Speech Fluency and Schizophrenic Negative Signs

1 downloads 0 Views 560KB Size Report
alogia suggests that the two negative signs share phe- nomenal and psychometric properties. The examina- tion of speech generation mechanisms may provide ...
At Issue: Speech Fluency and Schizophrenic Negative Signs by Murray Mpert, Antonis Kotsaftis, and Enrique R. Pouget

may be grammatical and appropriate (Brown 1958; Rochester and Martin 1979), but it is dysfluent. Pausing occupies more time in the speech of such patients than it does in the speech of demographically equivalent controls. Where speech is lacking in fluency, the presentation of the patient seems alogic and flat. In this discussion, we will examine evidence from acoustic and paralinguistic studies that suggests that the pauses result from a failure in speech generation mechanisms. This failure overlaps with cognitive processes that contribute to flat affect and alogia.

Abstract Pauses The spontaneous speech of negative-syndrome schizophrenia patients is underproductive and contains many hesitations and pauses. Acoustic analysis of the patient's speech during interview reveals that the duration of pauses, independent of other linguistic or paralinguistic measures, correlates strongly with the clinician's impressions of the patient's flat affect and alogia. Pausing is less related to asociality and other aspects of the negative syndrome. The hesitations appear to reflect a word-finding difficulty that, together with neuropsychological evidence of compromised performance on word fluency tasks, suggests a specific speech generation difficulty. The significant relationship between pausing and both flat affect and alogia suggests that the two negative signs share phenomenal and psychometric properties. The examination of speech generation mechanisms may provide an informative avenue for study of schizophrenic psychopathology. Acoustic analysis reveals processes that are not apparent to the clinician and may provide a useful basis for clinical assessments and research. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 23(2):171-177,1997.

Not all pauses are dysfluent. Pauses may be purposeful, such as those used to dramatize the succeeding phrase or to provide meter or audible punctuation. A pause also may be the speaker's way of allowing the listener to catch up when the information load is great, reflecting the speaker's sensitivity to the listener's needs. However, the excessive pauses in the speech of negative-syndrome patients are not dramatic or emphatic, nor do they suggest sensitivity to the needs of the patients' conversational partners. Pauses at different syntactic locations may reflect disturbances of specific speech generation processes. Switching pauses, which occur when the interviewer yields and the patient assumes the floor, reflect the time required to process the interviewer's question or comment and formulate a response. This reciprocal model presumes that the response depends on information in the interviewer's question or comment. However, some conversations, including some with thought-disordered patients, proceed more in parallel than as an interaction. Pauses may also occur between clauses. At this location a pause can provide emphasis or assign importance to

In interview, negative-syndrome schizophrenia patients often have hesitant speech with long pauses. The speech

Reprint requests should be sent to Dr. M. Alpert, Dept. of Psychiatry, New York University Medical Center, THN 323, 550 First Ave., New York, NY 10016.

171

Downloaded from schizophreniabulletin.oxfordjournals.org by guest on July 18, 2011

The At Issue section of the Schizophrenia Bulletin contains viewpoints and arguments on controversial issues. Articles published in this section may not meet the strict editorial and scientific standards that are applied to major articles in the Bulletin. In addition, the viewpoints expressed in the following article do not necessarily represent those of the staff or the Editorial Advisory Board of the Bulletin.—The Editors.

Schizophrenia Bulletin, Vol. 23, No. 2, 1997

M. Alpert et al.

Table 1. Duration of In-turn pauses and response latencies for normal controls, schizophrenia patients, and depressive patients

