loftus, 1993 for reviews). In applied memory research, the term suggestibility is used as an index of susceptibility to misleading information (Schooler and Loftus, 1993) or, more generally, the tendency to make errors in determining the source of a recollective experience (i.e. confusing what is merely imagined with an actual historical occurrence). Johnson et al. (1993) distinguished between internally generated sources of recollective experiences (e.g. imagination, fantasies, and confabulation), and externally generated sources of such experiences (e.g. actual perceived events, others' recollections, newspaper stories). Johnson and Raye (1981) identi®ed cues that help individuals to infer the likely source of a memory. For example, compared with memories for imagined events, Copyright # 2001 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 15: S49±S73 (2001)
S58
M. L. Eisen and S. J. Lynn
memories for perceived events include a relative abundance of perceptual information (e.g. sound, color), contextual information (e.g. spatial, temporal), semantic detail, and affective information (e.g. emotional reactions). Accordingly, a memory with a great deal of visual and spatial detail and contextual information would most often be judged as something that was actually perceived (Johnson et al., 1989). By and large, this decision strategy works quite well, and leads to accurate source monitoring in many situations. However, in situations in which: (a) an imagined event is perceived as very clear and vivid, and (b) few details of a perceived event can be recalled (e.g. due to distraction or stress), source monitoring problems and related memory errors are especially likely to occur (Hirt et al., 1999). Problems in source monitoring can also lead to dif®culties in distinguishing between events that actually occurred and events that were merely suggested by misinformation provided after an event. For example, participants continue to report misinformation from a post-event narrative even after they are told that the information is false (Lindsay, 1990). In some cases, subjects actually claim to remember in considerable detail the presentation of items in the original event, when the items were in fact only mentioned in the post-event narrative (Payne et al., 1996). Because highly dissociative individuals are thought to be more prone to making sourcemonitoring errors in everyday life, the report of dissociative experiences would be expected to be linked to increased suggestibility and risk of false memory production. Theoretically, this effect should be greatest for individuals who exhibit pathological dissociation. In the next section, we review the relationship between trait measures of dissociation and recall errors that arise in the context of a number of different paradigms that include the direct provision of misinformation and/or repeated imagining of counterfactual events. Loftus's childhood misinformation paradigm Loftus and her colleagues have devised a false childhood memory paradigm (originally named the memory implantation paradigm) that provided a demonstration proof that false memories for a never-experienced childhood event can be created (Loftus, 1993; Loftus and Pickrell, 1995). In the original study, Loftus asked the older brother of a 14-year-old boy to suggest a false memory about an event (being lost in a shopping mall) that was alleged to have occurred when the younger boy was 5 years old. After listening to the older brother's story, the boy was asked to write about his memory of the suggested false event (and of three other true events from his childhood) once every day for 5 days. By the end of this process, Loftus reported that the boy described the false event in detail and with great con®dence. Variations of this procedure have been used successfully by other investigators (Hyman and Billings, 1998; Hyman et al., 1995; Hyman and Pentland, 1996; Qin, 1999). Hyman and Billings (1998) examined the relationship between dissociation, as measured by the DES, and the acceptance of suggested early autobiographical memories using the Loftus (1993) memory implantation procedure. Hyman and Billings reported a moderately strong relationship between DES scores and the acceptance of false memories (r 0:48). Wilkinson and Hyman (1998) reanalyzed Hyman and Billings' data to examine the relationship between false memory creation and several different aspects of DES scoring. These investigators found that the acceptance of false memories was moderately related to the items that make up the depersonalization (r 0:37) and memory Copyright # 2001 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 15: S49±S73 (2001)
Dissociation and suggestibility
S59
failure (r 0:41) factors of the DES. Surprisingly, Wilkinson and Hyman did not examine the association between the acceptance of false memories and pathological dissociation taxon scores as measured by the DES-T subscale of the DES. In a similar study, using a variation of the same research paradigm, Qin (1999) reported a statistically signi®cant relationship between the acceptance of suggested memories and both overall scoring on the DES and DES-T subscale scores (r's 0.23). Interestingly, Qin found that participants who quali®ed for pathological dissociation taxon membership, based on DES-T scores (Waller and Ross, 1997), did not report greater con®dence or perceived clarity associated with false memories than individuals who were not taxon members. However, taxon members were more likely to have subjective experiences of false memories and provided more units of information in association with their false memories than did non-taxon members. Imagination in¯ation An interesting variation of the childhood misinformation paradigm involves `imagination in¯ation' (Garry et al., 1996). Studies in imagination in¯ation show that providing instructions to participants to imagine a childhood event that was never experienced in `real life' can engender an increase in con®dence that the event actually occurred (Garry et al., 1996; Heaps and Nash, 2000; Paddock et al., 1998, Study 1; but see Clancy et al., 1999; Paddock et al., 1998, Study 2). Using a version of the imagination in¯ation paradigm, Paddock et al. (1998) reported a positive relationship between scoring on the DES and enhanced certainty regarding imagined events that did not occur in reality. More speci®cally, the authors reported correlations (range 0:20 to 0.35) between enhanced con®dence for imagined nonevents and the 9 individual items that comprise the dissociability factor of the DES. Based on these data, Paddock et al. asserted that high DES scorers seemed to be more prone to interpersonal in¯uence when suggestions were made to imagine counterfactual events as if they were real. Unfortunately, Paddock and his colleagues did not examine DES-T scores. The DRM paradigm Roediger and McDermott (1995) provided a compelling demonstration of false recognition effects in a list-learning task (Deese, 1959). The paradigm they devised has inspired numerous studies and has come to be widely known as the Deese/Roediger±McDermott paradigm (DRM). In the DRM paradigm, participants are presented (auditorily) with a series of lists of thematically related words such a bed, rest, wake, doze, dream, pillow, and so forth, which are all thematically related to non-presented words such as sleep. They then are administered an oral recognition test consisting of the words actually presented in the experiment, `critical' non-presented words that are thematically related to the presented words (e.g. sleep), and a series of non-presented, thematically related, words. Using this paradigm, many participants (approximately 70%, on average) falsely recognize thematically related non-presented words with a high degree of con®dence. Since the initial research was conducted, numerous investigators have successfully replicated Roediger and McDermott's seminal ®ndings (e.g. Mather et al., 1997; McDermott, 1996; Norman and Schacter, 1997; Payne et al., 1996; Reed, 1996; Robinson and Roediger, 1997; Seamon et al., 1998; Tussing and Greene, 1997; Winograd et al., Copyright # 2001 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 15: S49±S73 (2001)
