doi:10.1093/brain/awp326
Brain 2010: 133; 512–528
| 512
BRAIN A JOURNAL OF NEUROLOGY
Increased metabolic vulnerability in early-onset Alzheimer’s disease is not related to amyloid burden Gil D. Rabinovici,1,2,3,4 Ansgar J. Furst,3,4 Adi Alkalay,1,2,3 Caroline A. Racine,1,2 James P. O’Neil,4 Mustafa Janabi,4 Suzanne L. Baker,3,4 Neha Agarwal,3 Stephen J. Bonasera,1 Elizabeth C. Mormino,3,4 Michael W. Weiner,5 Maria L. Gorno-Tempini,1,2 Howard J. Rosen,1,2 Bruce L. Miller1,2 and William J. Jagust1,2,3,4 1 2 3 4 5
Memory and Aging Center, University of California San Francisco, San Francisco, CA 94143, USA Department of Neurology, University of California San Francisco, San Francisco, CA 94143, USA Helen Wills Neuroscience Institute, University of California Berkeley, Berkeley, CA 94720, USA Lawrence Berkeley National Laboratory, Berkeley, California; Department of Radiology, University of California, San Francisco, CA 94720, USA Center for Imaging of Neurodegenerative Diseases, Department of Veterans Affairs Medical Center, San Francisco, CA 94121, USA and Department of Radiology, University of California San Francisco, San Francisco, CA 94143, USA
Correspondence to: Dr Gil D. Rabinovici, UCSF Memory & Aging Center, 350 Parnassus Ave., Suite 905, San Francisco, CA 94143, USA E-mail:
[email protected]
Patients with early age-of-onset Alzheimer’s disease show more rapid progression, more generalized cognitive deficits and greater cortical atrophy and hypometabolism compared to late-onset patients at a similar disease stage. The biological mechanisms that underlie these differences are not well understood. The purpose of this study was to examine in vivo whether metabolic differences between early-onset and late-onset Alzheimer’s disease are associated with differences in the distribution and burden of fibrillar amyloid-b. Patients meeting criteria for probable Alzheimer’s disease (National Institute of Neurological and Communicative Disorders and Stroke and the Alzheimer’s Disease and Related Disorders Association criteria) were divided based on estimated age at first symptom (less than or greater than 65 years) into early-onset (n = 21, mean age-at-onset 55.2 5.9 years) and late-onset (n = 18, 72.0 4.7 years) groups matched for disease duration and severity. Patients underwent positron emission tomography with the amyloid-b-ligand [11C]-labelled Pittsburgh compound-B and the glucose analogue [18F]-labelled fluorodeoxyglucose. A group of cognitively normal controls (n = 30, mean age 73.7 6.4) was studied for comparison. [11C]-labelled Pittsburgh compound-B images were analysed using Logan graphical analysis (cerebellar reference) and [18F]-labelled fluorodeoxyglucose images were normalized to mean activity in the pons. Group differences in tracer uptake were assessed on a voxel-wise basis using statistical parametric mapping, and by comparing mean values in regions of interest. To account for brain atrophy, analyses were repeated after applying partial volume correction to positron emission tomography data. Compared to normal controls, both early-onset and late-onset Alzheimer’s disease patient groups showed increased [11C]labelled Pittsburgh compound-B uptake throughout frontal, parietal and lateral temporal cortices and striatum on voxel-wise and region of interest comparisons (P50.05). However, there were no significant differences in regional or global [11C]-labelled Pittsburgh compound-B binding between early-onset and late-onset patients. In contrast, early-onset patients showed significantly lower glucose metabolism than late-onset patients in precuneus/posterior cingulate, lateral temporo–parietal and occipital corticies (voxel-wise and region of interest comparisons, P50.05). Similar results were found for [11C]-labelled Pittsburgh Received September 8, 2009. Revised November 1, 2009. Accepted November 22, 2009. Advance Access publication January 15, 2010 ß The Author (2010). Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of the Guarantors of Brain. All rights reserved. For Permissions, please email:
[email protected]
Age, amyloid and metabolism in Alzheimer’s disease
Brain 2010: 133; 512–528
| 513
compound-B and [18F]-labelled fluorodeoxyglucose using atrophy-corrected data. Age-at-onset correlated positively with glucose metabolism in precuneus, lateral parietal and occipital regions of interest (controlling for age, education and Mini Mental State Exam, P50.05), while no correlations were found between age-at-onset and [11C]-labelled Pittsburgh compound-B binding. In summary, a comparable burden of fibrillar amyloid-b was associated with greater posterior cortical hypometabolism in early-onset Alzheimer’s disease. Our data are consistent with a model in which both early amyloid-b accumulation and increased vulnerability to amyloid-b pathology play critical roles in the pathogenesis of Alzheimer’s disease in young patients.
Keywords: Alzheimer’s disease; age of onset; amyloid-b; [18F]-labelled fluorodeoxyglucose; [11C]-labelled Pittsburgh compound-B Abbreviations: ApoE4 = apolipoprotein E e4 allele; FDG = [18F]-labelled fluorodeoxyglucose; FWE = family-wise error; MMSE = Mini-Mental State Exam score; PIB = [11C]-labelled Pittsburgh compound-B
Introduction Though generally considered a disease of the elderly, Alzheimer’s disease is also a leading cause of early age-of-onset dementia (Ratnavalli et al., 2002; Knopman et al., 2004; Mercy et al., 2008). Patients who present with early-onset Alzheimer’s disease, defined in most studies as the onset of symptoms before age 65, show more rapid clinical decline and shorter survival than late-onset patients (Seltzer and Sherwin, 1983; Barclay et al., 1985; Heyman et al., 1987; Mortimer et al., 1992; Jacobs et al., 1994; Koss et al., 1996). The majority of studies have also found an effect of age on the pattern of cognitive deficits, with early-onset patients showing greater impairment in attention, language, visuo–spatial and executive functions, and late-onset patients showing comparatively greater deficits in episodic memory (Seltzer and Sherwin, 1983; Loring and Largen, 1985; Filley et al., 1986; Binetti et al., 1993; Jacobs et al., 1994; Koss et al., 1996; Fujimori et al., 1998; Imamura et al., 1998; Frisoni et al., 2005, 2007; Kim et al., 2005; Snowden et al., 2007). Mirroring these clinical differences, neuroimaging studies show greater cortical atrophy, hypoperfusion and hypometabolism in early-onset Alzheimer’s disease, particularly in parietal and lateral temporal cortices, and relatively greater medial temporal lesions in lateonset Alzheimer’s disease (Grady et al., 1987; Jagust et al., 1990; Yasuno et al., 1998; Salmon et al., 2000; Sakamoto et al., 2002; Kemp et al., 2003; Frisoni et al., 2005, 2007; Ishii et al., 2005; Kim et al., 2005; Shiino et al., 2006, 2008; Karas et al., 2007). Age-of-onset correlates positively with glucose metabolism in frontal, parietal and temporal corticies, with the youngest patients tending to show the most severe hypometabolism, while age correlates negatively with glucose metabolism in hippocampus (Yasuno et al., 1998). When controlling for dementia severity, patients with early-onset Alzheimer’s disease show more extensive structural and functional neuroimaging changes than their late-onset counterparts (Kim et al., 2005). The biological mechanisms underlying the clinical differences between early-onset and late-onset Alzheimer’s disease are not well understood. Only a small minority (1–5%) of patients carry disease-causing mutations that lead to autosomal dominant early-onset Alzheimer’s disease via early and rapid aggregation of amyloid-b (Rocchi et al., 2003). Patients with ‘sporadic’ early-onset disease are more likely to be carriers of the apolipoprotein E e4 allele (ApoE4) (Farrer et al., 1997), which is also
associated with early deposition of amyloid-b plaques (Kok et al., 2009; Morris et al., 2009). Post-mortem studies comparing the burden of Alzheimer’s disease pathology in early-onset and late-onset patients demonstrate a higher burden of both neuritic plaques and neurofibrillary tangles in younger patients, though this association has been more consistently found for tangles than plaques (Mann et al., 1984; Hansen et al., 1988; Nochlin et al., 1993; Berg et al., 1998; Bigio et al., 2002; Ho et al., 2002; Marshall et al., 2007). Neurochemical differences between early-onset and late-onset patients are also evident, with younger patients showing more diffuse and severe deficits in acetylcholine and norepinephrine, reflecting greater degeneration of the nucleus of Meynert and the locus coeruleus (Bird et al., 1983; Rossor et al., 1984). Taken together, these data suggest a model in which early-onset patients, in part due to genetic factors, develop early and accelerated deposition of pathology, which in turn leads to a precipitous clinical decline. The fact that early-onset patients show a higher burden of pathology and more widespread neurodegeneration in comparison to late-onset patients of similar clinical severity has been interpreted as evidence for greater ‘cognitive reserve’ in younger brains (Kim et al., 2005). While post-mortem studies comparing early-onset and lateonset Alzheimer’s disease are invaluable, they also have inherent limitations. Early-onset patients typically die in the end-stages of dementia, whereas late-onset patients are more likely to die at an earlier disease stage of an unrelated cause. This has led to a systematic bias towards more severe disease at the time of autopsy in early-onset Alzheimer’s disease (Berg et al., 1998; Ho et al., 2002). A limited number of brain regions are sampled on autopsy, subjecting studies to sampling error while making it difficult to describe the precise anatomic distribution of pathology. Many studies employed semi-quantitative measures of pathology such as CERAD (Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease) scores (Mirra et al., 1991) or Braak staging (Braak and Braak, 1991), limiting statistical inferences. Even studies that more precisely quantified lesion burden did not employ stereological techniques, and thus may have been biased by brain atrophy and tissue shrinkage, which are both greater in younger patients (West, 1993). Finally, in most studies there was a significant delay between the last meaningful clinical evaluation and the time of death, making it difficult to derive clinicopathologic correlations. A recent advance in neuroimaging allows the measurement of amyloid-b amyloid in vivo using the positron emission tomography
514
| Brain 2010: 133; 512–528
G. D. Rabinovici et al.
Table 1 Subject characteristics Normal controls (n = 30) Age at onset (years) Age at PET (years) Disease duration (years) Sex (F,M) Education (years) MMSE CDR Total Sum of boxes Medications ChE-I Memantine Family history (first-deg) ApoE4 (0,1,2)
73.7 (6.4) 22,8 17.7 (1.7) 29.2 (1.0)
21,8,1
Early-onset Alzheimer’s disease (n = 21) 55.2 60.2 5.0 10,11 16.7 20.3
(5.9) (6.2) (2.8) (3.2) (6.4)
Late-onset Alzheimer’s disease (n = 18) 72.0 77.8 5.8 5,13 16.8 23.5
(4.7) (4.9) (2.9) (2.3) (4.4)
P 50.0001 50.0001a 0.39 0.007b 0.29 50.0001c
0.9 (0.6) 5.2 (3.5)
0.8 (0.3) 5.0 (1.9)
0.60 0.84
16/21 (76%) 11/21 (52%) 8/21 (38%) 5,9,5
14/18 (78%) 7/18 (39%) 5/18 (28%) 7,3,4
1.00 0.40 0.50 0.02c
Values are presented as mean (standard deviation). ChE-I = acetylcholine esterase inhibitors; first-deg = first degree relative; CDR = Clinical Dementia Rating. Missing data: CDR (early-onset 2; late-onset 3); ApoE4 (early-onset 2, late-onset 4). a Early-onset versus normal controls and late-onset, P50.0001. b Late-onset versus normal controls, P = 0.002. c Early-onset and late-onset versus normal controls, P50.05.
