BarıŠKahramanâ¡. Hiromu Sakaiâ . â â¡Graduate School of Education, Hiroshima University ... structural factors, s
!"#$ %&'()*+, *+-( THE INSTITUTE OF ELECTRONICS,. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . IEICE Technical Report. INFORMATION AND COMMUNICATION ENGINEERS
Processing object relative clauses in the context. Another support for the discourse function account for the processing load asymmetry. Atsushi Sato/
Barı! Kahraman0
Hiromu Sakai/
/0Graduate School of Education, Hiroshima University E-mail:
/{asato1983, hsakai}@hiroshima-u.ac.jp
[email protected]
Abstract Roland et al. (2007, 2008) argued that the processing load asymmetry between object and subject relative clauses (ORCs/SRCs) arose from discourse factors rather than structural factors. According to Fox and Thompson (1991), the discourse function of ORCs is to anchor a newly introduced noun to the context. The lack of appropriate context thus makes the processing of ORCs more difficult than that of SRCs. We conducted a self-paced reading experiment using Japanese relative clauses and observed that grounding to the context resolved the processing asymmetry between ORCs and SRCs. The result thus provides another support for the discourse function account. Keyword Anchoring, Context, Discourse, Grounding, Relative clause, Sentence processing, Topic ORCs are typically used for grounding modified NP in the
1. Introduction ‘… The types of music that I don’t like are opera, and
discourse context, and subject relative clauses (SRCs) for
screaming heavy metal …’
other purposes, such as supplying additional information (Roland et al. 2007)
about the modified N P. Then, when the embedded NP is
People often use relative clauses (RCs) for introducing
not grounded, ORCs will incur more processing difficulty
a new discourse referent (‘the types of music’) by using discourse topic (‘I’) [1]. In general, personal pronouns
than that of SRCs. According to Roland et al., previous researches that
refer to entities that are topical. Many researchers have
found
found an extinguished difficulty in processing of object
appropriate contexts and this functional dissatisfaction
relative clause (ORC) when RC-internal NP is pronominal
might have caused delay in ORCs. In order to test the
[2, 3, 4]. Roland et al. [5, 6] hypothesize that the
effect of appropriate context, Roland et al. conducted an
asymmetry
the
experiment using two contexts (appropriate vs. neutral,
functional dissatisfaction of ORC. We conducted an
see Table 1). They found no difference at RC combined
experiment inspired by Roland et al.’s insight and found
region (3 and 4) in appropriate condition. In neutral
results in support for their functional account.
condition, reading time of ORCs was significantly longer
1.1. Hypotheses for relative clause processing
than that of SRCs.
in
RC
processing
is
derived
from
The inquiry into RC processing was initiated by examining the source of difficulty in forming filler-gap dependencies [7]. Dominant hypotheses were based on structural
factors,
such
as
linear
distance
[8]
and
structural depth [9]. As the research had progressed, however, other factors were found
that
showed
significant
impact on
the
processing difficulty [Animacy: 10, 11, 12; Frequency:
relative
difficulty
in
ORCs
did
not
supply
Table 1 Eample of Roland et al. (2008) Appropriate condition Neutral condition There was a dinner party The banker was friendly. Saturday night. RC SRC The lady that visited the banker enjoyed the party. ORC The lady that the banker visited enjoyed the party.
13]. In addition to these studies, Roland et al., proposed a new view, which takes the discourse function of RCs as the factor that determines the processing difficulty of
2. Importance of research on Japanese Although Roland et al's [5, 6] results are in favor of
ORCs.
their discourse-based account, their support is not strong
1.2. More on Roland et al. (2007)
enough due to the head-initial nature of English relative
The hypothesis proposed by Roland et al. is based
clause. Since the head noun precedes the relative clause,
primarily on Fox & Thompson’ idea [1]. They argued that
it is not clear at which point the head noun is 'grounded'
.