S60
M. L. Eisen and S. J. Lynn
1998). A number of studies have examined the relationship between dissociation and false recognition errors on the DRM task. Winograd et al. (1998) reported that DES scores were positively related to remember judgments for non-presented, thematically related words on the DRM task (r 0:34), although the authors failed to examine DES-T scores in this context. However, contrary to the ®ndings of Winograd et al.; Eisen et al. (presentation at the 3rd biennial meeting of the Society of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, Boulder, CO, 1999) and Qin (1999) failed to ®nd evidence for a relationship between dissociation, as measured by any key index of the DES (including DES-T scores), and false recognition errors on the DRM task (see Table 1). Misleading questions The relationship between dissociation and memory errors has been examined in the context of studies that expose participants to biased and/or suggestive questioning techniques. This methodology has been used more frequently in children's than in adults' eyewitness suggestibility research because children's tendency to accede to suggestive questions is arguably the the most common antecedent of false allegations of abuse (Ceci and Bruck, 1993). Schooler and Loftus (1993) proposed that errors in response to misleading questions should be related to poor memory and a lack of attention to detail (see also Gudjonsson and Clarke, 1986). The authors contended that individuals who have dif®culty remembering the details of an event they witnessed would be less likely to notice that post-event information contained in a misleading question is inconsistent or discrepant with the event they witnessed than individuals who have good initial recall of the event. Putnam (1997) has observed that because dissociative individuals exhibit memory problems and lack con®dence in their recollections, they are vulnerable to the effects of misinformation. Putnam's hypothesis is consistent with Gudjonsson and Clark's (1986) theory of interrogative suggestibility, which proposes that individuals who are less con®dent in their memories are more likely to acquiesce to misleading questions. According to Putnam, discontinuities in memory associated with pathological dissociation render the individual vulnerable to plausible suggestions that ®ll the gaps in autobiographical memory. We were able to locate only one published study done with children that examined relations between measures of dissociation and suggestibility as assessed by errors on misleading questions (Eisen et al., 1998). In this study, children were tested in the context of an inpatient hospitalization designed to assess allegations of maltreatment. As part of the medical assessment, each child received an ano-genital examination to assess overt signs and symptoms of physical and/or sexual abuse. Three to ®ve days later, the children were questioned about the exam using an interview protocol that included both factual and misleading questions related to the event. In addition, the children received a battery of tests that included two measures of dissociation. All children over the age of 5 were administered the Children's Perceptual Alteration Scale (CPAS: Evers-Szostak and Sanders, 1992), while all children over 11 years of age were administered the Adolescent-Dissociative Experiences Scale (A-DES: Armstrong et al., 1997). In addition, the parents and caretakers of all children who were available were administered the Child Dissociative Checklist (CDC; Putnam et al., 1993). Eisen and his colleagues examined several different indices of children's event memory and suggestibility including, correct responses and errors (errors of commission and Copyright # 2001 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 15: S49±S73 (2001)
Copyright # 2001 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Depersonalization CDC
Absorption DES
Wilkinson and Hyman (1998) 0.05
Eisen et al. (1999) 0.13 Platt et al. (1998) 0.03 Winograd et al. (1998) 0.34* Eisen et al. (1999) 0.11 Qin (1999) 0.01 Wilkinson and Hyman (1998) 0.03
Wilkinson and Hyman (1998) 0.04 Qin (1999) 0.03
DRM Intrusions: remember judgements for critical lures
Wilkinson and Hyman (1998) 0.37* Eisen et al. (1999a,b) 0.03y
Eisen and Carlson (1998) 0.24* Eisen et al. (in press) 0.18*
Eisen and Carlson (1998) 0.23* Eisen et al. (in press) 0.13 Wolfradt and Meyer (1998) 0.53*** Merckelbach et al. (2000) 0.29**
Errors on misleading questions
Wilkinson and Hyman (1998) 0.05
Wilkinson and Hyman (1998) 0.41*
DES Factor:
Heaps and Nash (2000) 0.34** Paddock et al. (1998) 0.17 Paddock et al. (1998) 0.32**
Imagination in¯ation: increased con®dence in non-events
Wilkinson and Hyman (1998) 0.41*
Qin (1999) 0.23*
DES-T
Memory Failure DES
Hyman and Billings (1998) 0.48** Qin (1999) 0.23*
DES
False memory acceptance
Continues
Classic misinformation paradigm
Table 1. Correlations between various indices of suggestibility and measures of dissociation, visualization, absorption and fantasy proneness
Dissociation and suggestibility S61
Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 15: S49±S73 (2001)
Copyright # 2001 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Hyman and Billings (1998) 0.36*
Hyman and Billings (1998) 0.23
False memory acceptance
< 0.05. < 0.01. p < 0.001. udy done with children.Q21
ICMI VVQ
CEQ
IDQ
CIS
VMIQ
VVIQ
TAS
CPAS
A-DES
Table 1. Continued
Heaps and Nash (2000) 0.06 Paddock et al. (1999) 0.16
Paddock et al. (1999) 0.16
Imagination in¯ation: increased con®dence in non-events
Merckelbach et al. (2000) 0.07
Eisen et al. (1999a,b) 0.57y Eisen et al. (1999a,b) 0.09y Eisen and Carlson (1998) 0.14 Wolfradt and Meyer (1998) 0.54*** Heaps and Nash (2000) 0.27
Errors on misleading questions
Platt et al. (1998) 0.04
Wingrad et al. (1998) 0.04 Wilkinson and Hyman (1998) 0.21*
Winograd et al. (1998) 0.14
Platt et al. (1998) 0.13
DRM Intrusions: remember judgements for critical lures
Tousignant (1984) 0.23*
Tousignant (1984) 0.22
Classic misinformation paradigm
S62 M. L. Eisen and S. J. Lynn
Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 15: S49±S73 (2001)
Dissociation and suggestibility
S63