(PET) tracer Pittsburgh compound-B (PIB) (Klunk et al., 2004). PIB is a thioflavin-T analogue that, at PET tracer concentrations, binds fibrillar amyloid-b with high sensitivity and specificity (Klunk et al., 2003). Elevated PIB binding is seen in the vast majority of patients with Alzheimer’s disease in the known distribution of amyloid-b neuritic plaques (Klunk et al., 2004; Kemppainen et al., 2006). PIB signal during life correlates strongly with the distribution and burden of amyloid-b pathology found at autopsy (Bacskai et al., 2007; Ikonomovic et al., 2008). Thus, PIB allows the quantification of fibrillar amyloid-b during life with high precision and anatomic resolution. In this study, we applied PIB-PET to investigate in vivo the effects of age-of-onset on the distribution and burden of amyloid-b plaques in Alzheimer’s disease. We selected patients in the mild-to-moderate (rather than severe) stage of disease, and compared the effect of age on amyloid to its impact on resting glucose metabolism. Based on previous work, we hypothesized that PIB uptake would be increased in early-onset Alzheimer’s disease compared to patients with late-onset disease, and that this would be accompanied by more severe temporoparietal hypometabolism in younger patients.
Methods Subject selection and characterization All patients were recruited from an Alzheimer’s disease research cohort followed at the University of California San Francisco Memory and Aging Centre. The clinical evaluation includes a history and physical examination by a neurologist, a structured caregiver interview by a nurse, and a battery of neuropsychological tests (Kramer et al., 2003). Clinical diagnosis was assigned by consensus at a
multi-disciplinary conference using standard research criteria (McKhann et al., 1984). Patients with significant co-morbid medical or psychiatric illness were not eligible for the study. Patients with significant cerebrovascular disease {as defined by more than one lacunar infarct or severe white matter hyperintensities on MRI [Longstreth scale 56 (Longstreth et al., 1996)]} were also excluded. For this study we included all patients who met research criteria for probable Alzheimer’s disease (McKhann et al., 1984) and underwent PIB-PET imaging at Lawrence Berkeley National Laboratory between April 2005 and January 2009. Enrolment was independent of PIB scan results (i.e. positive or negative for elevated tracer uptake). However, one patient (PIB-negative, age at PET 66.5) was excluded because on longitudinal follow-up the clinical diagnosis was changed from Alzheimer’s disease to frontotemporal dementia. Patients with a clinical diagnosis other than Alzheimer’s disease (e.g. primary progressive aphasia) were not included even if the clinician suspected the clinical syndromes were due to underlying Alzheimer’s disease histopathology. To avoid the confounding effects of co-morbid pathology, patients with a diagnosis of mixed dementia (e.g. mixed Alzheimer’s disease and dementia with Lewy bodies) were also excluded. Age at onset was determined based on the estimated date of first symptoms as stated by patients or caregivers. Consistent with most previous reports, we dichotomized Alzheimer’s disease patients into early-onset (age at onset 565) and late-onset (age at onset 465) groups. Our final cohort consisted of 39 Alzheimer’s disease patients (mean age-at-onset 62.0 10.1, median 61.7), 21 with early-onset disease (mean 55.2, range 43.4–64.7) and 18 with late-onset disease (mean 72.0, range 66.7–82.0) (Table 1). Thirty cognitively normal older volunteers were included as imaging controls. These subjects were recruited from the Berkeley Aging Cohort as previously described (Mormino et al., 2009). All control subjects were deemed to be cognitively normal following a battery of neuropsychological tests (Mormino et al., 2009). Apolipoprotein E genotyping was performed for 33 Alzheimer’s disease patients and 30 normal controls using previously published methods (Agosta et al., 2009).
Age, amyloid and metabolism in Alzheimer’s disease
Imaging PET acquisition [11C]-labelled PIB was synthesized at the Lawrence Berkeley National Laboratory’s Biomedical Isotope Facility using standard methods (Mathis et al., 2003). [18F]-labelled fluorodeoxyglucose (FDG) was purchased from a commercial vendor (Eastern Isotopes, Sterling, VA). PET scans were performed using a Siemens ECAT EXACT HR PET scanner in 3D acquisition mode. Approximately 15 mCi of PIB was injected as a bolus into an antecubital vein and dynamic acquisition frames were obtained for 90 min as previously described (Rabinovici et al., 2007). All 30 controls and 36/39 Alzheimer’s disease patients underwent FDG-PET immediately following the PIB scan, while three patients (two early-onset and one late-onset) could not receive an FDG scan for technical reasons. Patients were injected with 10 mCi of FDG and imaged at a minimum of 2 h after PIB injection (six 11C half-lives). Thirty minutes of emission data were collected at t = 30–60 min after tracer injection with patients resting quietly with eyes and ears unoccluded. Ten-minute transmission scans for attenuation correction were obtained either immediately before or after each PIB and FDG scan. PET data were reconstructed using an ordered subset expectation maximization algorithm with weighted attenuation. Images were smoothed with a 4 mm Gaussian kernel with scatter correction. All images were evaluated before analysis for patient motion and adequacy of statistical counts. Two FDG-PET scans (one each for early-onset and late-onset Alzheimer’s disease) were excluded from analysis because of technical problems.
MRI acquisition Thirty Alzheimer’s disease patients (17/21 early-onset patients and 13/ 18 late-onset patients) underwent research protocol MRI scans at the San Francisco Veterans Affair Medical Centre within one year of PET imaging (mean interval between MRI and PET 96 99 days). Nineteen patients (10 with early-onset Alzheimer’s disease and nine with late-onset Alzheimer’s disease) were scanned on a 1.5 Tesla Siemens VISION system (Rosen et al., 2002), and 11 patients (seven early-onset and fou late-onset) were scanned on a Bruker MedSpec 4 Tesla system controlled by a Siemens TrioTM console (Mueller et al., 2009). All 30 normal control subjects were imaged at Lawrence Berkeley National Laboratory on a 1.5 Tesla Siemens Avanto System (mean interval to PET 95 200 days) (Mormino et al., 2009). Given the heterogeneous MRI acquisition protocols, structural images were used only to assist with PET analysis and a primary MRI analysis was not performed.
PET analysis Image processing and analysis was performed using Statistical Parametric Mapping 2 software (http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm). Reference regions in whole cerebellum (for PIB) and pons (for FDG) were created in Montreal Neurological Institute space and then warped to each subject’s native space using a ‘reverse normalization’ procedure (Rabinovici et al., 2007). To allow for inter-subject comparisons, FDG frames for each subject were summed and normalized to mean activity in the pons (Minoshima et al., 1995). For PIB, voxel-wise distribution volume ratios were calculated using Logan graphical analysis, with the cerebellum time-activity curve used as a reference tissue input function (t = 35–90 min) (Lopresti et al., 2005; Price et al., 2005). The cerebellum is the preferred reference region for PIB because it is relatively free of fibrillar amyloid even in advanced Alzheimer’s disease (Joachim et al., 1989).
Brain 2010: 133; 512–528
| 515
To ensure that there were no differences in cerebellar PIB binding between early-onset and late-onset Alzheimer’s disease patients, we calculated distribution volume ratios for cerebellum in every Alzheimer’s disease patient using the pons time–activity curve as the reference input function. The pons has been previously shown to be an area of non-specific PIB binding with no difference in tracer uptake between Alzheimer’s disease patients and controls (Klunk et al., 2004). We found no difference in cerebellum distribution volume ratios between early-onset Alzheimer’s disease (mean distribution volume ratio 0.62 0.05) and late-onset Alzheimer’s disease (mean distribution volume ratio 0.61 0.05, P = 0.55), validating the use of cerebellum as a reference region when comparing these two patient groups.
Partial volume correction Given previous reports of greater cortical atrophy in early-onset versus late-onset Alzheimer’s disease (Frisoni et al., 2005, 2007; Ishii et al., 2005), we were concerned that brain atrophy may confound our group comparisons by preferentially lowering PET counts in early-onset subjects. We thus corrected PET data for atrophy by applying a two-compartmental partial-volume correction to all subjects with an MRI (Meltzer et al., 1990). The correction procedure involved convolving a brain mask (a sum of the grey and white matter segmented images from the subject’s T1-weighted MRI) with the point-spread function specific to the PET tomograph along all axes [previously empirically derived (Klein et al., 1997)]. This provided a means for estimating the percentage of brain tissue emitting radioactivity at each voxel. The PET counts for each voxel were then adjusted based on the percentage of estimated brain matter (Meltzer et al., 1999). All primary analyses were performed using uncorrected PET data from all subjects, while confirmatory analyses were conducted with atrophy-corrected data in the subset of patients with an available MRI.
Voxel-wise group comparisons Individual subject PIB and FDG volumes were spatially normalized to Montreal Neurological Institute space. This was done separately for non-atrophy corrected data (PET-based normalization) and atrophy-corrected data (MRI-based normalization). In the PET-based normalization stream, mean PIB and FDG images created during frame realignment were normalized to the Statistical Parametric Mapping PET template. Normalization parameters were then applied to the PIB distribution volume ratio and FDG pons-normalized volumes, respectively. In the MRI-based normalization stream, the subject’s T1-weighted MRI was spatially normalized to a template brain derived from older individuals (the minimal deformation template 2) (Sun et al., 2007). The normalization parameters were subsequently applied to the subject’s co-registered, atrophy-corrected PIB and FDG volumes. All spatially-normalized images were smoothed with a 12 mm kernel. Voxel-wise comparisons of PIB distribution volume ratio and pons-normalized FDG images were performed in an analysis of covariance model that included diagnosis (early-onset Alzheimer’s disease, late-onset Alzheimer’s disease or normal control) as the condition and sex, education and Mini-Mental State Exam score (MMSE) as nuisance variables. Pair-wise contrasts were performed between the three groups as follows: (i) early-onset Alzheimer’s disease4normal controls; (ii) late-onset Alzheimer’s disease4normal controls; (iii) early-onset Alzheimer’s disease4late-onset Alzheimer’s disease; (iv) late-onset Alzheimer’s disease4early-onset Alzheimer’s disease; (v) normal controls4early-onset Alzheimer’s disease; and (vi) normal
516
| Brain 2010: 133; 512–528
controls4late-onset Alzheimer’s disease. To allow broad visualization of the data, results were displayed on a template brain as T-maps thresholded at P50.001 uncorrected for multiple comparisons. Voxels were considered significant at P50.05 after family-wise error (FWE) correction. All voxel-wise analyses were repeated with atrophycorrected data using the same analysis of covariance model, post-hoc contrasts and statistical thresholds.
Region of interest definition PET counts were extracted in normalized space from regions of interest derived from the Automated Anatomic Labelling Atlas (TzourioMazoyer et al., 2002). In order to exclude PET counts from white matter and cerebrospinal fluid, automated regions of interest were masked by the individual subject’s grey matter segmented images (Sun et al., 2007). Pre-defined regions of interest included (separately for left and right hemispheres): frontal cortex (anterior to precentral gyrus), precuneus, posterior cingulate cortex, lateral parietal cortex, lateral temporal cortex, temporal pole, medial temporal cortex (parahippocampal, fusiform and lingual gyrus), occipital cortex (excluding striate cortex), hippocampus and striatum. As a measure of global amyloid burden we calculated a PIB index, representing the subject’s mean distribution volume ratio in frontal, posterior cingulate, precuneus, parietal and lateral temporal cortices (Mormino et al., 2009). Again, primary region of interest analyses were performed with non-atrophy corrected data (available for all subjects), and confirmatory analyses were performed with atrophy-corrected data (available for a subset of subjects).
Statistical analysis Group differences in continuous variables were examined using one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) and Tukey post hoc contrasts or Mann–Whitney U-tests when appropriate. Dichotomous variables were compared with the Pearson’s chi-square test or Fisher’s exact test. The effect of age-at-onset as a continuous (rather than dichotomous) variable on regional PIB and FDG binding in Alzheimer’s disease was studied using multi-linear regression, with education and MMSE included as covariates. Sex was not included as a covariate in this analysis since gender distribution did not differ based on age-at-onset. The study was approved by the University of California Berkeley, University of California San Francisco and Lawrence Berkeley National Laboratory institutional review boards for human research.
