Working Memory and Learning in Children With ... - CiteSeerX

5 downloads 0 Views 98KB Size Report
Keywords: working memory; learning; developmental coordination disorder; specific ... opmental coordination disorder (DCD; also known as ...... Caplan (1996).
Working Memory and Learning in Children With Developmental Coordination Disorder and Specific Language Impairment

Journal of Learning Disabilities Volume XX Number X Month XXXX xx-xx © 2008 Hammill Institute on Disabilities 10.1177/0022219408315815 http://journaloflearningdisabilities .sagepub.com hosted at http://online.sagepub.com

Tracy Packiam Alloway Durham University

Lisa Archibald University of Western Ontario The authors compared 6- to 11-year-olds with developmental coordination disorder (DCD) and those with specific language impairment (SLI) on measures of memory (verbal and visuospatial short-term and working memory) and learning (reading and mathematics). Children with DCD with typical language skills were impaired in all four areas of memory function for their age level, and this pattern was also found to be characteristic of a larger DCD group with varied language abilities. SLI-group deficits in standard scores were observed for the verbal versions of the short-term and working memory tasks only. There were also differential links between memory and attainment between the two groups, with visuospatial working memory strongly related to numeracy in the SLI group and all of the memory measures correlated with at least one attainment measure in the DCD group. Reasons for why working memory contributes to learning in these two developmental groups are discussed. Keywords:

T

working memory; learning; developmental coordination disorder; specific language impairment

he extent to which deficits in specific cognitive mechanisms may underlie developmental disorders is a matter of considerable interest in the fields of cognition and cognitive development. The functioning of the working memory system has been implicated in groups of children with marked learning difficulties, such as learning disabilities (e.g., Alloway et al., 2005; Swanson & Saez, 2003), reading impairments (Gathercole, Alloway, Willis & Adams, 2006), and specific language impairment (SLI; e.g., Archibald & Gathercole, 2006a). To date, however, no studies have provided a comparison of working memory profiles in children with different developmental pathologies, and it was the purpose of the present study to do so. The comparison groups represented children with impairments predominantly affecting either language or motor skills: SLI, an impairment disproportionately affecting language skills, and developmental coordination disorder (DCD; also known as motor dyspraxia), characterized by difficulties with motor coordination. The extent to which deficits in subcomponents of working memory may differentiate these groups was explored by measuring skills systematically across the verbal and visuospatial domains. SLI in children is an unexpected failure to develop language at the usual rate, despite normal general intellectual

abilities, sensory functions, and environmental exposure to language. It is a relatively common developmental condition, estimated to occur in approximately 7% of kindergarten children (Tomblin et al., 1997), and is more prevalent in male than female children (e.g., Choudhury & Benasich, 2003; Flax et al., 2003). Affected children have the greatest problems in learning word forms and the grammatical structure of language, with the acquisition of semantics and pragmatics relatively spared (Leonard, 1998). Although the difficulties disproportionately affect the verbal domain, deficits in more general skills such as processing speed (Miller, Kail, Leonard, & Tomblin, 2001) and hypothesis testing (e.g., Ellis Weismer, 1991; Nelson, Kamhi & Apel, 1987) have been reported. In addition, it is widely acknowledged that individuals with SLI commonly experience learning difficulties of a comparable magnitude across all scholastic domains, including mathematics (Arvedson, 2002; Donlan & Gourlay, 1999; Fazio, 1996) and literacy (Bishop & Adams, 1990; Catts, Fey, Tomblin, & Zhang, 2002; Flax et al., 2003). Authors’ Note: We would like to thank Susan Gathercole for valuable comments made to an earlier version of this article. We also wish to extend our gratitude to the primary schools and children who participated in this research.