what follows, or give the speaker time to formulate the next phrase. Longer pauses precede clauses that contain more information (Goldman-Eisler 1958), suggesting that the cognitive demands of speech formulation extend the pauses. Despite a general increase in pause duration in negative-syndrome patients, differential increase in the duration of between-clause pauses does not correlate with severity of alogia (Alpert et al. 1994). A third pause location, within clauses, may reflect a number of speech formulation difficulties. Some of these pauses occur as a consequence of speech repairs, such as false starts and repetitions. Many within-clause pauses occur between the first and second words of a clause (Boomer 1965). Such pauses reflect a tendency of the speaker to impulsively begin to talk with only a rough idea of the succeeding text. Decisions about the structure of the text and the words that best express the proposition are made within the clause. These decisions take time and can produce noticeable hesitations. Within-clause pauses also may precede uncommon or unpredictable words (Beattie and Butterworth 1979). We have examined within-clause pauses in patients with alogia (Alpert et al. 1994) and found a pattern of pausing similar to that seen in normal subjects, although the pauses were longer for patients with alogia. The words terminating the pauses were less common than the other words used by that patient. However, in patients with alogia the average frequency of words did not differ. The hesitant speech of the patient with alogia appears to reflect some fault in the mechanics of word-finding; these patients may scan their lexicons more slowly or may be more subject to distraction. These alternatives may be susceptible to empirical testing. The syntactic site of a pause may reflect the specific phase of speech formulation. Levelt (1989) examined the sequence of speech generation stages and reviewed productive and dysfluent aspects of pausing. Many of the pauses that he discusses are brief, between 0.1 and 0.5 seconds. We find that a pause of 0.2 second is barely noticeable. Even pauses of 0.5 second are barely detectable and difficult to locate in the flow of speech. Table 1 presents durations of in-turn pauses (combining within- and between-clause pauses) and response latencies for groups of normal controls, schizophrenia patients, and depressive patients. These results are calculated from an acoustic analysis (Alpert et al. 1989) of continuous speech from 5- to 10-minute semistructured interviews. The analysis includes all gaps that occurred during the patient's turn. Analysis of continuous speech tends to find longer pauses than would spectrographic analysis of brief samples. The control group and the schizophrenia group were demographically similar; the depressive

In-turn pause Response latency (seconds) (seconds) n Mean SD Mean SD Normal controls Schizophrenia patients Depressive patients

20

0 .9

0.21

1.5

0.50

19

1.6

0.77

3.4

1.93

17

1.1

0.41

2.3

1.30

Note.—SD - standard deviation.

Alogia and Flat Affect Alogia has been related to both paucity of speech and impoverishment of speech content The two characteristics are treated as alternate expressions of the same underlying process, although they differ in the amount of speech expected. We find that the training of raters for the assessment of impoverished speech content is problematic. The rater must take into account the patient's education and IQ, allow for patient differences resulting from prolonged inpatient stays, and calculate the influence of other demographic factors that can produce artifactual relationships between vacuous interests and psychopathology. Perhaps because of these problems, impoverished speech content is only weakly coherent with other measures of alogia. Factor-analytic studies of ratings of combined positive and negative symptoms (Miller et al. 1993) and of only negative symptoms (Mueser et al. 1994), using different statistical approaches, found that paucity of speech and flat affect shared the same factor. This factor was different from that of impoverishment of speech content. We examined the relationship between pause measures and clinical judgments of flat affect (omitting the global and inappropriate-affect items) and alogia (omitting the global and the poverty-of-content items) in a group of 61 schizophrenia subjects diagnosed by the Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia

172

Downloaded from schizophreniabulletin.oxfordjournals.org by guest on July 18, 2011

patients were a little older and included more females. Differences between the groups were noted for in-turn pauses (F = 9.29; df= 2,53; p < 0.001) and for response latencies (F = 8.23; df = 2,51; p < 0.001). Depressive patients showed increased pausing (see also Greden et al. 1981), but only in comparison with normal controls; the pauses for the schizophrenia patients were still longer. A number of other studies that used this system (Alpert et al. 1986) found similar group differences (Kring et al. 1994; Sisonetal. 19%).

Speech Fluency

Schizophrenia Bulletin, Vol. 23, No. 2, 1997

Isolating the Effect of Pauses on Flat-Affect and Alogia Ratings Converging evidence of the importance of pausing as a basis for clinical impressions of flat affect and alogia was

Figure 1. Subscales of the SANS (flat affect, alogia, asociality) were regressed on the patient's average in-turn pause time (n = 61)

Asociality

Flat Affect Alogia

0

1

2

3

4

Mean In-tum Pause (Sees) For flat affect, the observed rating •> 0.94 (pause time) + 0.40; ft2 • 0.28; F - 23.2; p < 0.001. For atogia, the observed rating . 0.95 (pause time) - 0 . 1 8 ; R2 -0.31; F- 26.5; p< 0.001. For asociality, the observed rating « 0.44 (pause time) + 1.98; R 2 - 0.07; F - 4.6; p > 0.05. SANS » Scale (or the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (Andreasen 1984).