Results Subject characteristics Patients with early-onset Alzheimer’s disease were significantly younger at onset than late-onset patients, and significantly younger at PET than both late-onset patients and normal controls (Table 1). Age at onset and age at PET were highly correlated (r = 0.96, P50.0001). The gender distribution differed between normal controls (female preponderance) and late-onset Alzheimer’s disease (male preponderance, P50.005), and was thus included as a nuisance variable in the three-way voxel-wise analysis. All participants were highly educated. Early-onset and late-onset patients were well matched for disease duration and for functional status, as measured by the Clinical Dementia
G. D. Rabinovici et al. Rating total and sum-of-boxes (Morris, 1993). Patients performed worse than normal controls on the MMSE (P50.0001) and there was a trend for worse performance by early-onset versus late-onset patients (P = 0.06). MMSE was therefore included as a covariate in group comparisons. There were no differences between early-onset and late-onset Alzheimer’s disease in medication use or in the percentage of patients with a family history of dementia in a first degree relative. Of note, none of the patients had a family history suggestive of autosomal dominant Alzheimer’s disease, though only one patient was screened (and tested negative) for mutations in presenilin-1 and -2. The ApoE4 genotype was more common in patients compared to controls, but there was no difference in ApoE gene distribution between early-onset and late-onset patients (P = 0.25). Only one Alzheimer’s disease patient carried an ApoE2 allele (E2/E3 genotype, age-at-onset 70.4 years).
Neuropsychological profiles Neuropsychological test batteries obtained within one year of PET were available for 18/21 early-onset patients and 14/18 late-onset patients (Table 2). The mean interval between cognitive testing and PET was 86 82 days. Because of the skewed distribution of most test scores, group medians and ranges are presented in place of means and standard deviations. Early-onset patients performed significantly worse than late-onset patients on digit span backwards, a test of attention and working memory (P = 0.02). There were non-significant trends for worse performance by early-onset patients on tests of executive function (Stroop Interference Test), language (syntax comprehension) and visuo-spatial function (copy of modified Rey-Osterrieth figure). Median scores were lower in late-onset Alzheimer’s disease for delayed recall tasks and lower in early-onset Alzheimer’s disease for most tests of language, visuo-spatial and executive function; but these differences were not significant.
Pittsburgh compound-B: voxel-wise comparisons Compared to normal controls, patients with both early-onset and late-onset Alzheimer’s disease showed diffuse, symmetric PIB binding throughout frontal, parietal and temporal cortices, with sparing of primary sensorimotor and visual cortices [P(FWE-corr)50.05, Fig. 1 left panels]. On direct contrast of the two patient groups, early-onset patients showed higher PIB binding in left medial temporal cortex and right temporal pole at P50.001 uncorrected (Fig. 1), but only a small cluster of voxels (n = 49) in left fusiform gyrus survived multiple comparisons correction. Late-onset patients showed higher PIB uptake in left superior parietal lobule at P50.001 uncorrected, but this did not survive FWE correction. There were no voxels in which PIB uptake was greater in normal controls than in early-onset or late-onset patients at P50.001 uncorrected. Atrophy correction did not alter the pattern of PIB uptake in patients compared to normal controls, though the statistical significance of the contrasts increased as PIB counts were raised in atrophic regions (Fig. 1, right panels). Following atrophy
Age, amyloid and metabolism in Alzheimer’s disease
Brain 2010: 133; 512–528
| 517
Table 2 Neuropsychological test scores
MMSE (30) Memory CVLT-SF total learning (36) CVLT-SF 10 min recall (9) Modified Rey 10 min recall (17) Executive function Modified Trails-B time (120’’) Modified Trails-B no. correct lines (14) Digits backward span Stroop Interference Test no. correct Language Boston Naming Test (15 item) Sentence repetition (5) Syntax comprehension (5) Letter fluency (D words) Category fluency (animals) Visuo–spatial Modified Rey copy (17) VOSP Spatial Location Test (10) Calculations (5)
Early-onset Alzheimer’s disease (n = 18)
Late-onset Alzheimer’s disease (n = 16)
P
23.0 (13–29)
25.0 (10–29)
0.20
16.0 (3–32) 0.5 (0–9) 3.0 (0–14)
17.5 (6–27) 0.0 (0–7) 0.0 (0–12)
0.82 0.68 0.34
120 7.0 3.0 5.0
(29–120) (1–14) (0–6) (0–55)
99 14.0 4.0 26.0
(32–120) (0–14) (3–7) (9–35)
0.41 0.52 0.02 0.07
12.0 4.0 3.0 8.0 9.5
(2–15) (1–5) (0–5) (2–18) (4–14)
11.0 3.0 4.0 10.0 10.0
(3–15) (1–5) (1–5) (4–15) (2–20)
0.84 0.54 0.09 0.23 0.51
14.0 (3–16) 9.0 (3–10) 4.0 (0–5)
0.12 0.27 0.19
11.5 (2–17) 6.0 (2–10) 3.0 (2–5)
Maximum attainable scores are presented in parentheses. Values are presented as medians (range). Missing data: CVLT learning (late-onset 4), CVLT recall (late-onset 3), Rey recall (early-onset 2, late-onset 1), Modified Trials (early-onset 5, late-onset 6), Stroop (early-onset 7, late-onset 5), Boston Naming Test (late-onset 2), Sentence repetition (late-onset 2), Sentence comprehension (early-onset 2, late-onset 5), Letter fluency (late-onset 3), Rey copy (early-onset 2, late-onset 1), VOSP (early-onset 3, late-onset 3), Calculations (early-onset 2, late-onset 2). CVLT-SF = California Verbal Learning Test – San Francisco (9-item); Modified Rey = Modified Rey-Osterrieth design; BNT – Boston Naming Test (15-item); VOSP – Visual Object and Space Perception (number location task).
Figure 1 Patterns of PIB binding in early-onset (EO) and late-onset (LO) Alzheimer’s disease (AD) compared to normal controls (NC) and to each other. Voxel-wise comparisons included sex, education and MMSE as nuisance variables. T-score maps prior to atrophy correction are rendered on the ch2 template brain (left), while T-score maps following atrophy correction are rendered on the minimal deformation template 2 template (right). All results are presented at a threshold of P50.001, uncorrected for multiple comparisons.
518
| Brain 2010: 133; 512–528
G. D. Rabinovici et al.
Table 3 PIB distribution volume ratios in regions of interest before and after atrophy correction PIB
No atrophy correction
Region
Normal controls (n = 30)
Early-onset Alzheimer’s disease (n = 21)
Late-onset Alzheimer’s disease (n = 18)
P (ANOVA)
Normal controls (n = 30)
Early-onset Alzheimer’s disease (n = 17)
Late-onset Alzheimer’s disease (n = 13)
P (ANOVA)
PIB index L frontal R frontal L precuneus R precuneus L posterior cingulate R posterior cingulate L lateral parietal R lateral parietal L lateral temporal R lateral temporal L medial temporal R medial temporal L occipital R occipital L temporal pole R temporal pole L hippocampus R hippocampus L striatum R striatum
1.13 1.13 1.13 1.08 1.09 1.26 1.28 1.12 1.11 1.15 1.15 1.08 1.07 1.17 1.15 0.92 0.92 1.15 1.13 1.22 1.22
1.58 1.61 1.59 1.58 1.55 1.62 1.48 1.52 1.51 1.59 1.58 1.29 1.30 1.45 1.44 1.25 1.25 1.21 1.20 1.58 1.59
1.58 1.60 1.58 1.62 1.64 1.54 1.42 1.60 1.58 1.53 1.55 1.22 1.23 1.47 1.45 1.15 1.13 1.10 1.12 1.59 1.56
50.0001a,b 50.0001a,b 50.0001a,b 50.0001a,b 50.0001a,b 50.0001a,b 0.01a 50.0001a,b 50.0001a,b 50.0001a,b 50.0001a,b 50.0001a,b 50.0001a,b 50.0001a,b 50.0001a,b 50.0001a,b 50.0001a,b 0.06 0.23 50.0001a,b 50.0001a,b
1.32 1.35 1.33 1.27 1.20 1.32 1.30 1.32 1.35 1.30 1.31 1.15 1.17 1.28 1.28 1.32 1.31 1.18 1.17 1.34 1.32
2.03 2.10 2.05 2.04 1.87 1.97 1.82 2.03 2.02 2.04 2.00 1.50 1.48 1.67 1.61 1.95 1.90 1.28 1.25 1.80 1.78
2.05 2.10 2.07 2.05 1.96 2.00 1.86 2.03 2.07 2.01 2.07 1.47 1.44 1.72 1.68 1.78 1.81 1.17 1.19 1.89 1.88
50.0001a,b 50.0001a,b 50.0001a,b 50.0001a,b 50.0001a,b 50.0001a,b 50.0001a,b 50.0001a,b 50.0001a,b 50.0001a,b 50.0001a,b 50.0001a,b 50.0001a,b 50.0001a,b 50.0001a,b 50.0001a,b 50.0001a,b 0.08 0.25 50.0001a,b 50.0001a,b
(0.18) (0.20) (0.19) (0.21) (0.21) (0.15) (0.17) (0.20) (0.20) (0.18) (0.16) (0.11) (0.11) (0.15) (0.14) (0.12) (0.11) (0.11) (0.13) (0.16) (0.16)
(0.21) (0.20) (0.22) (0.18) (0.25) (0.27) (0.30) (0.19) (0.22) (0.23) (0.29) (0.15) (0.19) (0.20) (0.23) (0.19) (0.22) (0.15) (0.19) (0.25) (0.24)
Atrophy correction
(0.33) (0.35) (0.35) (0.37) (0.37) (0.32) (0.25) (0.32) (0.32) (0.33) (0.35) (0.22) (0.23) (0.30) (0.30) (0.27) (0.26) (0.21) (0.21) (0.29) (0.28)
(0.24) (0.26) (0.26) (0.23) (0.22) (0.22) (0.23) (0.23) (0.25) (0.22) (0.19) (0.12) (0.11) (0.14) (0.12) (0.18) (0.16) (0.11) (0.12) (0.21) (0.20)
(0.35) (0.36) (0.34) (0.37) (0.35) (0.32) (0.34) (0.39) (0.40) (0.38) (0.43) (0.17) (0.19) (0.24) (0.23) (0.41) (0.37) (0.16) (0.18) (0.31) (0.35)
(0.36) (0.38) (0.37) (0.40) (0.38) (0.32) (0.30) (0.43) (0.41) (0.38) (0.39) (0.21) (0.19) (0.41) (0.30) (0.34) (0.33) (0.19) (0.21) (0.27) (0.220
Values are presented as mean (standard deviation). a Early-onset4normal controls, P50.05. b Late-onset4normal controls, P50.05.
correction, early-onset patients showed greater PIB uptake than late-onset patients in bilateral temporal poles while late-onset patients showed greater uptake in left superior parietal lobule at P50.001 uncorrected (Fig. 1); however none of these differences survived FWE correction.
Pittsburgh compound-B: region of interest comparisons Early-onset patients showed higher mean PIB distribution volume ratio values than normal controls in all regions of interest except hippocampus, while patients with late-onset Alzheimer’s disease showed higher uptake than normal controls in all regions of interest except hippocampus, right posterior cingulate and right temporal pole (Table 3). Mean PIB index was essentially identical in patients with both early-onset and late-onset Alzheimer’s disease, and no significant differences between patients groups were detected in any regions. Following atrophy correction, early-onset and late-onset patients showed higher PIB than normal controls in all regions except hippocampus. As expected, the atrophy correction procedure raised the PIB index of early-onset patients more than that of late-onset patients (early-onset 29.4% 7.8%, late-onset 21.8% 9.4%, P50.05). Nevertheless, even following atrophy correction, there were no differences in PIB index or in regional PIB uptake between patients with early-onset and late-onset disease (Table 3).