Table 2 Example Stimuli for the Experiment Appropriate condition Neutral condition Pre-RC Pre-RC Nihonzin-no daigakuinsei-ga hazimete gakkai-de happyou-siNihon-de hazimete kokusaiteki-na gakkai-ga kaisai-sare-ta. ta. "Japanese graduate student gave his presentation at the "The international conference was held in Japan for the first conference for the first time." time." RC SRC {Sono/Nihonjin-no} daigakuinsei-o hihansi-ta rikigaku-no kenkyuusya-wa okotte kaizyou-o ato-ni-si-ta {That/Japanese} graduate student-ACC criticize-PAST dynamics-GEN researcher-TOP angrily hall-ACC leave-PAST "The researcher of dynamics that criticized {the/Japanese} graduate student left the hall angrily." ORC {Sono/Nihonjin-no} daigakuinsei-ga hihansi-ta rikigaku-no kenkyuusya-wa okotte kaizyou-o ato-ni-si-ta "The researcher of dynamics that {the/Japanese} graduate student criticized left the hall angrily. Wrap-up Daigakuinsei-wa batu-ga warusou-ni danzyou-o orita. "The graduate student left the platform uncomfortably." in the context. As a result, we are not sure whether the
region must be directly related to the grounding process.
processing load observed in Roland et al's experiment reflects the lack of appropriate context for grounding
(3) gakusei-ga
head noun into the context. In
student-NOM
addition, the reading time increase is mainly
hihansi-ta
kenkyuusya
criticize-PAST
researcher
“the researcher that the student criticized”
observed at the RC-internal NP region in the neutral condition (squared region in the example below).
(4) gakusei-o student-ACC
(1) The lady that the banker visited ...
hihansi-ta
kenkyuusya
criticize-PAST
researcher
“the researcher that criticized the student”
(2) The lady that visited the banker …
In addition, since the head noun itself does not appear in the previous context, the effects of NP repetition can
The RC-internal subject is already mentioned in the
be strictly controlled. We can thus present more direct
appropriate condition, whereas it is newly introduced in
support for the discourse-based account using Japanese
the neutral condition. Although Roland et al. [6] reported
materials.
that the repetition of non-topic NP rather leads to ‘repeated
noun
penalty’,
it
does
not
exclude
Finall y, the results of experiments using Japanese
the
materials provide insight into the universality of the
possibility that the repetition of topic NP is mainly
discourse-based account. Since Japanese is often argued
responsible for the reduction of processing difficult y. It
to be a discourse oriented language [14], it might be that
would be desirable if the same effect is observed in a
context effects are more robust in Japanese.
region which does not contain the repeated N P.
Based
In short, the discourse-based account can be supported
on
these
considerations,
we
conducted
an
experiment using Japanese relative clauses.
more effectively if we can measure the processing load of the relative clause at the region that is more directly
3. Experiment
related to the discourse ‘grounding’ process and does not
3.1. Method
contain
the
repeated
N P.
experimental
material
for
We these
can
provide
respects
by
ideal using
Japanese relative clauses. Since Japanese relative clauses are strictly head-final, it is reasonable to assume that the grounding of RC head must take place in the head noun region (squared region in the example below). The reading time increase at this
3.1.1. Participants 42 graduate and undergraduate students from the Hiroshima University participated in the experiment.
3.1.2. Materials 20 experimental items were used with 4 conditions. Each
item
contains 3
sentences
(Pre-RC, RC,
and
.
Wrap-up sentences, see Table 1). These items were
3.2. Results
divided into 4 lists by Latin square design. Each list was
3.2.1. Data Trimming
combined with 48 filler sets. Filler sets contained 2 or 3 sentences.
Analysis for reading times was conducted only on correct responses. 3 subjects and 1 item were excluded
In the appropriate condition, first sentence introduced a
because of low comprehension accuracy (subjects: < 70%;
person as the topic in discourse (nihonzin-no daigakuinsei
item: < 60%) and the data exceeding 2 SD was also
“Japanese graduate student”), and the introduced noun
excluded. In total, the excluded data comprises 3.8% of
re-appeared at second sentence as a subject/object in a RC.
all data.