1

2

Journal of Learning Disabilities

DCD is characterized by marked motor impairment that affects functioning in daily activities in the absence of intellectual or neurological dysfunction (American Psychiatric Association, 1994). Observable behaviors in children with DCD include clumsiness, poor posture, confusion about which hand to use, difficulties throwing or catching a ball, reading and writing difficulties, and an inability to hold a pen or pencil properly. The estimated prevalence of DCD in children aged 5 to 11 years is about 6% (Mandich & Polatajko, 2003), with more male than female children being affected. As with SLI, substantial heterogeneity exists in the cognitive profiles of children with DCD, with some researchers suggesting that comorbidity is so widespread in DCD as to be the norm rather than the exception (Kaplan, Wilson, Dewey, & Crawford, 1998; Piek & Dyck, 2004; Wilson, 2005). The common occurrence of concomitant language impairments with DCD (e.g., Hill, 2001; Visser, 2003) has led to the suggestion that linguistic difficulties may underlie some of the learning problems experienced by children with DCD (Visser, 2003). Although these developmental disorders are distinguished by the domain of principal deficit, they are both associated with significant learning difficulties. A key contributing factor in learning during development is working memory abilities (e.g., Alloway, Gathercole, Adams, et al., 2005; Gathercole, et al., 2006; Swanson & Sachse-Lee, 2001). Working memory is the capacity to store and manipulate information over brief periods of time (Baddeley & Hitch, 1974; Just & Carpenter, 1992). Extensive research over the past three decades has established that working memory is not a single store but a memory system composed of separable interacting components. Functioning in concert, these components provide a kind of flexible mental workspace that can be used to maintain and transform information in the course of demanding cognitive activities. There are several theoretical accounts of working memory. In the present study, we used Baddeley and Hitch’s (1974) model of working memory model (see also Baddeley, 2000), a widely used model in both developmental and adult samples (e.g., Alloway, Gathercole, Willis & Adams, 2004; Baddeley, 1996). According to this model, there are separable components for the temporary storage of verbal and visuospatial information: the phonological loop and the visuospatial sketchpad, respectively. A centralized component is responsible for coordinating the flow of information between these storage systems and the temporary activation of long-term memory. The central executive also functions as a mental workspace involved in the temporary storage and manipulation of information needed in the execution of complex cognitive tasks, such as learning, reasoning, and comprehension.

In recent years, standard methods of assessing both verbal and visuospatial aspects of working memory have been developed and validated. Two tests, the Working Memory Test Battery for Children (WMTB-C; Pickering & Gathercole, 2001) and the Automated Working Memory Assessment (AWMA; Alloway & Gathercole, 2007), provide multiple measures of subcomponents of working memory standardized for children aged 4 to 11 years. Tasks tapping the temporary storage of the phonological loop and visuospatial sketchpad require the serial retention of either phonological information such as digits (e.g., Conrad & Hull, 1964) or visuospatial material such as visual patterns (e.g., Smyth & Scholey, 1996). Working memory is typically assessed using complex memory span paradigms that impose demands for both temporary storage and significant processing activity with selected task components varied across domains. An example of a verbal complex span task is reading span, in which a participant is asked to make a meaningbased judgment about each of a series of sentences and then remember the last word of each sentence in sequence (Daneman & Carpenter, 1980). A corresponding visuospatial task is spatial span, in which a participant is asked to judge the orientation of a set of letters and then remember the sequence of degrees of rotation of the letters (Shah & Miyake, 1996). Working memory skills can explain individual differences in learning. Poor performance on working memory measures is characteristic of children failing to progress normally in the areas of reading (e.g., De Jong, 1998; Swanson, 1994), mathematics (e.g., Bull & Scerif, 2001; Mayringer & Wimmer, 2000; Passolunghi & Siegel, 2001; Siegel & Ryan, 1989), and language comprehension (e.g., Nation, Adams, Bowyer-Crain, & Snowling, 1999; Seigneuric, Ehrlich, Oakhill, & Yuill, 2000). There are also links between performance on tasks measuring phonological loop capacity of working memory and vocabulary acquisition (for a review, see Baddeley, Gathercole, & Papagno, 1998). Associations have been reported as well between measures of the visuospatial sketchpad component of working memory and mental arithmetic (Lee & Kang, 2002; McLean & Hitch, 1999). Associations between working memory abilities and learning have also been found in children with special educational needs. In particular, children experiencing learning difficulties that are sufficiently severe to warrant special education provision have been found to perform very poorly on measures of working memory (Alloway, Gathercole, Willis, & Adams, 2005; Gathercole & Pickering, 2001; Pickering & Gathercole, 2004). In a recent study of children with deficits in literacy and numeracy, Gathercole et al. (2006) found that performance on verbal working memory tasks is not mediated