Scatter-plots of PIB index by diagnostic group (before and after atrophy correction) are shown in Fig. 2A. As expected, most normal controls showed low PIB uptake, though a minority showed elevated binding in the Alzheimer’s disease range. While overall group means were the same, the distribution of the PIB index appeared different in early-onset and late-onset Alzheimer’s disease. Late-onset patients showed a bimodal distribution, with four subjects falling in the low normal controls range, and the remaining subjects showing high index values. In contrast, patients with early-onset Alzheimer’s disease showed a continuum of PIB uptake beginning at the very top of the normal controls cluster and extending into the high index range (Fig. 2A). Only one of four ‘low PIB’ late-onset patients had an MRI available and this patient continued to show a low PIB index following atrophy correction (bottom subject in the atrophy-corrected late-onset distribution).
Fluorodeoxyglucose: voxel-wise comparisons Compared to normal controls, early-onset patients showed extensive regions of decreased glucose metabolism throughout bilateral temporo–parietal and occipital cortices (P50.001 uncorrected, Fig. 3). Differences in bilateral angular gyrus and right middle and inferior temporal gyrus remained significant after multiple comparisons correction [P(FWE-corr)50.05]. There were no regions
Age, amyloid and metabolism in Alzheimer’s disease
Brain 2010: 133; 512–528
| 519
Figure 2 Scatter-plots of PIB index (A) and normalized FDG uptake in precuneus (B) by diagnosis, pre- (left) and post-atrophy correction (right). Hatched bars represent mean values for each diagnostic group. DVR = distribution volume ratio. CONT = controls; EO = earlyonset; LO = late-onset; AD = Alzheimer’s disease. of decreased metabolism in late-onset Alzheimer’s disease compared to normal controls at P50.001 uncorrected, though bilateral temporo–parietal hypometabolism was found if MMSE was removed from the model and the statistical threshold was lowered to P50.01 (data not shown). Compared to late-onset Alzheimer’s disease, early-onset patients showed decreased metabolism in bilateral precuneus, lateral temporo–parietal and occipital cortices (P50.001 uncorrected). After FWE correction, significant differences remained in left precuneus, bilateral angular gyrus, right middle temporal gyrus and left middle occipital gyrus [P(FWE-corr)50.05]. There were no areas of relative hypometabolism in late-onset versus early-onset Alzheimer’s disease, or in normal controls compared to either patient group (P50.001 uncorrected). Atrophy correction did not appreciably alter the pattern of hypometabolism in early-onset Alzheimer’s disease compared to normal controls (Fig. 3), with significant differences found in bilateral precuneus, angular gyrus and middle temporal gyrus [P(FWE-corr)50.05]. Compared to normal controls, late-onset
patients now showed decreased metabolism in left angular gyrus, left middle temporal gyrus and right hippocampus at P50.001 uncorrected, though these regions did not survive multiple comparisons correction. In the direct patient contrasts, atrophy correction did restrict the regions of relative hypometabolism in early-onset compared to late-onset Alzheimer’s disease (Fig. 3), though significant differences remained in bilateral precuneus and in right angular/supramarginal gyrus [P(FWE-corr)50.05]. Late-onset patients showed lower glucose metabolism than early-onset patients in left frontal trigone, right lateral orbital gyrus, bilateral temporal poles and bilateral fusiform gyri (P50.001 uncorrected), but these differences did not survive FWE correction.
Fluorodeoxyglucose: region of interest comparisons Compared to normal controls, FDG uptake was decreased in early-onset Alzheimer’s disease throughout frontal,
520
| Brain 2010: 133; 512–528
G. D. Rabinovici et al.
Figure 3 Patterns of glucose hypometabolism in early-onset (EO) and late-onset (LO) Alzheimer’s disease (AD) compared to normal controls (NC) and to each other. Voxel-wise comparisons included sex, education and MMSE as nuisance variables. T-score maps prior to atrophy correction are rendered on the ch2 template brain (left), while T-score maps following atrophy correction are rendered on the minimal deformation template 2 template (right). All results are presented at a threshold of P50.001, uncorrected for multiple comparisons.
temporoparietal and occipital regions of interest, while late-onset patients showed lower FDG than normal controls in left posterior cingulate, left lateral temporal cortex and left temporal pole (Table 4). In direct patient contrasts, early-onset Alzheimer’s disease showed lower glucose metabolism in bilateral temporoparietal and occipital regions of interest, while there were no regions of relative decreased metabolism in late-onset Alzheimer’s disease. Following atrophy correction, early-onset Alzheimer’s disease patients continued to show lower metabolism than late-onset patients in left posterior cingulate, while there were again no regions of relative hypometabolism in late-onset Alzheimer’s disease (Table 4). Scatter-plots of FDG uptake in precuneus (pre- and post-atrophy correction) are shown in Fig. 2B.
Pittsburgh compound-B and fluorodeoxyglucose comparisons, excluding Pittsburgh compound-B-negative subjects A number of Alzheimer’s disease patients in our cohort showed low PIB index values, particularly in the late-onset group (Fig. 2A). We wanted to ensure that comparisons between early-onset and late-onset Alzheimer’s disease were not biased by including ‘PIB-negative’ subjects in whom the diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease is questioned (Engler et al., 2006). We therefore classified all Alzheimer’s disease subjects as ‘PIB-positive’ or ‘PIB-negative’ based on a visual read by an experienced PET investigator (WJJ)
blinded to clinical diagnosis (Rabinovici et al., 2007). A ‘PIB-positive’ scan was defined as a distribution volume ratio image in which PIB uptake was greater in cortex or striatum than in white matter. Voxel-wise and region of interest analyses for PIB and FDG were then repeated after excluding all ‘PIB-negative’ Alzheimer’s disease subjects. Based on visual read, all 21 early-onset Alzheimer’s disease subjects were classified ‘PIB-positive’, while four of 18 late-onset subjects were classified ‘PIB-negative’. The four visually negative scans corresponded to the four subjects with the lowest PIB index values in the late-onset group (Fig. 2A and B, no atrophy correction). Excluding these four, subjects had a minor effect on PIB comparisons between early-onset and late-onset Alzheimer’s disease. Prior to atrophy correction, late-onset patients showed higher PIB binding than early-onset patients in a 36 voxel cluster in the left superior parietal lobule [P(FWE-corr)50.05] on voxel-wise analysis, and in bilateral precuneus and lateral parietal cortex on region of interest analysis (P50.05, Supplementary Table 1). However, these differences were not significant following atrophy correction. There were no regions of greater PIB uptake in earlyonset Alzheimer’s disease compared to late-onset Alzheimer’s disease on voxel-wise or region of interest comparisons (Supplementary Table 1). FDG contrasts also did not change appreciably after excluding PIB-negative Alzheimer’s disease subjects. Without atrophy correction, early-onset patients showed lower glucose metabolism than late-onset patients in bilateral inferior parietal lobule, left precuneus, right superior parietal lobule, right angular gyrus and right middle
Age, amyloid and metabolism in Alzheimer’s disease
Brain 2010: 133; 512–528
| 521
Table 4 FDG normalized values in regions of interest before and after atrophy correction FDG
No Atrophy correction
Region
Normal controls (n = 30)
Early-onset Alzheimer’s disease (n = 18)
Late-onset Alzheimer’s disease (n = 16)
P (ANOVA)
Normal controls (n = 30)
Early-onset Alzheimer’s disease (n = 15)
Late-onset Alzheimer’s disease (n = 11)
P (ANOVA)
L frontal R frontal L precuneus R precuneus L posterior cingulate R posterior cingulate L lateral parietal R lateral parietal L lateral temporal R lateral temporal L medial temporal R medial temporal L occipital R occipital L temporal pole R temporal pole L hippocampus R hippocampus L striatum R striatum
1.62 1.62 1.65 1.65 1.65 1.53 1.68 1.67 1.62 1.64 1.48 1.46 1.77 1.78 1.23 1.22 1.13 1.12 1.53 1.49
1.45 1.46 1.30 1.26 1.21 1.14 1.20 1.21 1.25 1.27 1.31 1.30 1.37 1.42 1.13 1.14 1.04 1.03 1.46 1.44
1.54 1.55 1.59 1.59 1.44 1.37 1.51 1.55 1.40 1.49 1.39 1.41 1.62 1.69 1.09 1.12 1.06 1.06 1.59 1.54
0.03a 0.04a _0.0001a,b _0.0001a,b _0.0001a,b,c _0.0001a,b _0.0001a,b _0.0001a,b 50.0001a,c _0.0001a,b 0.01a 0.02a _0.0001a,b _0.0001a,b 0.04c 0.10 0.20 0.18 0.31 0.43
2.21 2.18 2.13 2.03 1.94 1.84 2.22 2.24 2.04 2.05 1.76 1.73 2.15 2.16 1.78 1.82 1.40 1.37 1.84 1.83
2.03 2.03 1.63 1.59 1.44 1.45 1.56 1.61 1.60 1.65 1.57 1.58 1.76 1.82 1.71 1.75 1.25 1.22 1.79 1.80
1.96 1.93 1.81 1.75 1.68 1.56 1.76 1.82 1.66 1.75 1.57 1.57 1.84 1.89 1.53 1.58 1.17 1.14 1.83 1.81
0.01a,c 0.01c 50.0001a,c 50.0001a,c _0.0001a,b,c 50.0001a,c 50.0001a,c 50.0001a,c 50.0001a,c 50.0001a,c 50.0001a,c 0.01a,c 50.0001a,c 0.002a,c 0.006c 0.01c 50.0001a,c 50.0001a,c 0.66 0.89
(0.15) (0.15) (0.17) (0.21) (0.24) (0.23) (0.16) (0.18) (0.17) (0.16) (0.15) (0.15) (0.19) (0.19) (0.13) (0.11) (0.12) (0.11) (0.19) (0.19)
(0.19) (0.17) (0.26) (0.27) (0.26) (0.27) (0.29) (0.26) (0.20) (0.21) (0.19) (0.18) (0.32) (0.33) (0.14) (0.15) (0.10) (0.14) (0.22) (0.18)
Atrophy correction
(0.31) (0.27) (0.24) (0.24) (0.30) (0.28) (0.26) (0.23) (0.31) (0.26) (0.26) (0.26) (0.27) (0.26) (0.28) (0.24) (0.28) (0.28) (0.37) (0.36)
(0.22) (0.24) (0.26) (0.26) (0.25) (0.26) (0.24) (0.29) (0.22) (0.23) (0.18) (0.17) (0.24) (0.26) (0.19) (0.20) (0.13) (0.12) (0.20) (0.21)
(0.22) (0.22) (0.25) (0.28) (0.19) (0.20) (0.32) (0.37) (0.19) (0.27) (0.16) (0.16) (0.34) (0.37) (0.18) (0.21) (0.10) (0.14) (0.17) (0.16)
(0.28) (0.27) (0.33) (0.28) (0.24) (0.22) (0.37) (0.37) (0.30) (0.27) (0.20) (0.19) (0.35) (0.34) (0.31) (0.27) (0.18) (0.17) (0.17) (0.17)
Values are presented as mean (standard deviation). Values are in bold when post hoc analysis revealed significant differences between early-onset and late-onset Alzheimer’s disease. a Early-onset5normal controls, P50.05. b Early-onset5late-onset, P50.05. c Late-onset5normal controls, P50.05.
occipital gyrus on voxel-wise analysis [P(FWE-corr)50.05], and in bilateral posterior cingulate, precuneus, lateral temporoparietal and occipital cortices on region of interest analysis (P50.05, Supplementary Table 2). There were no regions of relatively greater hypometabolism in late-onset Alzheimer’s disease. Following atrophy correction, early-onset patients showed hypometabolism in bilateral precuneus and right angular gyrus [P(FWE-corr)50.05] on voxel-wise analysis, while late-onset patients showed lower glucose metabolism in left temporal pole on voxel-wise comparison [P(FWE-corr)50.05], and in bilateral temporal poles on region of interest analysis (P50.05, Supplementary Table 2).