In the neutral condition, humans were not used in the first sentence, but were used in a relative clause in the second sentence. In the appropriate context condition, for the naturalness of RC sentences, and in order to avoid the repeated noun penalty, we used
“sono
(that)”. RC
sentences were divided into regions as follows; Region1 ({sono/nihonzin-no}),
Region2
(daigakuinsei-{o/ga}),
Region3 (hihansi-ta), Region4 (rikigaku-no), Region5 (kenkyuusya-wa), Region6 (okotte), Region7 (kaizyou-o), Region8 (ato-ni-si-ta). Before the experiment, we conducted a norming study in order to test the plausibility of the used materials. 26 sets of relative clauses were compared in pairs by t-test. As a result 4 sets were significantly different, and 2 sets were excluded due to low acceptabilit y. The total number of excluded items is 6. We used a word-by-word self-paced moving window paradigm. Whole sentences were presented to the subjects We
used
Overall accuracy was 89%. ANO VA reveals no reliable difference in accuracies [RC type: F 1 (1, 38) = 0.05, MSe = 0.07,
p > .10, F 2 (1, 18) = 0.41, MSe = 0.79, p > .10;
Context type: F 1 (1, 38) = 1.39, MSe = 0.07,
p > .10,
F 2 (1, 18) = 2.54, MSe = 0.79, p > .10], and No interaction was found [F 1 (1, 38) = 0.16, MSe = 0.07,
p > .10, F 2 (1,
18) = 1.22, MSe = 0.79, p > .10].
3.2.3. Reading time Reading time analyses were conducted only on the RC sentences. The critical region is 5 (relative head region). At region 5(kenkyuusya-wa “researcher-TOP”), the main effect of RC type was significant [F 1 (1, 38) = 18.244, MSe = 150,135,
p < .01, F 2 (1, 18) = 4.202, MSe =
205,876, p = .05]. The main effect of context type was significant only in subject analysis [F 1 (1, 38) = 9.252,
3.1.3. Procedure
word-by-word.
3.2.2. Comprehension Accuracy
a
DELL
laptop,
and
the
experiment was conducted on Linger Ver.2.84 software (developed by Dough Rohde). Yes/No questions followed the end of each set of sentences.
MSe = 150,135,
p < .05, F 2 (1, 18) = 2.899, MSe =
205,876, p = .11]. The relatively small number of items might have some influence on the results of the item analysis. The simple main effect analyses showed the following pattern. The RC type had no effect in the appropriate condition [F 1 (1, 38) = 1.59, MSe = 141,001, p > .10, F 2 (1, 18) = 1.06, MSe = 184,881, p > .10]. In the
.
4. Discussion 4.1. Region 3 The appropriate context eliminated the processing asymmetry between SRCs and ORCs. Ueno & Garnsey [15] argued that the parser is surprised by the initial O-marked NP of the sentence and this surprise effect would be spilt over the verb region. If their argument is on the right track, a grounded referent reduces the markedness of the initial O-marked NP of the sentence.
4.2. Region 5 These results are naturally accounted for by the discourse function hypothesis by Roland et al. [5]. The appropriate context greatly reduced the processing load of ORCs as shown in Fig. Since ‘grounding process’ seems to occur at the RC head region in Japanese, it is reasonable neutral condition, reading times for the ORCs were slower than those for the SRCs [F 1 (1, 38) = 11.27, MSe = 160,003,
p < .01, F 2 (1, 18) = 4.39, MSe = 228,371, p
< .05]. Fig. 3 indicates the difference in reading time between reading times for the SRCs minus those for the ORCs
in
each
condition.