omission) on misleading and speci®c questions related to both central and peripheral details of the medical exam. These investigators reported that none of the dissociation measures employed were consistently related to the various indices of memory and suggestibility examined. More recently, Eisen Goodman, Qin, and Davis (1999a,b) replicated this study with a substantially larger sample of children (N > 400), improved the procedures used to assess children's memory and suggestibility, and included an additional measure of dissociation (the Dissociation Scale from the Trauma Symptom Checklist for Children-TSC-C). In this effort, the investigators examined children's event memory and performance on misleading questions related to three different events (a medical exam, a game played with a research assistant, and a meeting with a psychologist). Preliminarily analyses of these unpublished data again indicated that the measures of dissociation used (CDC, A-DES, CPAS, TSC-C), were not were not consistently related to the various indices of memory and suggestibility examined across the three events. Eisen and Carlson (1998) and Eisen et al. (in press) examined the relationship between dissociation and errors in response to misleading information in college students. In each of these studies, students took part in a staged event and then returned one week later to be administered a structured interview that included both highly suggestive, misleading questions and factual, non-suggestive questions about the event. In each study, pathological dissociation, as measured by scoring on the DES-T sub-scale of the DES, was signi®cantly related to errors in response to misleading questions. Further, in both studies, errors in response to misleading questions were somewhat more strongly related to DES-T scoring than overall DES scores. It is important to note that in both studies neither overall DES scores nor DES-T sub-scale scores were related to errors on the speci®c, nonsuggestive questions designed to assess event memory (see Table 1). Misleading questions using the GSS According to Gudjonsson interrogative suggestibility involves `the tendency of an individual's account of events to be altered by misleading information and inter-personal pressure within an interview' (p. 155; Singh and Gudjonsson, 1992). Gudjonsson and his colleagues developed the Gudjonsson Scale of Interrogative Suggestibility (GSS; Gudjonsson, 1984) to predict which individuals will be susceptible to highly suggestive questioning during a police interrogation (e.g., Gudjonsson 1984; Gudjonsson and Clarke, 1986). The GSS involves three phases. First, the participants read a story. Next, they are presented with 20 questions, 15 of which are highly suggestive misleading questions related to the story. Errors on these questions make up the Yield 1 scale for the GSS. Finally, the participants are given negative feedback about their performance and asked to answer the questions again. Changes in responses following the negative feedback comprise the shift scale of the GSS. Two studies (Merckelbach et al., 2000; Wolfradt and Meyer, 1998) obtained evidence for a relationship between DES scores and suggestibility as measured by the GSS. Wolfradt and Meyer (1998) reported a surprisingly large correlation between the DES and GSS scores (r 0.53), whereas Merckelbach et al. (2000) reported a more modest correlation between the two measures (r 0.29). The magnitude of the latter correlation was more in line with the more modest effect reported by Eisen and his colleagues (Eisen and Carlson, 1998; Eisen et al., in press). Copyright # 2001 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 15: S49±S73 (2001)
S64
M. L. Eisen and S. J. Lynn
In summary, there appears to be a fairly consistent small to moderate relationship between dissociation (as measured by the DES) and recall errors in studies that employ a variety of different paradigms. The most notable exception to this pattern of ®ndings is the mixed results obtained in studies that employ the DRM paradigm. The disparity in research outcomes may be due to the fact that false recognition on the DRM task is more fundamentally related to encoding errors (see Roediger et al., 1998 for a review), rather than source monitoring problems, or errors due to the explicit provision of misinformation. Notably, very few studies have been done with children, and it is unclear how the adult ®ndings will generalize to younger age groups. Non-pathological dissociation and memory errors Measures of vividness of visualization have been found to be related to the production of memory errors across a variety of suggestibility paradigms (Hyman and Billings, 1998; Dobson and Markham, 1993; Tousignant, 1984; Qin, 1999; Winograd et al., 1998). However, this relationship is not consistently evident across all measures of visual imagery, and also appears to vary depending on the suggestibility paradigm employed (see Table 1). For example, among the studies we reviewed, the most commonly used measure of visualization capacity was the Vividness of Visual Imagery Questionnaire (VVIQ: Marks, 1973). Whereas the VVIQ was found to be related to false memory formation (Hyman and Billings, 1998) and errors in response to misleading questions (Heaps and Nash, 2000), the VVIQ was not signi®cantly correlated with performance on an imagination in¯ation task (Heaps and Nash, 2000) or with remember judgments for critical lures on the DRM task (Winograd et al., 1998). Absorption (i.e. the tendency to become involved or absorbed in everyday\activities) has also been found to be correlated with measures of visual imagery (Lynn and Rhue, 1988). Platt et al. (1998) examined the relationship between absorption and memory accuracy in relation to hearing about the verdict in the OJ. Simpson trail. Platt and his colleagues reported that scores on the TAS were inversely related to the accuracy of the participants' memories for this event. Hyman and Billings (1998) also reported a small correlation between scoring on the TAS and false memory creation that did not reach statistical signi®cance (see Table 1). Non-pathological dissociation and responses to misleading information We could ®nd only three studies that investigated the relationship between measures of non-pathological dissociation and responses to misleading questions. Eisen and Carlson (1998) failed to ®nd a relationship between absorption as measured by the TAS and errors in response to misleading questions. Similarly, Merkelbach et al. (2000) failed to obtain evidence for an association between interrogative suggestibility and fantasy proneness as measured by the Creative Experiences Questionnaire (CEQ: Merckelbach et al., 1998). However, in the single exception to this pattern of null ®ndings, Wolfradt and Meyer (1998) found that the TAS was related to interrogative suggestibility (r = 0.54). In summary, vividness of visualization has been found to be related to the production of memory errors in the laboratory, but the ®ndings in this area are not entirely consistent across studies. Unfortunately, no studies have examined the relationship between visualization ability and errors in response to misleading questions. Studies that have examined the relationship between absorption and various types of suggestibility have also Copyright # 2001 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 15: S49±S73 (2001)
Dissociation and suggestibility
S65