173

Figure 2. Subscales of the SANS (flat affect, alogia, asociality) were regressed on the patient's average response latency time (n = 61)

AsodaQy

FWAffactAlogia

0.0

2

4

6

8

Mean Response Latency (Sees) For flat affect the observed rating - 0.33 (latency time) + 0.64; R2 - 0.24; F. 18.6; p < 0.001. For atogia, the observed rating - 0.33 (latency time) + 0.07; ft2 . 0.26; F - 20.5; p < 0.001. For asociality, the observed rating . 0.08 (latency time) + 2.35; R2 - 0.01; F - 0 . 9 ; not significant SANS - Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (Andreasen 1984).

found in a study in which pause durations were electronically altered (Alpert et al. 1995a). Other aspects of patients' speech were not changed. We assembled three rating tapes, each containing about 5 minutes from six recorded interviews. Three of die samples were left unaltered as filler samples; these were the same on each tape. Three other samples were edited to create alternate versions: an original, one in which the duration of each pause was doubled, and a third in which the duration of each pause was halved. Only one version of each sample was on a tape. The words, even the inflection and emphasis, were unchanged in all versions. Listeners could not detect that the recordings had been altered. Clinicians each rated only one of the tapes, so each heard only one version of the three edited interviews. The version containing the lengthened pauses was rated as significantly flatter and more alogic (32% increase in flataffect and alogia ratings); the short-pause version was rated less flat and alogic. This study provided specific evidence implicating pausing in clinical impressions of flat affect and alogia, although the results do not preclude the possibility that other cues also may contribute to clinical impressions. For objective, behaviorally anchored rating scale items, acoustic measures of patients' speech can provide a yardstick against which to measure the validity of clinical assessment For example, to evaluate assessments of the item "increased latency to respond," which is included in both Andreasen's (1982) Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS) and Alphs' Negative Symptom Assessment (NSA; Alphs et al. 1989), we mea-

Downloaded from schizophreniabulletin.oxfordjournals.org by guest on July 18, 2011

(SADS; Endicott and Spitzer 1978). We described these subjects previously (Alpert et al. 1993). Fifty-eight male and three female subjects were included from four sites, including three Veterans Affairs (VA) hospitals. Figure 1 presents the results of the regression of flat affect and alogia on in-tum pause time; figure 2 presents a regression of the two signs on the response latency measure. The regressions are highly significant and virtually parallel. The difference between the flat-affect and alogia scores is significant; the same average pause duration produces a higher flat-affect rating than alogia rating (paired t = 4.34; n = 61; p < 0.001). The two pause measures are correlated (r = 0.63; n = 61; p < 0.001), as are the ratings of flat affect and alogia (r = 0.61; n = 6l;p< 0.001). In the same data set, the slopes and intercepts for ratings of asociality are quite different While the pause measures are strongly associated with ratings of flat affect and alogia, other measures of speech productivity, such as the duration of utterances, the number of syllables per utterance or per total time, and speech rate during utterances, were less related to these clinical ratings, suggesting that it is the dysfluency rather than the speech underproductivity that provides the critical cue for the raters assessing the negative signs of flat affect and alogia.

Schizophrenia Bulletin, Vol. 23, No. 2, 1997

M. Alpert et al.

sured average response latencies in a clinical trial that provided 31 rated recorded interviews. We found that only 22 percent of the variance in clinical ratings of this item was explained by the actual response latency measured acoustically; about half the rating variance was associated with diffuse "halo" effects (Alpert et al. 1995*). Other objectifiable items (e.g., slowed speech) showed similar weak associations with the target measure and stronger correlations with halo effects. It appears that such clinical ratings, even of objective, well-anchored items, are not very veridical. In addition, the strong relationship between ratings and halo effects might create artificially high estimates of interrater reliability (because raters could agree on the large component of halo variance). However, it remained possible that clinicians have some special ability to flexibly shift attention from the target item to especially salient aspects of the patient's behavior. The ratings in this study were from a double-blind treatment outcome trial, which allowed us to compare clinicians' ratings with acoustic measures of the same items on ability to distinguish between groups receiving typical and atypical neuroleptics. The acoustic measures showed much larger betweengroup effect sizes than did clinicians' ratings (Alpert et al. 1995a). Thus, the phenomena reflected in the ratings are relevant clinically but are difficult to ascertain.

also implicates a lack, or disorder, of thought as underlying the speech deficiency. There is a circularity to this definition, and more direct and independent measures of impoverished speech and negative thought disorder are needed.