Relationship between age-at-onset, Pittsburgh compound-B and fluorodeoxyglucose Graphs of age-at-onset as a continuous (rather than dichotomous) variable versus PIB and FDG suggest a positive linear relationship with glucose metabolism (in temporo–parietal regions) but no relationship with global or regional amyloid burden (Fig. 4A and B). To assess this further, we performed a series of multi-linear regressions that included age-at-onset, education and MMSE as independent variables, and regional PIB or FDG uptake as the dependent variable. Standardized correlation coefficients relating
age-at-onset to regional PIB and FDG are shown in Table 5. There was no correlation between age-at-onset and PIB in any regions. In contrast, age-at-onset was positively correlated with FDG in precuneus, lateral parietal and occipital cortices. MMSE was positively correlated with FDG in precuneus (b = 0.36, P = 0.02), posterior cingulate (b = 0.45, P = 0.007), lateral parietal (b = 0.44, P = 0.003) and lateral temporal cortices (b = 0.42, P = 0.02), but did not correlate with PIB in any region. In this highly educated cohort (Table 1), we did not find correlations between education and PIB or FDG in any region. Following atrophy correction, we again did not find a correlation between age-at-onset and PIB in any regions. Positive correlations between age and FDG were weakened [trends in precuneus (b = 0.38, P = 0.07) and posterior cingulate (b = 0.35, P = 0.08)], while significant negative correlations appeared in hippocampus (b = 0.50, P = 0.02) and temporal pole (b = 0.51, P = 0.01).
Effect of apolipoprotein E genotype on Pittsburgh compound-B and fluorodeoxyglucose Apolipoprotein E genotype has been previously reported to affect both PIB binding and glucose metabolism in Alzheimer’s disease, with the E4 genotype associated with higher amyloid load and
522
| Brain 2010: 133; 512–528
G. D. Rabinovici et al.
Figure 4 Relationships between age-at-onset, PIB and FDG. (A) and (B) demonstrate the unadjusted relationships between age-at-onset (as a continuous variable) and (A) PIB or (B) FDG. Regression lines have been added to best fit the data. (C) Plots PIB (x axis) versus FDG (y axis) in precuneus. Reference lines denoting one standard deviation above normal controls mean for PIB (vertical line), and one standard deviation below the normal controls mean for FDG (horizontal line) have been added to divide the data into quadrants. See text for discussion. DVR = distribution volume ratio; EO = early-onset; LO = late-onset; AD = Alzheimer’s disease; CONT = controls.
ships between ApoE genotype, PIB and FDG in our Alzheimer’s
Effect of age on the local relationship between Pittsburgh compound-B and fluorodeoxyglucose
disease cohort. Patients were dichotomized as E4 carriers (with
The relationship between amyloid load and glucose metabolism
one or two E4 alleles) and non-carriers for all analyses. Results
in precuneus is demonstrated in Fig. 4C. Precuneus is chosen
are presented in Supplementary Table 3. Prior to atrophy correction we found a surprising trend for lower global and regional amyloid load in ApoE4 carriers compared to
for illustrative purposes, with similar data scatter found in posterior
non-carriers, though this was only significant in lateral temporal
ation above the normal control mean for PIB, and one standard
cortex (P = 0.04). Following atrophy correction there were no sig-
deviation below the normal control mean for FDG. Nearly all normal controls fell into the low PIB/high FDG quadrant, though two fell into the high PIB/high FDG and one into the low PIB/low FDG quadrants. Three of the four PIB-negative patients with late-onset Alzheimer’s disease fell into the low PIB/ low FDG quadrant. In the high PIB quadrants there was a clear divide based on age-of-onset, with most early-onset patients
lower glucose metabolism (Drzezga et al., 2005, 2009). ApoE is also a major risk factor for developing Alzheimer’s disease at an early age (Farrer et al., 1997). We thus investigated the relation-
nificant differences in PIB binding between patients with and without the E4 allele. As expected, patients with an E4 allele showed lower glucose metabolism than non-carriers in posterior cortical regions (Supplementary Table 3), with significant differences found (following atrophy-correction only) in posterior cingulate (P = 0.02) and a trend found in precuneus (P = 0.06).
cingulate, lateral parietal and occipital cortices. The graph is divided into four quadrants, with lines denoting one standard devi-
Age, amyloid and metabolism in Alzheimer’s disease
Table 5 Results of multi-linear regressions Region
PIB (n = 39)
FDG (n = 34)
PIB index Frontal cortex Precuneus Posterior cingulate cortex Lateral parietal cortex Lateral temporal cortex Medial temporal cortex Occipital cortex Temporal pole Hippocampus Striatum
b = 0.08, P = 0.65 b = 0.08, P = 0.62 b = 0.15, P = 0.37 b = 0.10, P = 0.53 b = 0.23, P = 0.17 b = 0.03, P = 0.86 b = 0.19, P = 0.27 b = 0.01, P = 0.95 b = 0.19, P = 0.26 b = 0.21, P = 0.22 b = 0.13, P = 0.45
– b = 0.02, P = 0.91 b = 0.48, P = 0.002 b = 0.26, P = 0.11 b = 0.45, P = 0.002 b = 0.21, P = 0.20 b = 0.14, P = 0.43 b = 0.37, P = 0.03 b = 0.26, P = 0.14 b = 0.06, P = 0.74 b = 0.12, P = 0.51
b values represent standardized correlation coefficients relating age-at-onset to PIB and FDG after co-varying MMSE and education. Significant correlations are in bold.
falling into the high PIB/low FDG category, and most late-onset patients found in the high PIB/high FDG quadrant (Fig. 4C). At an individual level, early-onset patients tended to show more severe hypometabolism for a given amyloid burden than late-onset subjects. When all subjects were included we found a trend for a negative correlation between PIB and FDG in precuneus (r = 0.24, P = 0.06). However, when normal controls were excluded, we found no correlation between PIB and FDG in patients with Alzheimer’s disease when tested together (r = 0.11, P = 0.52) or separately as early-onset (r = 0.04, P = 0.87) or late-onset (r = 0.18, P = 0.51) Alzheimer’s disease. To determine which factors may explain lower glucose metabolism in early-onset patients, we performed step-wise regressions in which regional FDG was the dependent variable. Candidate independent variables included age-at-onset and MMSE (based on our previous regressions, Table 5), and three new variables: regional PIB, ApoE genotype (dichotomized into E4 carriers and non-carriers), and an interaction term between age-at-onset and ApoE (to exclude an age-specific effect of ApoE on glucose metabolism) (Hirono et al., 2002). Regressions were performed in precuneus, lateral parietal and occipital cortices, regions in which we previously found an effect of age-at-onset on FDG (Table 5). The analysis was performed using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences’ default step-wise regression settings [P(Fenter) 4 0.05, P(Fremove) 5 0.10]. Age-at-onset (b = 0.51, P = 0.002) and MMSE (b = 0.37, P = 0.02) predicted FDG in precuneus, while PIB (P = 0.52), ApoE (P = 0.34) and the interaction between age and ApoE (P = 0.40) were eliminated from the model. Similarly, age-at-onset (b = 0.44, P = 0.004) and MMSE (b = 0.47, P = 0.003) predicted FDG in lateral parietal cortex, while PIB (P = 0.72), ApoE (P = 0.75) and the interaction term (P = 0.86) were eliminated. Finally, age-at-onset (b = 0.49, P = 0.009) predicted occipital FDG, while MMSE (P = 0.18), PIB (P = 0.34), ApoE (P = 0.43) and the interaction term (P = 0.44) were excluded from the model.
Brain 2010: 133; 512–528
| 523
Discussion In this study we applied PIB and FDG-PET to investigate in vivo the relationships between age-at-onset, fibrillar amyloid-b and glucose metabolism in Alzheimer’s disease. We hypothesized that patients with early-onset (age 565) disease would show increased cortical PIB binding compared to late-onset (age 465) patients, and that this would be accompanied by more severe temporoparietal hypometabolism in younger patients. Contrary to our hypothesis, we found no significant effect of age on the burden or distribution of PIB binding, either when directly comparing patients with early-onset and late-onset disease, or when studying age-at-onset as a continuous variable. In contrast, congruent with our a priori hypothesis, we found more severe metabolic deficits in temporoparietal and occipital cortices in patients with early-onset Alzheimer’s disease compared to matched late-onset patients. We further found that in a multi-linear model, glucose metabolism in posterior brain regions was positively correlated with age-at-onset and with degree of cognitive impairment (as measured by the MMSE), but not with amyloid burden or apoE genotype. Current theories on the pathogenesis of early-onset Alzheimer’s disease have emphasized increased deposition of both amyloid-b and tau pathology as the primary mechanism for the greater functional and structural brain changes and more rapid decline seen in young patients. This hypothesis hinges on observations of greater pathologic burden in early-onset patients at the time of autopsy. In contrast, our finding of similar PIB binding regardless of age-at-onset suggests that increased amyloid-b deposition alone cannot explain the clinical differences between early and late-onset disease. Rather, our data are consistent with a model in which both early amyloid-b accumulation and increased vulnerability to pathology play critical roles in the pathogenesis of Alzheimer’s disease in young patients. Amyloid-b plaques are relatively uncommon in cognitively normal individuals before the seventh decade of life (Mintun et al., 2006; Kok et al., 2009; Morris et al., 2009), and are strongly correlated with dementia in younger individuals (Savva et al., 2009). Indeed, all patients with early-onset Alzheimer’s disease in our study showed evidence of elevated PIB binding compared to a group of older normal controls, suggesting that early amyloid-b accumulation is an important initial pathogenic step in early-onset Alzheimer’s disease (Figs 1 and 2A). Early-onset patients further showed more severe metabolic deficits in posterior brain regions compared to late-onset patients faced with similar amyloid burden (Figs 3 and 4C). One interpretation of this result is that younger patients have an increased susceptibility to the neurotoxic effects of fibrillar amyloid-b. However, there are a number of alternative explanations. In this cross-sectional study we cannot rule out the possibility that duration of exposure to amyloid-b, rather than absolute amyloid-b burden, leads to more severe hypometabolism in early-onset Alzheimer’s disease. Longitudinal PIB studies suggest that amyloid aggregation reaches a relative plateau early in the disease course (perhaps even in the mild cognitive impairment stage or earlier), while glucose metabolism and brain volume decline in concordance with disease
524
| Brain 2010: 133; 512–528
progression (Engler et al., 2006; Jack et al., 2009). It is thus possible that early-onset patients have a longer biological prodrome than late-onset patients, during which significant plaques are present without clinical symptoms, perhaps as a result of higher cognitive reserve in young brains. By the time symptoms are present, young patients have more widespread atrophy and hypometabolism than older patients, reflecting both the longer duration of exposure to amyloid-b and the more diffuse neuronal dysfunction necessary to cross the threshold into clinical dementia. However, this model would not account for a number of early-onset patients who show severe metabolic disruption in the face of a moderate amyloid burden, well below the typical ‘PIB saturation’ level (Fig. 4C). It is also possible that differences between early-onset and late-onset Alzheimer’s disease may be explained by pathology that is not imaged by PIB. For example, amyloid-b oligomers, considered by many to be the most neurotoxic of all amyloid-b species (Mucke et al., 2000; Walsh and Selkoe, 2007), are not detected by PIB, which binds to amyloid-b aggregates in direct relation to their fibrillar content (Klunk et al., 2003). Though speculative, one might even consider whether the apparent increased pathogenicity of amyloid-b in early-onset disease may be related to ‘inefficient fibrillization’, with patients developing symptoms at a young age in part because of an inability to sequester highly toxic soluble amyloid-b oligomers effectively into relatively less toxic insoluble plaques. Further support for a potential ‘protective’ effect of fibrillar amyloid-b can be found in our voxel-wise analyses, where trends for higher amyloid were seen for each group in regions of relative metabolic sparing, namely anteromedial temporal cortex for early-onset Alzheimer’s disease and parietal cortex for late-onset Alzheimer’s disease (compare direct patient contrasts in Figs 1 and 3). However, differences in PIB binding in these regions did not meet our conservative significance threshold for voxel-wise comparisons, were not reproduced in region of interest data, and may also have been affected by differential atrophy patterns (greater parietal atrophy in early-onset Alzheimer’s disease and medial temporal atrophy in late-onset Alzheimer’s disease), even after applying partial volume correction. Therefore, we cannot confidently conclude that our data support an inverse association between amyloid burden and glucose metabolism in these regions. Differences between early-onset and late-onset Alzheimer’s disease may also be explained by a differential burden of neurofibrillary tangles, which do not appreciably bind PIB at PET tracer concentrations (Lockhart et al., 2007; Ikonomovic et al., 2008). Indeed, tangle pathology correlates more strongly with age-at-onset than amyloid-b plaques in most post-mortem studies (Hansen et al., 1988; Nochlin et al., 1993; Berg et al., 1998; Marshall et al., 2007). Whether and how amyloid-b and tau aggregates are related in Alzheimer’s disease remains an area of active debate. Finally, clinical and metabolic differences may in part be explained by greater neurochemical deficits in younger patients that occur independently of cortical amyloid-b deposition (Bird et al., 1983; Rossor et al., 1984). The apolipoprotein E4 allele is the strongest known genetic risk factor for both early-onset and late-onset Alzheimer’s disease (Farrer et al., 1997), and was present in 74% of early-onset and