t-test
reveals
significant
difference between ORCs in topic condition and neutral condition [t 1 (38) = 2.63, p < .01, t 2 (18) = 1.73, p < .10]. There was no reliable difference between SRCs in the topic and neutral conditions [t 1 (38) = 0.88, p > .10, t 2 (18) = 0.49, p > .10]. At the embedded verb region (Region 3: hihansi-ta “criticized”), the main effect of the RC type was significant [F 1 (1, 38) = 5.98, MSe = 44,563.3,
p < .05,
F 2 (1, 18) = 4.58, MSe = 66,683.5, p < .05]. In the neutral condition, reading times of SRCs were significantly slower than that of ORCs [F 1 (1, 38) = 5.32, MSe = 47,649.5,
p < .05, F 2 (1, 18) = 6.22, MSe = 67,123, p
< .05]. This tendency was also observed in Ueno & Garnsey [15]. In the appropriate condition, the simple main effect of RC type was not significant [F 1 (1, 38) = 0.25, MSe = 41,677.8,
p > .10, F 2 (1, 18) = 0.004, MSe =
66,268.6, p > .10]. In whole regions, the main effect of the context type was significant [F 1 (1, 38) = 42.123, MSe = 70,557.9,
p
< .01, F 2 (1, 18) = 11.052, MSe = 94,968, p < .01]. There was no reliable main effect of RC type [F 1 (1, 38) = 0.002, MSe = 70,557.9, p > .10].
p > .10, F 2 (1, 18) = 0.10, MSe = 94,968,
to
think
the
processing
load
reduction
observed in our experiment is initiated by the appropriate context. As an alternative to Roland et al.’s discourse function hypothesis, Mak et al. [11, 18] proposed a topichood hypothesis. Their hypothesis assumes that an entity that has higher topicworthiness in an RC is regarded as a syntactic subject. According to the topichood hypothesis, in the appropriate condition, RC-internal NPs are higher in topicworthiness and the parser analyzes them as the subject of the RC. This prediction
accelerates
the
processing of ORCs in the appropriate context because RC-internal NPs are actually in the syntactic subject position. The topichood hypothesis share some intuition with discourse function hypothesis and equally effective in explaining English data. The results of our experiment, however, hypothesis
cannot
be
because
easily
explained
Japanese
relative
by
the
clauses
topic are
head-final and reading time differentce is observed not at the RC internal NP position but in the head noun position. Since topichood hypothesis is concerned about processing of the subject NP in RCs, it cannot explain the processing difficulty of the head noun. Finall y, although the effect turns out to be not significant, we observed 57 ms difference between SRCs and ORCs in neutral condition. This might be caused by structural factor [9]. Dependency Locality Theory (D LT) [8] predicts ORCs are easier to process than SRCs in Japanese. This prediction is inconsistent with experiments in Japanese. But Warren & Gibson [4] revised D LT; discourse
status
also
affects
the
integration
cost.
Although it predicts that ORCs are basically easier in
.
Japanese, D LT might be right under the condition that the
Combined with the previous research using head-initial
discourse status of NP is equal like Ishizuka et al.
relative clauses in English or Dutch [5, 6, 18], our result
observed [19].
indicated some universal characteristics of the discourse
It might be argued that reduction of processing load of
function of object relative clauses. Further research is
ORC is not the effect of context but the effect of
needed to clarify the exact nature of discourse function of
anaphoric expression “sono (that)”. In some researches [2,
relative clauses and their relationship to topichood [11,
13], the processing load of ORCs is reduced or eliminated
18], inference [20, 21], or other discourse related
even under the isolated sentences when RC-internal NPs
concepts..
are pronoun. We leave this possibility open for future
References
research. Van den Broek [20] and his colleagues [21] argued that during discourse processing, the inference has a special role on bridging sentences. Because the sentences in the neutral condition might be more distant than those of the appropriate condition, the backward inference, which bridges
between
discourse
and
a
newly
introduced
sentence, incur heavier processing load in the neutral condition than in the appropriate condition, and this cognitive load might be responsible for the results in our experiment.
If
the
second
sentence
is
headed
by
“Kaizyou-de (at the hall)”, the backward inference might be easier and, as a result, the cognitive load can be reduced. (5) gakkai-ga
kaisai-sare-ta.
conference-NOM
hold-PAS-PAST
“The conference was held.” Kaizyou-de
gakusei-{o/ga}
hihansi-ta
hall- LOC
student-{ACC/NOM}
criticize-PAST
kenkyuusya-wa … researcher-TOP “The reseacher that {criticized
the student/the
student criticized}at the hall …” If the difference of the cognitive load for bridging between the first and second sentence in each condition is the main
source of the difficulty observed in our
experiment, the use of “Kaizyou-de” in the second sentence should eliminate the processing difficulty in neutral condition. We leave this unanswered question for a further stud y.