Neuropsychology of Fluency

Pausing and Thought Disorder Much of the psychopathology research on pausing in schizophrenia has focused on the association between dysfluency and thought disorder. Thought-disordered schizophrenia patients tend to have longer between-clause pauses than do non-thought-disordered subjects (Rochester et al. 1977; Clemmer 1980; Resnick and Oltmanns 1984). These studies found that very long (greater than 5-seconds) between-clause pauses often are followed by incoherent speech; and it is specifically between-clause pauses that are associated with thought disorder. This pause type did not show a differential association with flat affect and alogia (Alpert et al. 1994). Some disturbance in speech generation at the betweenclause site (where the task is to formulate and carry forward the intended theme) might make the subsequent clause less cohesive and increase the risk of disordered, nonlinear thinking. Pausing has different implications for positive than for negative thought disorder. DSM-IV (American Psychiatric Association 1994) suggests that alogia, characterized by brief concrete replies to questions and a restriction in the amount of spontaneous speech, reflects an impoverishment in thinking. Andreasen (1982, 1984)

174

Downloaded from schizophreniabulletin.oxfordjournals.org by guest on July 18, 2011

It may be useful to contrast flat affect and alogia with nonfluent aphasia. While similar in some ways, flat affect and alogia and the aphasias differ in the quality as well as the quantity of speech behaviors. Nonfluent (Broca) aphasias are characterized by extremely sparse speech; considerable effort associated with the production of words; poor articulation; short phrase length (often a single word); loss of melodic, emphatic, and inflectional qualities of language (dysprosody); and a tendency to restrict speech to concrete content words (Benson and Geschwind 1985). Because of the monosyllabic output, pauses often are not found between words as in flat affect and alogia, where productivity is somewhat greater. In flat affect and alogia, syntax is more complex and dysarthria is not characteristically present. Prosody, including affective prosody, which reflects the loss of movements associated with emotional expressiveness, and linguistic prosody, which involves word and phrasal stress, is reduced or absent (Alpert and Anderson 1977). This reduction in prosody probably interacts with dysfluency to limit productivity. Discussing the neuropsychology of fluency, Lezak (1995) emphasized both ease and speed of verbal productivity, reflected in the length of uninterrupted strings of connected words. Neuropsychologists assess fluency by means of word-production tasks, counting the number of items (words) the subject produces (usually within a set time limit) within a restricted category (e.g., animals) or in response to a stimulus. Effortlessness is described as intrinsic to fluency but, to our knowledge, has not been operationalized. In an interesting aside to this issue, Sison et al. (1996) noted that flat-affect schizophrenia patients showed an increase in brow-corrugator electromylograph (EMG) activity during a task requiring them to describe emotional memories. Flat-affect patients generally show a decrease in activity of muscles involved in facial expression, but Darwin (1872/1965) had suggested that increased brow (forehead) activity accompanied mental effort. In the Sison study, the amount of pausing during an interview was a correlate of the amount of EMG corrugator activity during imagery. These results support the suggestion that ease of speech production is an important, albeit elusive, aspect of fluency. Frith and Allen (1988) suggested that schizophrenia

Schizophrenia Bulletin, Vol. 23, No. 2, 1997

Speech Fluency

son yields the floor to another) is a busy transition, involving many operations; the switching pause could be extended where calculations are slowed. (3) The period during which responses are formulated may place cognitive operations at risk, and does appear to mark vulnerability of such operations during this period and would be consistent with the observed association between thought disorder and between-clause pauses. (4) There is a delay in the initiation of motor-response actions. Difficulty in initiation of motor acts is a hallmark of hypodopaminergic states and is consistent witfi the loss of emotion-associated movements that characterizes flat affect Other possibilities affecting response latency can be imagined on the basis of the unique processes that occur in the interactions between the interviewer and the patient For such an explanation to be parsimonious, it should explain both the underproductivity of the patient's speech and its dysprosody. Although our data base of acoustic measures is insufficient to support specific hypotheses, focused data collection and cognitive studies with currently available methods could facilitate the design of experiments to discriminate among the possibilities. This discussion was buttressed by results of acoustic analysis of speech. Determining the patterns and timings of pauses requires use of such acoustic methods; even highly skilled clinical listeners cannot discern these phenomena simply from listening to an interview. Acoustic methods provide a means of validating the clinicians' ratings, at least for objective, well-anchored items. When this validation is done, the intrusion of halo effects becomes clear. Raters can achieve reliability on the basis of interviews' shared halo variance while capturing very little of the specific item variance. Although reliability is necessary, it is not sufficient to establish validity. A clinical laboratory infrastructure, similar to that of clinical medicine, would be very useful for improving the precision of patient assessment in psychiatry. Acoustic measurement of the patient's voice is one possible tool for assessing disturbed feeling states and, perhaps, disordered thoughts.