G. D. Rabinovici et al. 50% of late-onset Alzheimer’s disease patients compared to 30% of normal controls in our study (P = 0.02). ApoE4 directly promotes amyloid-b fibrillization, but may also contribute to Alzheimer’s disease pathogenesis through a variety of other mechanisms, including promotion of tau phosphorylation, direct mitochondrial toxicity, decreased cognitive reserve and impaired neuronal response to injury (Mahley et al., 2006). The ApoE4 allele decreases age-at-onset in Alzheimer’s disease in a dose-dependant fashion (Farrer et al., 1997; Meyer et al., 1998). Recent autopsy and PIB studies of cognitively normal individuals demonstrate that ApoE4 is strongly associated with amyloid-b deposits between ages 40 and 90 years, and plaque pathology in mid-life is seen almost exclusively in ApoE4 carriers (Kok et al., 2009; Morris et al., 2009; Reiman et al., 2009). Thus, ApoE4 may contribute to the early-onset of Alzheimer’s disease by predisposing carriers to overproduce and deposit amyloid-b at a young age. We did not find evidence for increased PIB binding in Alzheimer’s disease patients with an ApoE4 genotype, and actually found a trend for lower binding in E4 carriers in temporoparietal cortex, though this was ameliorated by atrophy correction (Supplementary Table 3). Our study thus adds to the ambiguity in the literature, where increased amyloid deposition in ApoE4 carriers has been found (on autopsy or with PIB-PET) in some studies of Alzheimer’s disease (Gomez-Isla et al., 1996; Drzezga et al., 2009) but not others (Berg et al., 1998; Rowe et al., 2007). Similar to previous reports (Drzezga et al., 2005), we found lower glucose metabolism in precuneus/posterior cingulate cortex in Alzheimer’s disease patients carrying an ApoE4 allele (Supplementary Table 3). However, step-wise regression did not identify ApoE genotype as a significant predictor of glucose metabolism after controlling for age-at-onset and MMSE. Thus, our findings suggest that ApoE4 acts as a risk factor for early-onset Alzheimer’s disease primarily by predisposing towards early accumulation of fibrillar amyloid-b, rather than by increasing the overall burden of amyloid-b or by accentuating the metabolic response to pathology. Our data highlight the precuneus as one of the most metabolically vulnerable regions in early-onset Alzheimer’s disease (Figs 2–4, Tables 4 and 5). The precuneus/posterior cingulate complex is a central cortical ‘hub’ that is structurally and functionally interconnected with other heteromodal association regions in lateral parietal, temporal and prefrontal cortices (Buckner et al., 2009; Greicius et al., 2009). The precuneus/posterior cingulate appears to play a central role in episodic memory processes, with functional MRI experiments demonstrating deactivation during successful stimulus encoding, and activation during episodic memory retrieval (Daselaar et al., 2004; Wagner et al., 2005; Svoboda et al., 2006). PIB-PET studies of ageing have identified the precuneus/posterior cingulate as one of the earliest sites of amyloid-b deposition (Mintun et al., 2006; Mormino et al., 2009; Reiman et al., 2009), with elevated PIB binding associated with decreased precuneus/posterior cingulate connectivity at rest (Hedden et al., 2009; Sheline et al., 2009), and aberrantly increased precuneus/posterior cingulate activation during encoding (Sperling et al., 2009). The precuneus/posterior cingulate is also a site of early metabolic disruption in Alzheimer’s disease (Minoshima et al., 1997), with hypometabolism apparent even in asymptomatic ApoE4 gene carriers (Reiman et al., 1996).
Age, amyloid and metabolism in Alzheimer’s disease Buckner and others have observed that amyloid-b pathology and functional and structural changes in Alzheimer’s disease all converge in the precuneus/posterior cingulate, and have hypothesized that this may be related to high neuronal activity in the precuneus/posterior cingulate due its inter-connectivity and frequent fluctuation between an activated and deactivated state. This elevated level of activity may predispose the precuneus/posterior cingulate to early amyloid-b aggregation (extrapolating from in vitro data that link amyloid-b release to synaptic activity) (Cirrito et al., 2005) and render it more vulnerable to amyloid-b neurotoxicity due to high metabolic stress (Buckner et al., 2005, 2009; Liang et al., 2008). In our study patients with early-onset and late-onset Alzheimer’s disease showed comparably high levels of amyloid-b deposition in the precuneus/posterior cingulate (Fig. 1), but hypometabolism was markedly more severe in early-onset patients, suggesting that metabolic vulnerability in the precuneus/posterior cingulate may be particularly relevant to the pathogenesis of Alzheimer’s disease in young patients (Figs 3 and 4). Further studies are needed to identify factors that may increase metabolic susceptibility in early-onset Alzheimer’s disease, such as potential genetic variations in key metabolic enzymes and pathways (Li et al., 2004; Liang et al., 2008). Four clinically diagnosed Alzheimer’s disease patients in our study, all with late-onset disease, did not show evidence of elevated PIB binding (Fig. 2A). Patients with PIB-negative Alzheimer’s disease have been reported in previous series, and are likely to represent a combination of clinical misdiagnosis and true ‘falsenegatives’ related to failure of PIB to detect amyloid-b pathology (Klunk et al., 2004; Edison et al., 2007; Leinonen et al., 2008; Rosen et al., 2009). Clinically, all four patients met research criteria for Alzheimer’s disease with a progressive amnestic disorder, though one had very mild symptoms and deficits on cognitive testing, one patient had a prominent aphasia and another developed significant behavioural disturbances, albeit in the moderate stage of dementia. The fourth patient was felt to have a typical Alzheimer’s disease clinical course. Excluding these four patients did not appreciably alter the results of PIB or FDG comparisons between early-onset and late-onset Alzheimer’s disease (Supplementary Tables 1 and 2). Our finding of similar PIB binding in patients with early-onset and late-onset Alzheimer’s disease deviates from reports of greater amyloid-b plaque burden at autopsy in early-onset disease (Mann et al., 1984; Hansen et al., 1988; Nochlin et al., 1993; Bigio et al., 2002; Ho et al., 2002; Marshall et al., 2007). This incongruity may be accounted for by significant methodological differences between our study and previous post-mortem work. By virtue of applying an in vivo technique, we were able to select subjects in the mild-to-moderate stage of Alzheimer’s disease, while most autopsies were conducted in patients in the late stages of dementia. We were able to match patients closely for dementia severity, while in autopsy studies older patients are more likely to have died at an earlier disease stage (Berg et al., 1998; Ho et al., 2002). We also excluded patients with evidence of co-morbid pathology, including clinical features of another dementia syndrome or significant vascular lesions on MRI. To our knowledge previous autopsy studies did not apply such strict exclusion criteria, again introducing potential bias since late-onset patients are more
Brain 2010: 133; 512–528
| 525
likely to suffer from co-pathology (Schneider et al., 2007). To measure amyloid-b burden, post-mortem studies typically applied semi-quantitative measures (Nochlin et al., 1993; Bigio et al., 2002) and rarely differentiated between different plaque morphologies (Ho et al., 2002; Marshall et al., 2007). PIB binding, on the other hand, quantifies amyloid-b pathology in direct relation to its fibrillar content, with stronger binding to more mature plaques. Our results are most comparable to the those of Berg and colleagues, who found that total plaques correlated negatively with age throughout cortex and hippocampus, but found more restricted correlations when the analysis was limited to cored plaques with high fibrillar content (Berg et al., 1998). Finally, the greater degree of cortical atrophy in early-onset patients would tend to confound PIB and autopsy comparisons of early-onset and late-onset Alzheimer’s disease in opposite directions, decreasing PET signal but artificially increasing lesion density in pathologic specimens from younger patients. Notably, our results are consistent with another in vivo biomarker study that found no difference in cerebrospinal fluid levels of amyloid-b1–42, tau and phosphorylated tau in patients with mild-to-moderate early-onset and late-onset Alzheimer’s disease (Bouwman et al., 2009). Thus, both biomarker studies comparing early-onset and late-onset Alzheimer’s disease raise doubts about whether previous postmortem findings truly reflect the effects of age-at-onset on Alzheimer’s disease pathogenesis in vivo. Our study has limitations. PIB-PET is a relatively new technique, and although early clinicopathologic studies suggest that in vivo PIB signal correlates strongly with post-mortem measures of amyloid-b (Bacskai et al., 2007; Ikonomovic et al., 2008), the limitations of PIB have not yet been fully defined. For example, two recent case reports suggest that PIB may have reduced binding affinity for fibrillar amyloid in rare individuals (Leinonen et al., 2008; Rosen et al., 2009). We attempted to control for two possible confounders related to the technique. We confirmed that PIB binding did not differ between early-onset and late-onset patients in the cerebellum, which is the reference tissue used to normalize PIB counts across subjects. To account for PET signal loss due to atrophy, we confirmed the results of all PET analyses with atrophy-corrected data, though this analysis was limited to the subset of patients that had a recent research-protocol MRI. However, we cannot exclude the possibility that currently unknown limitations of PIB (e.g. a ceiling effect on tracer binding, age-related effects on binding site availability, etc.) may have influenced our results. Finally, although our study is one of the largest PIB studies in Alzheimer’s disease to date, the size of our early-onset and late-onset groups was modest. However, our sample size was adequate to detect significant effects of age on FDG uptake, and is comparable to many autopsy studies comparing early-onset and late-onset Alzheimer’s disease (Mann et al., 1984; Hansen et al., 1988; Nochlin et al., 1993; Bigio et al., 2002; Marshall et al., 2007). In summary, using PIB and FDG-PET we found similar amyloid burden but greater posterior cortical metabolic deficits in patients with early-onset Alzheimer’s disease compared to late-onset patients matched for disease severity. Our results suggest that both early amyloid-b accumulation and increased vulnerability to pathology act in complement to cause Alzheimer’s disease in
526
| Brain 2010: 133; 512–528
young patients. Both amyloid-b production and susceptibility to pathology are likely to be governed by a multitude of genetic and environmental factors that interact with each other, modulating an individual’s risk of developing clinical Alzheimer’s disease over the course of the life span.