Alloway, Archibald / DCD and SLI

by other associated cognitive skills and that it taps an ability that is uniquely linked with learning abilities. This is consistent with the view that verbal working memory is not just a proxy for IQ and is supported by related research on learning disabilities, verbal working memory, and IQ (e.g., Cain, Oakhill & Bryant, 2004; Siegel & Ryan, 1989). Working memory functioning has been investigated in some detail in SLI. Deficits in phonological loop capacity in SLI have been widely reported in studies of nonword repetition (e.g., Botting & Conti-Ramsden, 2001; Dollaghan & Campbell, 1998; Edwards & Lahey, 1998; Ellis Weismer et al., 2000; Gathercole & Baddeley, 1990), as well as studies using more conventional verbal shortterm memory measures, such as serial recall of digits or words (Archibald & Gathercole, 2006a; Graham, 1980; Wiig & Semel, 1976). Age-appropriate performance, on the other hand, has been reported for SLI groups on visuospatial storage tasks (Archibald & Gathercole, 2006a, 2006b). Children with SLI have also been found to be impaired on working memory tasks involving the storage and processing of verbal (e.g., Archibald & Gathercole; 2006a; Ellis Weismer, Evans, & Hesketh, 1999) but not visuospatial information (e.g., Archibald & Gathercole, 2006; Bavin, Wilson, Maruff, & Sleeman, 2005). This dissociation has been explained in light of evidence that performance decrements occur only when the task additionally taps the impaired phonological but not preserved visuospatial storage abilities (Archibald & Gathercole, 2007). However children with SLI are slower than typically developing peers in completing the processing portion of both verbal and visuospatial working memory tasks. With respect to children with DCD, there is emerging evidence that they have a specific deficit in visuospatial memory (e.g., Alloway, 2006; Alloway & Temple, 2007). This deficit can be understood in light of research indicating that visuospatial skills are linked with movement planning and control (e.g., Quinn, 1994; Smyth, Pearson, & Pendleton, 1988). For example, Smyth et al. (1988) found that participants’ retention of simple movements in sequence was comparable with their retention of verbal information, indicating that visuospatial memory parallels verbal memory. This is consistent with the view that certain aspects of visuospatial working memory overlap with cognitive resources allocated to generating movement (e.g., Duff & Logie, 1999). Visuospatial memory skills in children with DCD have been linked to learning. A recent study comparing DCD children with high and low visuospatial memory skills found that children with low visuospatial memory skills performed significantly worse than children with high visuospatial memory skills in literacy and numeracy, even when IQ was taken

3

into account (Alloway, 2007b). The unique link between visuospatial memory skills and learning suggests that the processing demands of the working memory tasks and the active motor component reflected in the visuospatial memory tasks both play a crucial role in learning in children with DCD. The current work represents one of the first comparisons of working memory and learning in children with DCD and those with SLI. Because one of the criteria for DCD included in the fourth edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV) is “significant difficulties with motor coordination, which affects their academic achievement” (American Psychiatric Association, 1994), these children often perform worse than age-matched controls on measures of literacy, such as reading, writing, and spelling (e.g., Dewey, Kaplan, Crawford, & Wilson, 2002; Iversen, Berg, Ellertsen, & Tonnessen, 2005). A longitudinal study confirmed that children who are diagnosed with DCD at 7 years of age are limited in their reading and comprehension levels by the age of 10 (Kadesjo & Gillberg, 1999). As with other atypical populations, such as children with poor reading comprehension (Swanson & Berninger, 1995) and children with persistent learning difficulties (Gathercole et al., 2006), it may be that the learning difficulties of children with DCD are associated with working memory impairments. An alternative possibility is that deficits in comorbid cognitive or language skills account for the performance levels of children with DCD. In this study, measures of verbal and visuospatial shortterm memory and verbal or visuospatial working memory were completed by three groups of children: children with SLI, an age-matched group of children with typically developing language skills and DCD, and an unselected group of children with DCD that also included children with language impairments. Although the selection of a DCD and typical language group provides an interesting contrast to an SLI group, it was unknown whether a DCD and typical language group would perform differently than an unselected DCD group on the memory and learning measures included in the present study. One goal of this study therefore was to compare the working memory profiles of the two DCD groups. A second goal was to compare the SLI and DCD and typical language groups. Similar working memory profiles across groups would favor an account emphasizing the commonalities between these developmental disorders and suggesting that a generalized deficit may underlie both conditions (e.g., Hill, 2001; Visser, 2003). Findings of differential working memory impairments would make an important contribution to understanding the cognitive mechanisms that may give rise to domain-specific deficits.