failed to yield consistent results, and evidence linking these constructs is weak. It is important to note that we could ®nd no studies done with children that have assessed the association between non-pathological dissociation and indices of memory or suggestibility. CONCLUSIONS The research on trauma, memory, and dissociation suffers not so much from a dearth of theory or explanatory models, as much as a lack of well-controlled research. The hypothesized ways that memory is affected by neurobiological mechanisms, exposure to a traumatic event, and the variables that contribute to resilience versus vulnerability are, to varying degrees, speculative. Further, although the ®ndings reviewed imply that there is link between the experience of a traumatic event, dissociation, and memory problems, the strength and interpretability of the evidence can be criticized on the following grounds: (1) The ®ndings that support an association between early trauma and later dissociative psychopathology are largely correlational in nature. Accordingly, it is not at all clear whether trauma precedes dissociative symptomatology or whether pre-existing `dissociative' coping strategies (e.g. cognitive avoidance) and tendencies are exacerbated by the experience of trauma. (2) A similar problem in identifying a cause±effect relationship is evident with respect to the correlational relationship between dissociation and memory. Poor memory functioning may not necessarily be a symptom of dissociation, but rather a vulnerability factor related to the development of trauma-related pathology. For example, individuals who have innate memory de®cits may also exhibit dif®culty processing complex information associated with traumatic events and, therefore, be more vulnerable to peritraumatic dissociation and more serious long-standing dissociative symptoms. Clearly, prospective studies are needed to clarify these issues. (3) Prospective studies are also needed in order to address the fact that the data that support an association between childhood abuse, memory problems, and later psychopathology are largely based on the retrospective reports of adults, and therefore are subject to memory and report biases (Lilienfeld et al., 1999). Studies that vary the demand characteristics and procedures used to elicit memories and dissociative experiences, and that examine the in¯uence of agreeableness, acquiescence, and related response sets and personality variables would represent worthwhile contributions to the literature. (4) De®nitions of dissociation in the literature are often ambiguous and do not specify whether dissociation is: (a) regarded as pathological, (b) a trait-like propensity, or the response to a single traumatic event or more chronic exposure to highly stressful events, and (c) regarded as nothing more than cognitive avoidance or dif®culties encoding traumatic events. (5) In particular, the construct of peritraumatic dissociation could bene®t from additional elaboration and speci®cation, particularly regarding the nature of the purported dissociative state that is elicited by trauma. It is imperative that future studies provide clear operational de®nitions of dissociation so that it can be determined whether there are different antecedents and consequences of diverse manifestations of dissociation including amnesia, feelings of unreality, and depersonalization. Copyright # 2001 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 15: S49±S73 (2001)
S66
M. L. Eisen and S. J. Lynn
(6) In general, very little is known about the way traumatic experiences are processed. The assumption that avoidance of trauma-related cues at the time of a traumatic event constitutes a defensive response has not been subjected to rigorous empirical test. Additionally, much needs to be learned regarding the antecedents of problems in encoding versus retrieval of trauma-related information, and under what circumstances gist versus peripheral memories are subject to memory distortions. More attention could be usefully devoted to explicating: (a) the motivational and reinforcing aspects of the avoidance of traumatic stimuli and cues (i.e dissociation), (b) the role played by attributions and interpretations of traumatic events in mediating memory de®cits and buffering stress-related sequelae, and (c) the role of social support or a positive family environment in mitigating dissociative reactions. (7) Perhaps the most glaring de®ciency in the literature is the scant number of studies of actual children. It is not altogether clear that research on adults will generalize to children insofar as prominent developmental differences demarcate these groups. As noted above, only two studies have addressed dissociation and false memories in children, and both investigations used memory paradigms that were focused on children's responsiveness to misleading questions. No studies have examined the relationship between dissociation in the context of (a) methodologies likely to evoke source monitoring errors or (b) studies that have examined the relationship between suggestibility effects and non-pathological forms of dissociation. (8) Major obstacles must be overcome before a seamless integration of the child and adult literatures is possible. First, the distinction between pathological and non-pathological dissociation is not as well delineated in children and adolescents as in adult populations (Hornstein and Putnam, 1992). For example, the high levels of imaginative involvements and excessive daydreaming that might resemble trance experiences in adults are more normative in young children (Putnam, 1997). Putnam (1994) also maintains that normal children often display dissociative behaviors such as imaginary companionship that would be considered abnormal in adult populations. Furthermore, certain relatively well-adjusted children engage in intense and prolonged daydreaming, elaborate complex fantasy worlds, and invent imaginary characters with distinct personality attributes. As a result, it can be extremely dif®cult to differentiate highly imaginative children who engage in adaptive daydreams and fantasy from children who use fantasy excessively as a habitual coping mechanism that places them at risk for the development of dissociative disorders. Future prospective studies should be conducted to identify variables associated with predicting which highly imaginative children will develop pathological dissociative traits over time. Second, studies that incorporate reliable and valid measures of key constructs of nonpathological as well as more pathological dissociation are needed. A major obstacle faced by researchers working with children is that measures of the construct of dissociation do not correlate consistently with one another. For example, Eisen et al. (1999a,b), reported that the A-DES and the CPAS were positively correlated (r 0.66), but the CDC shared very little variance with either the A-DES (r 0.28) or the CPAS (r 0.00). The lack of consistency in the assessment of dissociation across instruments may re¯ect a lack of agreement about how dissociation is manifested and should be measured in children. For example, the authors of the CPAS designed their scale to address the potential relationship between dissociation and eating disorders, whereas the authors of Copyright # 2001 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 15: S49±S73 (2001)
Dissociation and suggestibility
S67