Acknowledgements The authors would like to thank Matthew Growdon, Amynta Hayenga, Jung Jang, Anna Karydas, Cindee Madison and Irene Yen for administrative and technical support and Giovanni Coppola for ApoE genotyping.
Funding National Institute on Aging [grant numbers K23-AG031861 to G.D.R, R01-AG027859 to W.J.J, P01-AG1972403 and P50-AG023501 to B.L.M.]; Alzheimer’s Association [grant numbers NIRG-07-59422 to G.D.R. and ZEN-08-87090 to W.J.J]; John Douglas French Alzheimer’s Foundation [to G.D.R.] and State of California Department of Health Services Alzheimer’s Disease Research Center of California [grant number 04-33516 to B.L.M].
Supplementary material Supplementary material is available at Brain online.
References Agosta F, Vossel KA, Miller BL, Migliaccio R, Bonasera SJ, Filippi M, et al. Apolipoprotein E epsilon4 is associated with disease-specific effects on brain atrophy in Alzheimer’s disease and frontotemporal dementia. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2009; 106: 2018–22. Bacskai BJ, Frosch MP, Freeman SH, Raymond SB, Augustinack JC, Johnson KA, et al. Molecular imaging with Pittsburgh Compound B confirmed at autopsy: a case report. Arch Neurol 2007; 64: 431–4. Barclay LL, Zemcov A, Blass JP, McDowell FH. Factors associated with duration of survival in Alzheimer’s disease. Biol Psychiatry 1985; 20: 86–93. Berg L, McKeel DW Jr, Miller JP, Storandt M, Rubin EH, Morris JC, et al. Clinicopathologic studies in cognitively healthy aging and Alzheimer’s disease: relation of histologic markers to dementia severity, age, sex, and apolipoprotein E genotype. Arch Neurol 1998; 55: 326–35. Bigio EH, Hynan LS, Sontag E, Satumtira S, White CL. Synapse loss is greater in presenile than senile onset Alzheimer disease: implications for the cognitive reserve hypothesis. Neuropathol Appl Neurobiol 2002; 28: 218–27. Binetti G, Magni E, Padovani A, Cappa SF, Bianchetti A, Trabucchi M. Neuropsychological heterogeneity in mild Alzheimer’s disease. Dementia 1993; 4: 321–6. Bird TD, Stranahan S, Sumi SM, Raskind M. Alzheimer’s disease: choline acetyltransferase activity in brain tissue from clinical and pathological subgroups. Ann Neurol 1983; 14: 284–93. Bouwman FH, Schoonenboom NS, Verwey NA, van Elk EJ, Kok A, Blankenstein MA, et al. CSF biomarker levels in early and late onset Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol Aging 2009; 30: 1895–901. Braak H, Braak E. Neuropathological stageing of Alzheimer-related changes. Acta Neuropathol 1991; 82: 239–59.
G. D. Rabinovici et al. Buckner RL, Sepulcre J, Talukdar T, Krienen FM, Liu H, Hedden T, et al. Cortical hubs revealed by intrinsic functional connectivity: mapping, assessment of stability, and relation to Alzheimer’s disease. J Neurosci 2009; 29: 1860–73. Buckner RL, Snyder AZ, Shannon BJ, LaRossa G, Sachs R, Fotenos AF, et al. Molecular, structural, and functional characterization of Alzheimer’s disease: evidence for a relationship between default activity, amyloid, and memory. J Neurosci 2005; 25: 7709–17. Cirrito JR, Yamada KA, Finn MB, Sloviter RS, Bales KR, May PC, et al. Synaptic activity regulates interstitial fluid amyloid-beta levels in vivo. Neuron 2005; 48: 913–22. Daselaar SM, Prince SE, Cabeza R. When less means more: deactivations during encoding that predict subsequent memory. Neuroimage 2004; 23: 921–7. Drzezga A, Grimmer T, Henriksen G, Muhlau M, Perneczky R, Miederer I, et al. Effect of APOE genotype on amyloid plaque load and gray matter volume in Alzheimer disease. Neurology 2009; 72: 1487–94. Drzezga A, Riemenschneider M, Strassner B, Grimmer T, Peller M, Knoll A, et al. Cerebral glucose metabolism in patients with AD and different APOE genotypes. Neurology 2005; 64: 102–7. Edison P, Archer HA, Hinz R, Hammers A, Pavese N, Tai YF, et al. Amyloid, hypometabolism, and cognition in Alzheimer disease: an [11C]PIB and [18F]FDG PET study. Neurology 2007; 68: 501–8. Engler H, Forsberg A, Almkvist O, Blomquist G, Larsson E, Savitcheva I, et al. Two-year follow-up of amyloid deposition in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Brain 2006; 129: 2856–66. Farrer LA, Cupples LA, Haines JL, Hyman B, Kukull WA, Mayeux R, et al. Effects of age, sex, and ethnicity on the association between apolipoprotein E genotype and Alzheimer disease. A meta-analysis. APOE and Alzheimer Disease Meta Analysis Consortium. JAMA 1997; 278: 1349–56. Filley CM, Kelly J, Heaton RK. Neuropsychologic features of early- and late-onset Alzheimer’s disease. Arch Neurol 1986; 43: 574–6. Frisoni GB, Pievani M, Testa C, Sabattoli F, Bresciani L, Bonetti M, et al. The topography of grey matter involvement in early and late onset Alzheimer’s disease. Brain 2007; 130: 720–30. Frisoni GB, Testa C, Sabattoli F, Beltramello A, Soininen H, Laakso MP. Structural correlates of early and late onset Alzheimer’s disease: voxel based morphometric study. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2005; 76: 112–4. Fujimori M, Imamura T, Yamashita H, Hirono N, Ikejiri Y, Shimomura T, et al. Age at onset and visuocognitive disturbances in Alzheimer disease. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 1998; 12: 163–6. Gomez-Isla T, West HL, Rebeck GW, Harr SD, Growdon JH, Locascio JJ, et al. Clinical and pathological correlates of apolipoprotein E epsilon 4 in Alzheimer’s disease. Ann Neurol 1996; 39: 62–70. Grady CL, Haxby JV, Horwitz B, Berg G, Rapoport SI. Neuropsychological and cerebral metabolic function in early vs late onset dementia of the Alzheimer type. Neuropsychologia 1987; 25: 807–16. Greicius MD, Supekar K, Menon V, Dougherty RF. Resting-state functional connectivity reflects structural connectivity in the default mode network. Cereb Cortex 2009; 19: 72–8. Hansen LA, DeTeresa R, Davies P, Terry RD. Neocortical morphometry, lesion counts, and choline acetyltransferase levels in the age spectrum of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 1988; 38: 48–54. Hedden T, Van Dijk KR, Becker JA, Mehta A, Sperling RA, Johnson KA, et al. Disruption of functional connectivity in clinically normal older adults harboring amyloid burden. J Neurosci 2009; 29: 12686–94. Heyman A, Wilkinson WE, Hurwitz BJ, et al. Early-onset Alzheimer’s disease: clinical predictors of institutionalisation and death. Neurology 1987; 27: 980–984. Hirono N, Hashimoto M, Yasuda M, Ishii K, Sakamoto S, Kazui H, et al. The effect of APOE epsilon4 allele on cerebral glucose metabolism in AD is a function of age at onset. Neurology 2002; 58: 743–50. Ho GJ, Hansen LA, Alford MF, Foster K, Salmon DP, Galasko D, et al. Age at onset is associated with disease severity in Lewy body variant and Alzheimer’s disease. Neuroreport 2002; 13: 1825–8.
Age, amyloid and metabolism in Alzheimer’s disease Ikonomovic MD, Klunk WE, Abrahamson EE, Mathis CA, Price JC, Tsopelas ND, et al. Post-mortem correlates of in vivo PiB-PET amyloid imaging in a typical case of Alzheimer’s disease. Brain 2008; 131: 1630–45. Imamura T, Takatsuki Y, Fujimori M, Hirono N, Ikejiri Y, Shimomura T, et al. Age at onset and language disturbances in Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychologia 1998; 36: 945–9. Ishii K, Kawachi T, Sasaki H, Kono AK, Fukuda T, Kojima Y, et al. Voxelbased morphometric comparison between early- and late-onset mild Alzheimer’s disease and assessment of diagnostic performance of z score images. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol 2005; 26: 333–40. Jack CR Jr, Lowe VJ, Weigand SD, Wiste HJ, Senjem ML, Knopman DS, et al. Serial PIB and MRI in normal, mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease: implications for sequence of pathological events in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain 2009; 132: 1355–65. Jacobs D, Sano M, Marder K, Bell K, Bylsma F, Lafleche G, et al. Age at onset of Alzheimer’s disease: relation to pattern of cognitive dysfunction and rate of decline. Neurology 1994; 44: 1215–20. Jagust WJ, Reed BR, Seab JP, Budinger TF. Alzheimer’s disease: age at onset and single-photon emission computed tomographic patterns of regional cerebral blood flow. Arch Neurol 1990; 47: 628–33. Joachim CL, Morris JH, Selkoe DJ. Diffuse senile plaques occur commonly in the cerebellum in Alzheimer’s disease. Am J Pathol 1989; 135: 309–19. Karas G, Scheltens P, Rombouts S, van Schijndel R, Klein M, Jones B, et al. Precuneus atrophy in early-onset Alzheimer’s disease: a morphometric structural MRI study. Neuroradiology 2007; 49: 967–76. Kemp PM, Holmes C, Hoffmann SM, Bolt L, Holmes R, Rowden J, et al. Alzheimer’s disease: differences in technetium-99m HMPAO SPECT scan findings between early onset and late onset dementia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2003; 74: 715–9. Kemppainen NM, Aalto S, Wilson IA, Nagren K, Helin S, Bruck A, et al. Voxel-based analysis of PET amyloid ligand [11C]PIB uptake in Alzheimer disease. Neurology 2006; 67: 1575–80. Kim EJ, Cho SS, Jeong Y, Park KC, Kang SJ, Kang E, et al. Glucose metabolism in early onset versus late onset Alzheimer’s disease: an SPM analysis of 120 patients. Brain 2005; 128: 1790–801. Klein GJ, Teng X, Jagust WJ, Eberling JL, Acharya A, Reutter BW, et al. A methodology for specifying PET VOI’s using multimodality techniques. IEEE Trans Med Imaging 1997; 16: 405–15. Klunk WE, Engler H, Nordberg A, Wang Y, Blomqvist G, Holt DP, et al. Imaging brain amyloid in Alzheimer’s disease with Pittsburgh Compound-B. Ann Neurol 2004; 55: 306–19. Klunk WE, Wang Y, Huang GF, Debnath ML, Holt DP, Shao L, et al. The binding of 2-(4’-methylaminophenyl)benzothiazole to postmortem brain homogenates is dominated by the amyloid component. J Neurosci 2003; 23: 2086–92. Knopman DS, Petersen RC, Edland SD, Cha RH, Rocca WA. The incidence of frontotemporal lobar degeneration in Rochester, Minnesota, 1990 through 1994. Neurology 2004; 62: 506–8. Kok E, Haikonen S, Luoto T, Huhtala H, Goebeler S, Haapasalo H, et al. Apolipoprotein E-dependent accumulation of Alzheimer disease-related lesions begins in middle age. Ann Neurol 2009; 65: 650–7. Koss E, Edland S, Fillenbaum G, Mohs R, Clark C, Galasko D, et al. Clinical and neuropsychological differences between patients with earlier and later onset of Alzheimer’s disease: A CERAD analysis, Part XII. Neurology 1996; 46: 136–41. Kramer JH, Jurik J, Sha SJ, Rankin KP, Rosen HJ, Johnson JK, et al. Distinctive neuropsychological patterns in frontotemporal dementia, semantic dementia, and Alzheimer disease. Cogn Behav Neurol 2003; 16: 211–8. Leinonen V, Alafuzoff I, Aalto S, Suotunen T, Savolainen S, Nagren K, et al. Assessment of beta-amyloid in a frontal cortical brain biopsy specimen and by positron emission tomography with carbon 11-labeled Pittsburgh Compound B. Arch Neurol 2008; 65: 1304–9. Li Y, Nowotny P, Holmans P, Smemo S, Kauwe JS, Hinrichs AL, et al. Association of late-onset Alzheimer’s disease with genetic variation in