4

Journal of Learning Disabilities

Method Participants Thirty-four children participated in three groups in the present study: 11 children with SLI (7 boys, 4 girls), 11 chronologically age-matched children with DCD and typical language skills (8 boys, 3 girls), and 12 additional, unselected children with DCD (8 boys, 4 girls) participated in the present study. The mean ages of the groups were as follows: SLI: 8 years 10 months (SD = 1.41 years, range = 6 years 9 months to 10 years 10 months); DCD and typical language: 8 years 11 months (SD = 1.43 years, range = 6 years 11 months to 11 years 0 months); unselected DCD: 8 years 6 months (SD = 1.64 years, range = 5 years 0 months to 11 years 8 months). All participants were native English speakers. None of the children were diagnosed with attention-deficit disorder or attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, autism spectrum disorder, or hearing impairment. SLI group. The children in the SLI group were recruited from language units in urban areas of the Northeast of England over a 4-month period. All of the children met SLI criteria consistent with those described by Records and Tomblin (1994). They achieved standard scores of 85 or greater on a test of nonverbal reasoning (Raven’s Coloured Matrices; Raven, Court, & Raven, 1986). In addition, they performed at least 1.25 standard deviations below the mean on two of four language measures, including one receptive measure. The receptive measures were the British Picture Vocabulary Scales, 2nd Edition (BPVS-II; Dunn, Dunn, Whetton, & Burley, 1997) and the Test for Reception of Grammar (TROG; Bishop, 1982). The expressive measures were the Expressive Vocabulary Test (Williams, 1997) and the Recalling Sentences subtest of Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals–UK 3 (CELF-UK3; Semel, Wiig, & Secord, 1995). It is worth noting that although some of the language tests may have minimal working memory demands, these tests are commonly used to identify language impairments and were used for this purpose in the present study. None of the children had motor difficulties as identified by the Movement Assessment Battery Teacher Checklist (Henderson & Sugden, 1996), completed by the participants’ classroom teachers. DCD groups. The children with DCD also attended schools in the Northeast of England and were referred by an occupational therapist who had identified them as experiencing motor difficulties using the DSM-IV criteria for DCD (American Psychiatric Association, 1994) and standardized motor assessments such as the Movement Assessment Battery for Children (Henderson & Sugden,

1992). The Movement Assessment Battery Teacher Checklist (Henderson & Sugden, 1996), completed for each participating child at the time of the present study, confirmed the severity of the child’s movement difficulties. All 23 participants with DCD completed the BPVS-II (Dunn et al., 1997) and the Recalling Sentences subtest of the CELF-3UK (Semel et al., 1995), and 19 completed Raven’s Coloured Matrices (Raven et al., 1986) and the TROG (Bishop, 1982). None of the children experienced difficulty in performing these tasks as a function of their motor impairments. From these, a group of 11 children with DCD individually age matched with participants in the SLI group who had achieved standard scores of 85 or greater on Raven’s Coloured Matrices and the BPVS-II were selected to form the DCD and typical language group.

Procedure The measures reported her were completed in three or four individual half-hour sessions in a quiet room in the children’s schools. Because the aim of the present study was to compare the performance of two groups, SLI and DCD, standardized measures were used in all instances. In addition to the language screening measures and nonverbal ability test listed above, each child was also tested first on a working memory assessment battery, followed by attainment measures. The SLI group completed the WMTB-C (Pickering & Gathercole, 2001), as well as the visuospatial working memory measures of the AWMA (Alloway, & Gathercole, 2007), and the DCD groups completed the AWMA. Additionally, both groups completed measures of attainment in reading and mathematics. No more than two standardized tests were completed in one session. The subtest order recommended by the standardized test was followed within each test. The AWMA (Alloway & Gathercole, 2007) was designed as an automated equivalent of the WMTB-C (Pickering & Gathercole, 2001) (see Note 1). Both tests provide multiple assessments of each of the three components of Baddeley and Hitch’s (1974) working memory model, using tasks that have been validated as measures of each component. The batteries include three measures of verbal short-term memory (digit recall, word recall, and nonword recall), three measures of visuospatial short-term memory (block recall, mazes memory, and dot matrix), and three verbal working memory measures (backward digit recall, listening recall, and counting recall), and the AWMA includes three visuospatial working memory measures (odd one out, Mr. X, and spatial span). Both tests provide standardized scores, with a mean value of 100 and a standard deviation of 15, for 4- to