the A-DES did not take this relationship into consideration. In contrast to both of these instruments, the CDC is a parent report behaviour checklist. Unfortunately, there are few available indices of non-pathological dissociation (e.g. imaginative involvement, absorption, visual imagery) in children, and those measures that are available are not well established. This creates a major obstacle for researchers working in this area. Not surprisingly, we could not ®nd any studies that examined nonpathological dissociation and resistance to misleading information in children. Although children (and adults) with dissociative disorders clearly show disruptions in memory (Putnam, 1995), no empirical studies of suggestibility have focused on pathological dissociation per se, apart from the more normative aspects of this construct. Surprisingly, most of the published studies that have examined dissociation and suggestibility make no mention of data related to pathological dissociation. For example, although most adult studies in this area have used the DES to assess dissociation, the (easily derivable) DES-T scores are rarely reported. This omission is notable, if not remarkable, considering the widely accepted distinction between pathological and non-pathological dissociation. Third, measures that relate speci®cally to theory-driven hypotheses would be welcome. For instance, Freyd (1996) has proposed that sexual abuse by a primary caregiver represents a special type of betrayal that engenders coping to block awareness of the abuse. Freyd avers that dissociation of the memories of the abuse is necessary in order to maintain normal attachment and survive in the family unit. Although betrayal is posited to lead to memory loss, Freyd proposes that other types of trauma, unrelated to betrayal, should improve memory for the traumatic experience (e.g. extra-familial sexual abuse or other traumatic experiences). Empirical data have not, as yet, substantiated the hypothesis that memories for interfamilial sexual abuse experiences are more poorly recalled than extra-familial sexual abuse or other forms of abuse or childhood trauma. Scienti®c studies based on reliable and valid measures are needed to examine the interrelationships among betrayal, trauma, attachment and children's memory and suggestibility. In research on attachment and betrayal it will also be important to assess the stress-buffering effects of social support as well as the quality of the family environment independent of abuse. Whereas the extant literature is de®cient in certain respects, there are also indications that dissociation at the time of a trauma, and when expressed as a more enduring `trait', is likely to be associated with memory de®cits and distortions. Relatedly, there is reason to believe that memories that are purportedly `dissociated' and are later `recovered', in psychotherapy or elsewhere, are mutable and subject to mundane processes of memory reconstruction. In fact, the limited available evidence implies that dissociative individuals are more prone to accepting misinformation than their less dissociative counterparts, although precisely when and under what circumstances this occurs has not, as yet, been pinpointed. Over the past hundred years, the study of dissociation has been characterized by vast ¯uctuations in interest from the scienti®c community, ranging from outright neglect to guarded fascination (see Lynn and Rhue, 1994). Today, dissociative phenomena rank among the most perplexing puzzles on the scienti®c frontier. It is our hope that cognitive scientists will be at the vanguard of efforts to more ®rmly establish the study of dissociation as a legitimate area of inquiry, and that this effort will illuminate fundamental mechanisms of memory, the manifold immediate reactions and long-term adaptations to traumatic events, iatrogenic in¯uences in psychotherapy, and vulnerability to misinformation in adult and child populations. Copyright # 2001 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 15: S49±S73 (2001)
S68
M. L. Eisen and S. J. Lynn REFERENCES
American Psychiatric Association. 1994. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th edn). APA: Washington, DC. Armstrong JG, Putnam FW, Carlson EB, Libero DZ, Smith SR. 1997. Development and validation of a measure of adolescent dissociation: The Adolescent Dissociative Experiences Scale. Journal of Nervous and Mental Diseases 185(7): 491±497. Barach PM. 1991. Multiple personality disorder as an attachment disorder. Dissociation 4: 117±123. Bernstein EM, Putnam FW. 1986. Development, reliability, and validity of a dissociation scale. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 174: 727±735. Bremner JD, Davis M, Southwick SM, Krystal JH, Charney DS. 1994. Neurobiology of posttraumatic stress disorder. In Post Traumatic Stress Disorder: A Clinical Review, Pynoos RS et al. (eds). Sidran Press: Lutherville, MD; 43±64. Bremner JD, Southwick S, Brett E, Fontana A, Rosenbeck R, Charney D. 1992. Dissociation and posttraumatic stress disorder in Vietnam combat veterans. American Journal of Psychiatry 149: 328±332. Briere JN. 1992. Child Abuse Trauma: Theory and Treatment of the Lasting Effects. Sage: Newbury Park, CA. Briere J. 1995. Trauma Symptom Inventory Professional Manual. Psychological Assessment Resources: Odessa, FLA. Briere J, Runtz M. 1988. Symptomatology associated with childhood sexual victimization in a nonclinical adult sample. Child Abuse and Neglect 12: 51±59. Brown D, Sche¯in AW, Hammond DC. 1998. Memory, Trauma Treatment, and the Law. Norton: New York. Cardena E. 1994. The domain of dissociation. In Dissociation: Clinical and Theoretical Perspectives, Lynn SJ et al. (eds). Guilford Press: New York; 15±31. Carlson EA. 1998. A prospective longitudinal study of attachment disorganization/disorientation. Child Development 69(4): 1107±1128. Carlson EB, Rosser-Hogan R. 1993. Trauma experiences, posttraumatic stress, dissociation and depression in Cambodian refugees. American Journal of Psychiatry 148: 1548±1551. Carlson EB. 1997. A conceptual framework for the long-term psychological effects of traumatic childhood abuse. Child Maltreatment 2(3): 272±295. Ceci SJ, Bruck M. 1993. The suggestibility of the child witness: A historical review and synthesis. Psychological Bulletin 113(3): 403±439. Christianson SA. 1992a. Remembering emotional events: Potential mechanisms. In Handbook of Emotion and Memory: Research and Theory, Christianson SA (ed.). Erlbaum: Hillsdale, NJ; 307± 340. Christianson SA. 1992b. Emotional stress and eyewitness memory: A critical review. Psychological Bulletin 112: 284±309. Christianson SA, Loftus EF. 1987. Memory for traumatic events. Applied Cognitive Psychology 1: 225±239. Christianson SA, Loftus EF. 1990. Some characteristics of people's traumatic memories. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 28: 195±198. Christianson SA, Nilsson LG. 1984. Functional amnesia as induced by a psychological trauma. Memory and Cognition 12: 142±155. Chu JA, Dill DL. 1990. Dissociative symptoms in relation to childhood physical and sexual abuse. American Journal of Psychiatry 147: 887±892. Clancy SA, McNally RJ, Schacter DL. 1999. Effects of guided imagery on memory distortion in women reporting recovered memories childhood sexual abuse. Journal of Traumatic Stress 12(4): 559±569. Classen C, Koopman C, Hales R, Spiegel D. 1998. Acute stress disorder as a predictor of posttraumatic stress symptoms. American Journal of Psychiatry 155: 620±624. Coons PM, Milstein V. 1986. Psychosexual disturbances in multiple personality: Characteristics, etiology, and treatment. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 47: 106±110. Deese J. 1959. In¯uence of inter-item associative strength upon immediate free recall. Psychological Reports 5: 305±312.