Summary We have presented evidence from several lines of research that suggests that there is a convergence between flat affect and alogia. The evidence also implies that both negative signs are related to a speech dysfluency. A disturbance in the mechanics of lexicon search, such as slower search speed, imprecision, or reduced resistance to distraction, might explain the low speech productivity and increased dysfluency of schizophrenia patients with negative signs. The syntactic location of many of the hesitations favors the hypothesis that specific rather than nonspecific factors are at work. It might be heuristic to explore the mechanisms relating extended response latencies and negative signs. Among the factors that might increase response latency are the following: (1) The patient requires extra time to decode the examiner's question or comment For instance, a delay could result from a failure to recognize the interviewer's prosodic cues indicating end-of-turn intentions. (2) The cognitive operator calculating the response works more slowly in these patients. The switch (when one per-

References Allen, H.A.; Liddle, P.F.; and Frith, C D . Negative features, retrieval processes and verbal fluency in schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychiatry, 163:769-775, 1993. Alpert, M., and Anderson, L.T. Imagery mediation of vocal emphasis in flat affect. Archives of General Psychiatry, 34:208-212, 1977. Alpert, M.; Clark, A.; and Pouget, E.R. The syntactic role of pauses in the speech of schizophrenic patients with alogia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 4:750-757, 1994.

175

Downloaded from schizophreniabulletin.oxfordjournals.org by guest on July 18, 2011

patients have difficulty sustaining the lexical search process and hypothesized that different forms of this difficulty may underlie negative or positive thought disorder. Either the speech is sparse or it is repetitive and perhaps inappropriate. Both disturbances result from a defective self-directed search process. To test this, Allen et al. (1993) examined schizophrenia patients in a categorynaming task. Their prediction, which the results confirmed, was that patients with poverty of speech would produce fewer items, while patients with incoherence would produce inappropriate (out-of-category) items. The authors also suggested that the reduced number of items did not simply reflect a smaller lexicon. It is possible to estimate the size of a subject's lexicon (i.e., the number of words in each category), by extrapolating from performance on a number of repetitions of the category-naming task. One can use the ratio of new items to the total number of items named on each iteration to calculate the size of the lexicon (Graesser and Mandler 1978). Lexicon size did not differ as a function of alogia. The size of a person's lexicon might be considered a correlate of IQ since the vocabulary subtest score is an indirect measure of lexicon size and is a strong correlate of full-scale IQ (Wechsler 1939). We could not find a significant relationship between the vocabulary subtest score and measures of pausing in flat-affect and non-flat-affect schizophrenia patients. Thus, the decreased speech productivity and hesitant speech of flat-affect and alogic patients appear to reflect some specific disturbance in speech generation.

Schizophrenia Bulletin, Vol. 23, No. 2, 1997

M. Alpert et al.

Alpert, M.; Merewhether, F.; Homel, P.; Martz, M.R.; and Lomask, M. Voxcom: A system for analyzing natural speech in real time. Behavioral Research Methods: Instruments and Computers, 18:267-272, 1986.

Animals. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press, 1872/1965. Endicott, J., and Spitzer, R.L. A diagnostic interview: The Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 35:837-844, 1978.

Alpert, M.; Pouget, E.; and Silva, R. Cues to the assessment of affects and moods: Speech fluency and pausing. Psychopharmacology Bulletin, 31(2):419-422, 1995a.

Frith, CD., and Allen, H.A. Language disorders in schizophrenia and their implications for neuropsychology. In: Bebbington, P., and McGuffin P., eds. Schizophrenia: The Major Issues. Oxford, England: Heinemann Medical, 1988. pp. 172-186.

Alpert, M ; Pouget, E.; Sison, C ; Yahia, M.; and Allan, E. Clinical and acoustic measures of the negative syndrome. Psychopharmacology Bulletin, 31(2):321-326, 1995*. Alpert, M.; Pouget, E.; Welkowitz, J.; and Cohen, J. Mapping schizophrenic negative symptoms onto measures of the patient's speech: Set correlational analysis. Psychiatry Research, 48:181-190, 1993.

Goldman-Eisler, F. The predictability of words in context and the length of pauses in speech. Language and Speech, 1:226-231, 1958.

Greden, J.F.; Albala, A.A.; Smokier, I.A.; Gardner, R.; and Carroll, B.J. Speech pause time: A marker of psychomotor retardation among endogenous depressives. Biological Psychiatry, 16:851-859, 1981.