Alloway, Archibald / DCD and SLI

11-year-olds (AWMA: Alloway, 2007a; WMTB-C: Pickering & Gathercole, 2001). Verbal short-term memory. The digit recall, word recall, and nonword recall tests involve the presentation of a sequence of digits, words, or nonwords that a child is required to recall in correct serial order. Digit lists are random constructions without replacement from the digits ranging from 1 to 9, spoken at a rate of one digit per second. The word lists are monosyllabic words with a consonant-vowel-consonant structure, and no stimuli are repeated. The nonwords have the same structure and were created using the same pool of phonemes as the words used in the word recall subtest. The words and nonwords, which are spoken at a rate of one syllable per second, must be recalled with full accuracy (i.e., with all three phonemes correct) and in the correct serial position. Credit was given for phoneme substitutions when the substitution constituted the child’s habitual articulation pattern for that phoneme. Verbal working memory. In the backward digit recall test, a child is required to recall a sequence of spoken digits in reverse order. Test trials begin with two numbers and increase by one number in each block until the child is unable to recall four correct trials at a particular block. In the listening recall task, a child is presented with a series of spoken sentences (e.g., “Lions have four legs,” “Pineapples play football”) and has to verify the sentence by stating “true” or “false” and recall the final word for each sentence in sequence. Test trials begin with one sentence and continue with additional sentences in each block until the child is unable to recall three correct trials at a block. In the counting recall test, a child is presented with a visual array of red circles and blue triangles. He or she is required to count the number of circles in an array and then recall the tallies of circles in the arrays that were presented. The test trial begins with one visual array and increases by an additional visual array in each block until the child is unable to correctly recall four trials. Each visual array stayed on the computer screen until the child indicated that he or she had completed counting all the circles. The number of correct trials was scored for each child. Visuospatial short-term memory. In the block recall test, the presenter taps a sequence of cubes with a finger on a specifically designed board that has nine randomly located cubes. The child’s task is to repeat the sequence in the same order. Testing begins with a single block tap and increases by one additional block. In the mazes memory test, the child views a two-dimensional line maze with a path drawn through the maze. The same maze is then shown to the child without the path, and the child is

5

asked to recall the path by drawing it on the maze. Each maze is presented for 3 seconds. Maze complexity is increased by adding additional walls to the maze. In the dot matrix task, a sequence of red dots is presented on a 4 × 4 grid, and the child is required to point to the positions of each dot that had appeared in the sequence in the same order. Each dot appears for 2 seconds. The sequences were random, with no location being highlighted more than once within a trial. Visuospatial working memory. In the odd one out task, a child is presented with a horizontal row of three boxes in which three complex shapes are presented. The child points to the shape that does not match the others and remembers its location. At the end of the trial, a blank set of three boxes appears on the screen. The child points to the boxes in which the odd shapes had appeared in the correct order. The boxes always appear centered horizontally on the screen, but at different positions along the vertical axis to eliminate visual traces. In the Mr. X task, a child is presented with a picture of two Mr. X figures. The child identifies whether the Mr. X with the blue hat is holding the ball in the same hand as the Mr. X with the yellow hat. The Mr. X with the blue hat may also be rotated. At the end of each trial, the child has to recall the location of each ball in Mr. X’s hand in sequence, by pointing to a picture with eight compass points. Both the Mr. X figures and the compass points stayed on the computer screen until the child provided a response. In the spatial span task, a child views a picture of two identical shapes in which the shape on the right has a red dot on it. The child identifies whether the shape on the right is the same or opposite of the shape on the left. The shape with the red dot may also be rotated. At the end of each trial, child has to recall the location of each red dot on the shape in sequence, by pointing to a picture with three compass points. Both the shapes and the compass points stayed on the computer screen until the child provided a response. Attainment measures. Abilities in the areas of literacy and numeracy were also tested. All participants completed the basic reading and spelling subtests of the Wechsler Objective Reading Dimensions (Wechsler, 1993), and the numerical operations subtest of the Wechsler Objective Numerical Dimensions (Wechsler, 1996).