Copyright # 2001 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 15: S49±S73 (2001)
Dissociation and suggestibility
S69
DiTomasso MJ, Routh DK. 1994. Recall of abuse in childhood and three measures of dissociation. Child Abuse and Neglect 17(4): 447±485. Dobson M, Markham R. 1993. Imagery ability and source monitoring: Implications for eyewitness memory. British Journal of Psychology 32: 111±118. Edwards V, Fivush R. 1998. Autobiographical memory disruptions in adults survivors of childhood sexual abuse. In Childhood Trauma & Memory, Eisen M, Fivush R (Chairs). Symposium presented at the Biennial meeting of the American Psychology Law Society, Redondo Beach, CA. Eisen M, Carlson EB. 1998. Individual Differences in suggestibility: Examining the in¯uence of dissociation, absorption, and a history of childhood abuse. Applied Cognitive Psychology 12: 47±61. Eisen ML, Goodman GS. 1998. Trauma, memory, and suggestibility in children. Development and Psychopathology 10: 717±738. Eisen ML, Goodman GS, Qin J, Davis SL. 1998. Memory and suggestibility in maltreated children: New research relevant to evaluating allegations of abuse. In Truth in Memory, Lynn SJ, McKonkey KM (eds). Guilford Press: New York; 163±189. Eisen ML, Goodman GS, Qin J, Davis SL. 1999a. Final Report: The Impact of Dissociation, Trauma, and Stress Arousal on the Memory and Suggestibility of Abused and Neglected Children. National Center on Child Abuse and Neglect Clearinghouse: Washington, DC. Eisen ML, Goodman GS, Qin J, Davis SL. 1999b. Individual differences in maltreated children's memory and suggestibility. In Trauma and Memory, Williams L (ed.). Sage: London; 163±189. Eisen ML, Morgan D, Mickes L. in press. Individual differences in eyewitness memory and suggestibility: Examining relations between dissociation, acquiescence and resistance to misleading questions. Personality and Individual Differences. Evers-Szostak M, Sanders S. 1992. The Children's Perceptual Alteration Scale (CPAS): A measure of children's dissociation. Dissociation: Progress in the Dissociative Disorders 5(2): 91±97. Eysenck HJ, Furneaux WD. 1945. Primary and secondary suggestibility: An experimental and statistical study. Journal of Experimental Psychology 35: 485±503. Fivush R. 1993. Developmental perspectives on autobiographical recall. In Child Victims, Child Witnesses, Goodman GS et al. (eds). Guilford Press: New York; 1±24. Fivush R. 1998. Children's recollections of traumatic and nontraumatic events. Development and Psychopathology 10(4): 699±716. Fivush R, Schwarzmueller A. 1995. Say it once again: Effects of repeated questions on children's event recall. Journal of Traumatic Stress 8: 555±580. Freyd J. 1996. Betrayal Trauma: The logic of Forgetting Childhood Abuse. Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA. Garry M, Loftus EF. 1994. Pseudomemories without hypnosis. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hyponsis Special Issue: Hyponsis and Delayed Recall: I 42(4): 363±378. Garry M, Manning CG, Loftus EF, Sherman SJ. 1996. Imagination in¯ation: Imagining a childhood event in¯ates con®dence that it occurred. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 3: 208±214. Goodman GS, Hepps D, Reed RS. 1986. The child victim's testimony. In New Issues for Child Advocates, Haralambie A (ed.). Arizona Council of Attorneys for Children: Phoenix, AZ. Gudjonsson GH. 1984. A new Scale of Interrogative Suggestibility. Personality and Individual Differences 5(3): 303±314. Gudjonsson GH, Clark R. 1986. Suggestibility in police interrogation: A social psychological model. Social Behavior 1: 83±104. Harvey AG, Bryant RA. 1998. The relationship between acute stress disorder and posttraumatic stress disorder: A prospective evaluation of motor vehicle accident survivors. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 66: 507±512. Heaps C, Nash MR. 2000. Individual differences in imagination in¯ation. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review. Hewitt SK. 1994. Preverbal sexual abuse: What two children report in later years. Child Abuse & Neglect 18(10): 821±826. Hirt E, Lynn SJ, Payne D, Krackow E, McCrea S. 1999. Memory and expectancy: Inferring the past from what we know must have been. In Expectancy, Experience, and Behavior, Kirsch I (ed.). American Psychological Association: Washington, DC. Hornstein NL, Putnam FW. 1992. Clinical phenomenology of child and adolescent dissociative disorders. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 31: 1077±1085. Copyright # 2001 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 15: S49±S73 (2001)
S70
M. L. Eisen and S. J. Lynn
Hyman IE Jr, Billings FJ. 1998. Individual differences and the creation of false childhood memories. Memory 6(1): 1±20. Hyman IE Jr, Husband TH, Billings FJ. 1995. False memories of childhood experiences. Applied Cognitive Psychology 9(3): 181±197. Hyman IE, Pentland J. 1998. The role of mental imagery in the creation of false childhood memories. Journal of Memory & Language 35(2): 101±117. Janet P. 1901. The Mental State of Hystericals. A Study of Mental Stigmata and Mental Accidents (Vol. 1). C. R. Corson, Trans. Putnam: New York. Janet P. 1925. Psychological Healing (Vols. 1±2). Macmillan: New York. (Original work published in 1919.) Johnson MK, Foley MA, Suengas AG, Raye CL. 1989. Phenomenal characteristics of memories for perceived and imagined autobiographical events. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 117: 371±376. Johnson MK, Hashtroudi S, Lindsay DS. 1993. Source monitoring. Psychological Bulletin 114: 3±28. Johnson MK, Raye CL. 1981. Reality monitoring. Psychological Review 88: 67±85. Kail R. 1990. The Development of Memory in Children (3rd edn). Freeman: New York. Koopman G, Classen C, Spiegel D. 1994. Predictors of post-traumatic stress symptoms among survivors of the Oakland/Berkely California ®restorm. American Journal of Psychiatry 151: 888±894. Koopman G, Classen C, Spiegel D. 1996. Dissociative responses in the immediate aftermath of the Oakland/Berkeley Firestorm. Journal of Traumatic Stress 9: 521±540. Lilienfeld S, Lynn SJ, Kirsch I, Chaves J, Sarbin T, Ganaway G. 1999. Dissociative identity disorder and the sociocognitive model: Recalling the lessons of the past. Psychological Bulletin 125: 507±523. Lindsay DS. 1990. Misleading suggestions can impair eyewitnesses' ability to remember event details. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 16: 1077±1083. Lindsay DS. 1998. Depolarizing views in recovered memories. In Truth in Memory, Lynn SJ, McConkey KM (eds). Guilford Press: New York; 481±494. Liotti G. 1991. Disorganized/disoriented attachment in the etiology of dissociative disorders. Dissociation 4: 196±204. Loftus EF. 1993. The reality of repressed memories. American Psychologist 48: 518±537. Loftus EF. 1997. Creating false memories. Scienti®c American 277: 70±75. Loftus EF, Pickrell JE. 1995. The formation of false memories. Psychiatric Annals 25(12): 720±725. Lynn SJ, Rhue JW. 1988. Fantasy proneness: Hypnosis, developmental antecedents, and psychopathology. American Psychologist 43(1): 35±44. Lynn SJ, Rhue JW. 1994. Dissociation: Clinical and Theoretical Perspectives. Guilford Press: New York. Lynn SJ, Rhue JW, Green JP. 1988. Multiple personality and fantasy proneness: Is there an association or dissociation? British Journal of Experimental and Clinical Hypnosis 5: 138±142. Marks DF. 1973. The vividness of visual imagery questionnaire. British Journal of Psychology 64: 17±24. Marmar CR, Weiss DS, Metzler TJ, Delucchi K. 1996. Characteristics of emergency services personnel related to peritraumatic dissociation during critical incident exposure. American Journal of Psychiatry 153(7): 94±103. Marmar CR, Weiss DS, Schlenger WE, Fairbanks JA, Jordan K, Kulka RA, Hough RI. 1994. Peritraumatic dissociation and post-traumatic stress in male Vietnam theater veterans. American Journal of Psychiatry 151: 902±907. Mather M, Henkel LA, Johnson MK. 1997. Evaluating characteristics of false memories: Remember/know judgments and memory characteristics questionnaire compared. Memory and Cognition 25: 826±237. McDermott KB. 1996. The persistence of false memories in list recall. Journal of Memory and Language 35(2): 212±230. Merckelbach H, Muris P, Rassin E, Horselenberg R. 2000. Dissociative experiences and interrogative suggestibility in college students. Personality & Individual Differences 29(6): 1133±1140. Copyright # 2001 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 15: S49±S73 (2001)
Dissociation and suggestibility
S71
Merckelbach H, Muris P, Schmidt H, Rassin E, Herslenberg R. 1998. De Creatieve Ervaringen Vragenlijst als maat voor `fantasy proneness' (The Creative Experiences Questionnaire as a measure of Fantasy proneness). De Psycholoog 33: 204±208. Nelson CA, Carver LJ. 1998. The effects of stress and trauma on brain and memory: A view from developmental cognitive neuroscience. Development & Psychopathology Special Issue: Risk, Trauma, and Memory 10(4): 793±809. Norman KA, Schacter DL. 1997. False recognition in young and older adults: Exploring the characteristics of illusory memories. Memory and Cognition 25: 838±848. Ogawa JR, Sroufe AL, Wein®eld NS, Carlson EA, Egland B. 1997. Development of the fragmented self: Longitudinal Study of dissociative symtomology in a nonclinical sample. Development & Psychopathology 9(4): 855±879. Paddock RJ, Joseph AL, Chan FM, Terranova S, Loftus EF, Manning C. 1998. When guided visualization procedures may back®re: Imagination in¯ation and predicting individual differences in suggestibility. Applied Cognitive Psychology 12: 63±75. Payne DG, Elie CJ, Blackwell JM, Neuschatz JS. 1996. Memory illusions: Recalling, recognizing, and recollecting events that never occurred. Journal of Memory and Language 35: 261±285. Payne D, Neuschatz J, Lampinen JM, Lynn SJ. 1997. Compelling memory illusions: The phenomenological qualities of false memories. Current Directions in Psychological Science 6: 56±60. Perry BD. 1994. Neurobiological sequelae of childhood trauma. Post-traumatic stress disorders in children. In Catecholamines in Post-traumatic Stress Disorder: Emerging Concepts, Murberg M (ed.). American Psychiatric Press: Washington, DC; 253±276. Perry BD, Pollard RA, Blakley TL, Baker WL, Vigilante D. 1995. Childhood trauma, the neurobiology of adaptation, and `use-dependent' development of the brain: How `states' become `traits' Infant Mental Health Journal 16: 271±282. Peterson C, Bell M. 1996. Children's memory for traumatic injury. Child Development 67: 3045±3070. Platt RD, Lacey SC, Iobst AD, Finkelman D. 1998. Absorption, dissociation, and fantasy-proneness as predictors or memory distortion in autobiographical and laboratory-generated memories. Applied Cognitive Psychology 12: 77±89. Putnam FW. 1985. Dissociation as a response to extreme trauma. In Childhood Antecedents of Multiple Personality, Kluft RP (ed.). American Psychiatric Press: Washington, DC. Putnam FW. 1989. Pierre Janet and modern view of dissociation. Journal of Traumatic Stress 2: 413±429. Putnam FW. 1994. Dissociative disorders in children and adolescents. Dissociation: Clinical and Theoretical Perspectives 175±189. Putnam FW. 1995. Development of dissociative disorders. In Developmental Psychopathology: Vol. 2: Risk, Disorder, and Adaptation, Cicchetti D, Cohen DJ (eds). John Wiley & Sons. Putnam FW. 1996. Posttraumatic stress disorder in children and adolescents. In Review of Psychiatry (Vol. 15), Dickstein L, Riba M, Oldham J (eds). American Psychiatric Press: Washington, DC; 447±467. Putnam FW. 1997. Dissociation in Children and Adolescents: A Developmental Perspective. Guilford Press: New York. Putnam FW, Guroff JJ, Silberman EK, Barban L, Post A. 1986. The clinical phenomenology of multiple personality disorder: Review of 100 recent cases. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 47(6): 285±293. Putnam FW, Helmers K, Trickett PK. 1993. Developmental, reliability and validity of a child dissociation scale. Child Abuse and Neglect 17: 731±741. Putnam FW, Trickett PK. 1997. The psychobiological effects of sexual abuse: A longitudinal study. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 821: 150±159. Pynoos RS, Nader K. 1988. Memory and proximity to violence. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 27: 567±572. Pynoos RS, Nader K. 1989. Children's memory and proximity to violence. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 28: 236±241. Pynoos RS, Steinberg AM, Wraith R. 1995. Developmental model of childhood traumatic stress. In Developmental Psychopathology, Vol. 2: Risk Disorder and Adaptation, Cicchetti D and Cohen DJ. Wiley: New York. Copyright # 2001 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 15: S49±S73 (2001)
S72
M. L. Eisen and S. J. Lynn