Brain 2010: 133; 512–528
| 527
multiple members of the GAPD gene family. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2004; 101: 15688–93. Liang WS, Reiman EM, Valla J, Dunckley T, Beach TG, Grover A, et al. Alzheimer’s disease is associated with reduced expression of energy metabolism genes in posterior cingulate neurons. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2008; 105: 4441–6. Lockhart A, Lamb JR, Osredkar T, Sue LI, Joyce JN, Ye L, et al. PIB is a non-specific imaging marker of amyloid-beta (Abeta) peptide-related cerebral amyloidosis. Brain 2007; 130: 2607–15. Longstreth WT Jr, Manolio TA, Arnold A, Burke GL, Bryan N, Jungreis CA, et al. Clinical correlates of white matter findings on cranial magnetic resonance imaging of 3301 elderly people. The Cardiovascular Health Study. Stroke 1996; 27: 1274–82. Lopresti BJ, Klunk WE, Mathis CA, Hoge JA, Ziolko SK, Lu X, et al. Simplified quantification of Pittsburgh Compound B amyloid imaging PET studies: a comparative analysis. J Nucl Med 2005; 46: 1959–72. Loring DW, Largen JW. Neuropsychological patterns of presenile and senile dementia of the Alzheimer type. Neuropsychologia 1985; 23: 351–7. Mahley RW, Weisgraber KH, Huang Y. Apolipoprotein E4: a causative factor and therapeutic target in neuropathology, including Alzheimer’s disease. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2006; 103: 5644–51. Mann DM, Yates PO, Marcyniuk B. Alzheimer’s presenile dementia, senile dementia of Alzheimer type and Down’s syndrome in middle age form an age related continuum of pathological changes. Neuropathol Appl Neurobiol 1984; 10: 185–207. Marshall GA, Fairbanks LA, Tekin S, Vinters HV, Cummings JL. Earlyonset Alzheimer’s disease is associated with greater pathologic burden. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 2007; 20: 29–33. Mathis CA, Wang Y, Holt DP, Huang GF, Debnath ML, Klunk WE. Synthesis and evaluation of 11C-labeled 6-substituted 2-arylbenzothiazoles as amyloid imaging agents. J Med Chem 2003; 46: 2740–54. McKhann G, Drachman DA, Folstein M, Katzman R, Price D, Stadlan EM. Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: report of the NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer’s Disease. Neurology 1984; 34: 939–44. Meltzer CC, Kinahan PE, Greer PJ, Nichols TE, Comtat C, Cantwell MN, et al. Comparative evaluation of MR-based partial-volume correction schemes for PET. J Nucl Med 1999; 40: 2053–65. Meltzer CC, Leal JP, Mayberg HS, Wagner HN Jr, Frost JJ. Correction of PET data for partial volume effects in human cerebral cortex by MR imaging. J Comput Assist Tomogr 1990; 14: 561–70. Mercy L, Hodges JR, Dawson K, Barker RA, Brayne C. Incidence of earlyonset dementias in Cambridgeshire, United Kingdom. Neurology 2008; 71: 1496–9. Meyer MR, Tschanz JT, Norton MC, Welsh-Bohmer KA, Steffens DC, Wyse BW, et al. APOE genotype predicts when – not whether – one is predisposed to develop Alzheimer disease. Nat Genet 1998; 19: 321–2. Minoshima S, Frey KA, Foster NL, Kuhl DE. Preserved pontine glucose metabolism in Alzheimer disease: a reference region for functional brain image (PET) analysis. J Comput Assist Tomogr 1995; 19: 541–7. Minoshima S, Giordani B, Berent S, Frey KA, Foster NL, Kuhl DE. Metabolic reduction in the posterior cingulate cortex in very early Alzheimer’s disease. Annals of Neurology 1997; 42: 85–94. Mintun MA, Larossa GN, Sheline YI, Dence CS, Lee SY, Mach RH, et al. [11C]PIB in a nondemented population: potential antecedent marker of Alzheimer disease. Neurology 2006; 67: 446–52. Mirra SS, Heyman A, McKeel D, Sumi SM, Crain BJ, Brownlee LM, et al. The Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease (CERAD). Part II. Standardization of the neuropathologic assessment of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 1991; 41: 479–86. Mormino EC, Kluth JT, Madison CM, Rabinovici GD, Baker SL, Miller BL, et al. Episodic memory loss is related to hippocampal-mediated betaamyloid deposition in elderly subjects. Brain 2009; 132: 1310–23. Morris JC. The Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR): current version and scoring rules [see comments]. Neurology 1993; 43: 2412–4.
528
| Brain 2010: 133; 512–528
Morris JC, Roe CM, Xiong C, Fagan AM, Holtzman DM, Mintun MA. APOE predicts abeta but not tau Alzheimer’s pathology in cognitively normal aging. Annals of Neurology 2009; in press. Mortimer JA, Ebbitt B, Jun SP, Finch MD. Predictors of cognitive and functional progression in patients with probable Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 1992; 42: 1689–96. Mucke L, Masliah E, Yu GQ, Mallory M, Rockenstein EM, Tatsuno G, et al. High-level neuronal expression of abeta 1-42 in wild-type human amyloid protein precursor transgenic mice: synaptotoxicity without plaque formation. J Neurosci 2000; 20: 4050–8. Mueller SG, Laxer KD, Barakos J, Cheong I, Garcia P, Weiner MW. Widespread neocortical abnormalities in temporal lobe epilepsy with and without mesial sclerosis. Neuroimage 2009; 46: 353–9. Nochlin D, van Belle G, Bird TD, Sumi SM. Comparison of the severity of neuropathologic changes in familial and sporadic Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 1993; 7: 212–22. Price JC, Klunk WE, Lopresti BJ, Lu X, Hoge JA, Ziolko SK, et al. Kinetic modeling of amyloid binding in humans using PET imaging and Pittsburgh Compound-B. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 2005; 25: 1528–47. Rabinovici GD, Furst AJ, O’Neil JP, Racine CA, Mormino EC, Baker SL, et al. 11C-PIB PET imaging in Alzheimer disease and frontotemporal lobar degeneration. Neurology 2007; 68: 1205–12. Ratnavalli E, Brayne C, Dawson K, Hodges JR. The prevalence of frontotemporal dementia. Neurology 2002; 58: 1615–21. Reiman EM, Caselli RJ, Yun LS, Chen K, Bandy D, Minoshima S, et al. Preclinical evidence of Alzheimer’s disease in persons homozygous for the epsilon 4 allele for apolipoprotein E [see comments]. New England J Med 1996; 334: 752–8. Reiman EM, Chen K, Liu X, Bandy D, Yu M, Lee W, et al. Fibrillar amyloid-beta burden in cognitively normal people at 3 levels of genetic risk for Alzheimer’s disease. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2009; 106: 6820–5. Rocchi A, Pellegrini S, Siciliano G, Murri L. Causative and susceptibility genes for Alzheimer’s disease: a review. Brain Res Bull 2003; 61: 1–24. Rosen HJ, Gorno-Tempini ML, Goldman WP, Perry RJ, Schuff N, Weiner M, et al. Patterns of brain atrophy in frontotemporal dementia and semantic dementia. Neurology 2002; 58: 198–208. Rosen RF, Ciliax BJ, Wingo TS, Gearing M, Dooyema J, Lah JJ, et al. Deficient high-affinity binding of Pittsburgh compound B in a case of Alzheimer’s disease. Acta Neuropathol 2009; Aug 19 [Epub ahead of print]. Rossor MN, Iversen LL, Reynolds GP, Mountjoy CQ, Roth M. Neurochemical characteristics of early and late onset types of Alzheimer’s disease. Br Med J (Clin Res Ed) 1984; 288: 961–4. Rowe CC, Ng S, Ackermann U, Gong SJ, Pike K, Savage G, et al. Imaging beta-amyloid burden in aging and dementia. Neurology 2007; 68: 1718–25. Sakamoto S, Ishii K, Sasaki M, Hosaka K, Mori T, Matsui M, et al. Differences in cerebral metabolic impairment between early and late onset types of Alzheimer’s disease. J Neurol Sci 2002; 200: 27–32.
G. D. Rabinovici et al. Salmon E, Collette F, Degueldre C, Lemaire C, Franck G. Voxel-based analysis of confounding effects of age and dementia severity on cerebral metabolism in Alzheimer’s disease. Hum Brain Mapp 2000; 10: 39–48. Savva GM, Wharton SB, Ince PG, Forster G, Matthews FE, Brayne C. Age, neuropathology, and dementia. N Engl J Med 2009; 360: 2302–9. Schneider JA, Arvanitakis Z, Bang W, Bennett DA. Mixed brain pathologies account for most dementia cases in community-dwelling older persons. Neurology 2007; 69: 2197–204. Seltzer B, Sherwin I. A comparison of clinical features in early- and lateonset primary degenerative dementia. One entity or two? Arch Neurol 1983; 40: 143–6. Sheline YI, Raichle ME, Snyder AZ, Morris JC, Head D, Wang S, et al. Amyloid plaques disrupt resting state default mode network connectivity in cognitively normal elderly. Biol Psychiatry 2009; Oct 13. [Epub ahead of print]. Shiino A, Watanabe T, Kitagawa T, Kotani E, Takahashi J, Morikawa S, et al. Different atrophic patterns in early- and late-onset Alzheimer’s disease and evaluation of clinical utility of a method of regional z-score analysis using voxel-based morphometry. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2008; 26: 175–86. Shiino A, Watanabe T, Maeda K, Kotani E, Akiguchi I, Matsuda M. Four subgroups of Alzheimer’s disease based on patterns of atrophy using VBM and a unique pattern for early onset disease. Neuroimage 2006; 33: 17–26. Snowden JS, Stopford CL, Julien CL, Thompson JC, Davidson Y, Gibbons L, et al. Cognitive phenotypes in Alzheimer’s disease and genetic risk. Cortex 2007; 43: 835–45. Sperling RA, Laviolette PS, O’Keefe K, O’Brien J, Rentz DM, Pihlajamaki M, et al. Amyloid deposition is associated with impaired default network function in older persons without dementia. Neuron 2009; 63: 178–88. Sun FT, Schriber RA, Greenia JM, He J, Gitcho A, Jagust WJ. Automated template-based PET region of interest analyses in the aging brain. Neuroimage 2007; 34: 608–17. Svoboda E, McKinnon MC, Levine B. The functional neuroanatomy of autobiographical memory: a meta-analysis. Neuropsychologia 2006; 44: 2189–208. Tzourio-Mazoyer N, Landeau B, Papathanassiou D, Crivello F, Etard O, Delcroix N, et al. Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI singlesubject brain. Neuroimage 2002; 15: 273–89. Wagner AD, Shannon BJ, Kahn I, Buckner RL. Parietal lobe contributions to episodic memory retrieval. Trends Cogn Sci 2005; 9: 445–53. Walsh DM, Selkoe DJ. A beta oligomers – a decade of discovery. J Neurochem 2007; 101: 1172–84. West MJ. New stereological methods for counting neurons. Neurobiol Aging 1993; 14: 275–85. Yasuno F, Imamura T, Hirono N, Ishii K, Sasaki M, Ikejiri Y, et al. Age at onset and regional cerebral glucose metabolism in Alzheimer’s disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 1998; 9: 63–7.