Alphs, L.D.; Summerfelt, A.; Lann, H.; and Muller, RJ. The Negative Symptom Assessment: A new instrument to assess negative symptoms of schizophrenia. Psychopharmacology Bulletin, 25(2): 159-163, 1989. American Psychiatric Association. DSM-IV: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 4th ed. Washington, DC: The Association, 1994.

Kring, A.M.; Alpert, M.; Neale, J.M.; and Harvey, P.D. A multimethod, multichannel assessment of affective flattening in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research, 54:211-222, 1994.

Andreasen, N.C. Negative symptoms in schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 39:784-788, 1982.

Levelt, W.J.M. Speaking: From Intention to Articulation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1989. Lezak, M. Neuropsychological Assessment. 3rd ed. New York, NY: Oxford University Press, 1995.

Andreasen, N.C. The Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS). Iowa City, IA: The University of Iowa, 1984.

Miller, D.D.; Arndt, S.; and Andreasen, N.C. Alogia, attentional impairment, and inappropriate affect: Their status in the dimensions of schizophrenia. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 34:221-226, 1993.

Beattie, G.W., and Butterworth, B.L. Contextual probability and word frequency as determinants of pauses and errors in spontaneous speech. Language and Speech, 22:201-211, 1979.

Mueser, K.T.; Sayers, S.L.; Schooler, N.R.; Mance, R.M.; and Haas, G.L. A multisite investigation of the reliability of the scale for the assessment of negative symptoms. American Journal of Psychiatry, 151:1453-1462, 1994.

Benson, D.F., and Geschwind, N. Aphasia and clinical disorders: A clinical approach. In: Mesulam M.-M., ed. Principles of Behavioral Neurology. Philadelphia, PA: F.A.Davis, 1985. pp. 193-238.

Resnick, H., and Oltmanns, T.F. Hesitation patterns in the speech of thought-disordered schizophrenic and manic patients. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 93:80—86, 1984.

Boomer, D.S. Hesitation and grammatical encoding. Language and Speech, 8:148-158, 1965. Brown, R. Words and Things. Glencoe, IL: Free Press, 1958.

Rochester, S.R., and Martin, J.R. Crazy Talk. New York, NY: Plenum Press, 1979.

Clemmer, E.J. Psycholinguistic aspects of pauses and temporal patterns in schizophrenic speech. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 9:161-185, 1980.

Rochester, S.R.; Martin, J.R.; and Thurston, S. Thoughtprocess disorder in schizophrenia: The listener's task. Brain and Language, 4:95-114, 1977.

Darwin, C. The Expression of the Emotions in Man and

176

Downloaded from schizophreniabulletin.oxfordjournals.org by guest on July 18, 2011

Graesser, A., and Mandler, G. Limited processing capacity constrains the storage of unrelated sets of words and retrieval from natural categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 4:86—100, 1978.

Alpert, M.; Rosen, A.; Welkowitz, J.; Sobin, C ; and Borod, J.C. Vocal acoustic correlates of flat affect in schizophrenia: Similarity to Parkinson's disease and right hemisphere disease and contrast with depression. British Journal of Psychiatry, 155:51-56, 1989.

Schizophrenia Bulletin, Vol. 23, No. 2, 1997

Speech Fluency

Sison, C ; Alpert, M.; Fudge, R.; and Stern, H. Constricted expressiveness and psycho-physiological reactivity in schizophrenia. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 184:589-597, 19%.

Laboratories, Department of Psychiatry, New York University School of Medicine, New York, NY. Antonis Kotsaftis, Ph.D., is Clinical Instructor, and Enrique R. Pouget is Assistant Research Scientist, Millhauser Laboratories, Department of Psychiatry, New York University School of Medicine, New York, NY.

Wechsler, D. The Measurement of Adult Intelligence. Baltimore, MD: Williams and Wilkins Company, 1939.

The Authors Murray Alpert, Ph.D., is Professor and Director of Psychology and Associate Director of the Millhauser

The article by van den Bosch et al. entitled "What Determines Continuous Performance Task Performance?" (Schizophrenia Bulletin, 22(4):643-651, 1996) contains errors on pages 648 and 649. Figures 2 and 3 have been

reversed; figure 2 that appears on page 648 should be figure 3 and figure 3 on page 649 should be figure 2. The captions and footnotes for the figures were placed in the article correctly.

177

Downloaded from schizophreniabulletin.oxfordjournals.org by guest on July 18, 2011

Erratum