Pynoos RS, Steinberg AM, Aronson L. 1997. Traumatic experiences: The early organization of memory in school-age children and adolescents. In Trauma and Memory: Clinical and Legal Controversies, Applebaum PS, Uyehara LA, Elin MR (eds). Oxford University Press: New York; 272±289. Qin JJ. 1999. Adults' Memories of Childhood: True Versus False Reports. Dissertation, University of California, Davis, CA. Rauschenberger S, Lynn SJ. 1995. Fantasy proneness, DSM-III-R Axis I psychopathology, and dissociation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 104: 373±380. Read JD. 1996. From a passing thought to a false memory in 2 minutes: Confusing real and illusory events. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 3(1): 105±111. Reviere S. 1996. Memory of Childhood Trauma. Guilford Press: New York. Rhue J, Lynn SJ, Sandberg D. 1996. Dissociation, imagination, and fantasy in childhood: A comparison of sexually abused, physically abused, and non-abused children. Contemporary Hypnosis 12: 131±136. Robinson KJ, Roediger HL III. 1997. Associative processes in false recall and false recognition. Psychological Science 8(3): 231±237. Robinson KJ, Roediger HL. 1997. Associative processes in False recall and False recognition. Psychological Science 8: 231±237. Roediger HL. 1996. Memory illusions. Journal of Memory & Language Special Issue: Illusions of Memory 35(2): 76±100. Roediger HL, McDermott KB. 1995. Creating False Memories: Remembering words not presented in lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 21: 803±814. Roediger HL, McDermott KB, Robinson KJ. 1998. The role of associative processes in creating false memories. In Theories of Memory II, Conway MA, Gathercole SE, Cornoldi C (eds). Psychological Press: Hove; 187±254. Sandberg D, Lynn SJ. 1992. Dissociative experiences, psychopathology and adjustment, and child and adolescent maltreatment in female college students. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 101: 717±723. Schooler JW, Loftus EF. 1993. Multiple mechanisms mediate individual differences in eyewitness accuracy and suggestibility. In Mechanisms of Everyday Cognition, Pucket JM, Reese HW (eds). Erlbaum: Hillsdale, NJ; 177±203. Seamon JG, Luo CR, Gallo DA. 1998. Creating false memories of words with or without recognition of list items: Evidence for non-conscious processes. Psychological Science 9(1): 20±26. Segal D, Lynn SJ. 1992±1993. Predicting dissociative experiences: Imagination, hypnotizability, psychopathology, and alcohol. Imagination Cognition, and Personality 12: 287±300. Shobe KK, Kihlstrom JF. 1997. Is traumatic memory special? Current Directions in Psychological Science 6: 70±78. Spiegel D. 1991. Dissociation and trauma. In American Psychiatric Press Annual Review of Psychiatry, Tasman A (ed.). American Psychiatric Press: Washington, DC; 261±275. Spiegel D. 1995. Hypnosis and suggestion. In Memory Distortion: How Minds, Brains, and Societies Reconstruct the Past, Schachter DL (ed.). Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA; 129±149. Steinberg M. 1994. Systematizing dissociation: Symptomatology and diagnostic assessment. In Dissociation: Culture, Mind, and Body, Spiegel D (ed.). American Psychiatric Press: Washington DC. Tellegen A, Atkinson G. 1974. Openness to absorbing and self-altering experience (`absorption'), a trait related to hypnotic susceptibility. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 83: 268±277. Tousignant JP. 1984. Individual Differences in Response Bias and Recall: A Characterization of the Effects of Misleading Post-event Information. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Washington. Terr LC. 1991. Childhood traumas: An outline and overview. American Journal of Psychiatry 148: 10±20. Tussing AA, Green RL. 1997. False recognition of associates: How robust is the effect? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 4: 572±576. Van der Hart O, Friedman B. 1989. A reader's guide to Pierre Janet on dissociation: A neglected intellectual heritage. Dissociation 21: 3±16.
Copyright # 2001 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 15: S49±S73 (2001)
Dissociation and suggestibility
S73
Van der Kolk BA. 1996. Trauma and memory. In Traumatic Stress: The Effects of Overwhelming Experience on Mind, Body and Society, Van der Kolk BA, McFarlane AC, Weisaeth L (eds). Guilford Press: New York; 279±302. Van der Kolk BA, Brown P, van der Hart O. 1989. Pierre Janet and the breakdown of adaptation in psychological trauma. American Journal of Psychiatry 146: 1530±1540. Van der Kolk BA, van der Hart O, Marmar CR. 1996. Dissociation and information processing in post traumatic stress disorder. In Traumatic Stress: The Effects of Overwhelming Experience on Mind, Body and Society, van der Kolk BA, McFarlane AC, Weisaeth L (eds). Guilford Press: New York; 303±330. Waller NG, Putnam FW, Carlson EB. 1996. Types of dissociation and dissociative types: A taxometric analysis of dissociative experiences. Psychological Methods 1: 300±321. Waller NG, Ross CA. 1997. The prevalence and biometric structure of pathological dissociation in the general population: Taxometric and behavior genetic ®ndings. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 106: 499±510. Wilkinson C, Hyman IE. 1998. Individual differences related to two types of memory errors: Wordlists may not generalize to autobiographical memory. Applied Cognitive Psychology 12: S29±S46. Williams LM. 1994. Recall of childhood trauma: A prospective study of women's memories of child sexual abuse. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 62: 1167±1176. Wilson SC, Barber TX. 1981. Vivid fantasy and hallucinatory abilities in the life histories of excellent hypnotic subjects (somnabules): Preliminary report with female subjects. In Imagery: Concepts, Results, and Applications, Klinger E (ed.). Plenum Press: New York; 133±149. Wilson SC, Barber TX. 1981. The creative imagination Scale as a measure of hypnotic responsiveness: Application to experimental and clinical hypnosis. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis 20(4): 235±249. Wilson SC, Barber TX. 1983. The fantasy-prone personality: Implications for understanding imagery, hypnosis, and parapsychological phenomena. In Imagery: Current Theory, Research, and Application, Sheikh AA (ed.). Wiley: New York; 340±390. Winograd E, Peluso JP, Glover TA. 1998. Individual differences in susceptibility to memory illusions. Applied Cognitive Psychology 12: 5±27. Wolfradt U, Meyer T. 1998. Interrogative suggestibility, anxiety and dissociation among anxious patients and normal controls. Personality and Individual Differences 25(3): 425±432.
Copyright # 2001 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 15: S49±S73 (2001)