5. Conclusion In this stud y, we presented a new set of supporting data for the discourse function account for the processing difficulty using head-final Japanese relative clauses.
[1] Fox, B. & Thompson, S., A discourse explanation of the grammar of relative clauses in English conversation, Language, Vol.66, no.2, pp297-316, 1990. [2] Gordon, P.C., Hnedrick, R., & Johnson, M., Memory interference during language processing, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 27, pp.1411-1423, 2001. [3] Kaan, E., Effects of NP type on the resolution of word-order ambiguities, Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 30, pp.529-547, 2001. [4] Warren, T., & Gibson, E., The influence of referential processing on sentence complexity, Cognition, 85, pp.79-112. [5] Roland, D, O’Meara, C., Yun, H., & Mauner, G., Processing object relative clauses: Discourse or frequency?, Poster presented at the 20 th annual CUNY conference on Human sentence processing, University of California at Santa Barbara, 2007, California, US. [6] Roland, D, O’Meara, C., Yun, H., & Mauner, G., Discourse and Object Relative Clauses: The Effect of Topic versus Mention, Paper presented at the 21 s t annual CUNY conference on Human sentence processing, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, March 12-15 th , 2008, North Carolina, US. [7] Frazier, L., Syntactic Processing: Evidence from Dutch, Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 5, pp.519-559, 1987. [8] Gibson, E., The Dependency Locality Theory: A Distance-Based Theory of Linguistic Complexity, In Marantz, A., Miyashita, Y., & O’Neil, W. (Eds.), Image, Language, Brain: Papers from the First Mind Articulation Project Symposium, pp.95-126, 2002. [9] Hawkins, J. A., Processing Complexity and Filler-Gap Dependencies Across Grammars, Language, 75, pp.244-285, 1999 [10] Mak, W.M., Vonk, W., & Schriefers, H., The influence of animacy on relative clause processing, Journal of Memory and Language, 47, 50-68, 2002. [11] Mak, W.M., Vonk, W., & Schriefers, H., Animacy in processing relative clauses: The hikers that rocks crush, Journal of Memory and Language, 54, 466-490, 2006. [12] Traxler, M. J., Morris, R. K., & Seely, R. E., Processing Subject and Object Relative Clauses: Evidence from Eye Movements, Journal of Memory and Language, 47, pp.69-90., 2002. [13] Reali, F., & Christiansen, M.H., Processing of relative clauses is made easier by frequency of occurrence, Journal of Memory and Language, 57,
.
1-23, 2007. [14] Kuno, S., Danwa no Bunpoo, Taisyuukan, 1978. [15] Ueno, M., & Garnsey, S.M., An ERP study of the processing of subject and object relative clauses in Japanese, Language and Cognitive Processes, Volume 23, 646 - 688, 2008. [16] Ishizuka, T., Processing Relative Clauses in Japanese, In Okabe and Nielsen (Eds.), UCLA Working Papers in Linguistics, 13, pp.135-157, 2005. [17] Miyamoto, E. T., & Nakamura, M., Subject/Object Asymmetries in the Processing of Relative Clauses in Japanese, In G. Garding & M. Tsujimura (Eds.), WCCFL 22 Proceedings, pp. 342-355, 2003. [18] Mak, W.M., Vonk, W., & Schriefers, H., Discourse structure and relative clause processing, Memory and Cognition, 36, 170-181, 2008. [19] Ishizuka, T., Nakatani, K., & Gibson. E., Processing Japanese Relative Clauses in context, 19th Annual CUNY Conference on Human Sentence Processing, New York, March 2006. [20] van den Broek, P., Comprehension and memory of narrative texts, In M. A. Gernsbacher (Ed.), Handbook of Psycholinguistics, 1s t edition, Academic press, 1994. [21] van den Broek, P., Fletcher, C.R., & Risden, K., Investigations of Inferential Processes in Reading: A theoretical and methodological integration, Discourse P rocesses, 16, pp.169-180, 1993