Results DCD Groups Descriptive statistics for the DCD and typical language and unselected DCD groups on the screening, attainment,

6

Journal of Learning Disabilities

and memory measures are shown in Table 1. Group mean standard scores were within 1 standard deviation of the mean for both groups on Raven’s Coloured Matrices, the BPVS-II, and the TROG, and for the DCD and typical language group on all of the attainment measures, and the mazes memory subtest. Performance on the majority of the memory measures was more than 1.25 standard deviations below the standardized mean for both DCD groups. Group means were remarkably similar across measures, with the greatest group differences occurring on the receptive language measures (BPVS-II and TROG) and visuospatial short-term and working memory measures (mazes memory, Mr. X, and spatial span). Consider first the screening and attainment measures. In independent t tests, the only significant group effect was found for the BPVS-II, t(21) = 2.58, p = .02, as expected given the selection criteria. The remaining comparisons were nonsignificant: Raven’s Coloured Matrices, t(17) < 1; TROG, t(21) = 1.79, p = .09; recalling sentences, t(21) < 1; numerical operations, t(21) < 1; reading, t(21) < 1; and spelling, t(21) = 1.11, p = .28. It is clear from these results that although the DCD and typical language group had superior receptive vocabulary skills, their performance on a variety of cognitive, language, and attainment measures was indistinguishable from that of other children with DCD. To minimize the number of group comparisons on the memory measures, a multivariate analysis of variance was completed on the composite scores (verbal short-term memory, verbal working memory, visuospatial short-term memory, and visuospatial working memory) as a function of group. The overall group Hotelling’s T was nonsignificant, F(4, 18) = 0.65, p = .63, η2p = .13, and none of the univariate comparisons approached significance (p > .12 in all cases). These results indicate that the working memory profile of the DCD and typical language group was not significantly different from that of other children with DCD. Also of interest is the fact that although the vocabulary skills of the groups differed, their performance on the verbal memory measures was not unduly influenced by language level.

DCD and Typical Language and SLI Groups Descriptive statistics for children with DCD and typical language and SLI on the screening, memory, and attainment measures are shown in Table 1. Group mean standard scores were within 1 standard deviation of the mean for both groups on Raven’s Coloured Matrices and all of the attainment measures, for the DCD and typical language group on the BPVS-II and TROG, and for the SLI group on the visuospatial short-term and working memory measures. Performance on the verbal short-term and working

memory measures was generally more than 1.25 standard deviation below the standardized mean for both groups. The DCD and typical language group achieved higher scores on all of the language measures (BPVS, TROG, and the Recalling Sentences subtest of the CELF-UK3), as expected. The SLI group had slightly higher scores on the attainment and verbal short-term memory measures and considerably higher scores on the visuospatial short-term and working memory tasks. Group means for the verbal working memory tests were similar. Consider first the screening and attainment measures. In independent t tests, the only significant group effects were found for the language measures: BPVS-II, t(20) = 6.04, p < .001; TROG, t(20) = 5.14, p < .001; Recalling Sentences subtest of the CELF-UK3, t(20) = 4.24, p < .001. The remaining comparisons were nonsignificant: t(20) < 1 for Raven’s Coloured Matrices, numerical operations, reading, and spelling. These results indicate that although the two groups differed in language skills, they performed at similar levels on the cognitive and attainment measures. In the multivariate analysis of variance performed on the composite memory scores as a function of group, the overall Hotelling’s T was significant, F(4, 17) = 3.86, p = .02, η2p = .48. Two of the univariate group comparisons were significant: visuospatial short-term memory, F(1, 20) = 4.992, p = .035, η2p = .20, and visuospatial working memory, F(1, 20) = 11.91, p = .003, η2p = .37. The remaining comparisons were nonsignificant (p > .25 in both cases). Thus, the SLI group performed at superior levels on the visuospatial memory measures but at a similar level to the DCD and typical language group on the verbal memory measures. To confirm that language skills were not mediating performance on memory measures between the two groups, a multivariate analysis of covariance was performed on the four composite memory measures, with the receptive language measure (BPVS-II raw scores) as a covariate. The overall group term was no longer significant, F = 1.52, p = .24, η2p = .28), although the DCD group still performed significantly worse compared with the SLI children in visuospatial working memory, F(1, 19) = 4.20, p = .05, η2p = .18. These findings indicate that even when the contribution of receptive language skills was accounted for, the DCD group performed significantly worse on the visuospatial working memory measures compared with the SLI group.

Group Membership As a test of the extent to which memory scores uniquely differentiated the two atypical groups, discriminant function analyses were conducted for the two memory measures that produced significant group differences and

7

119 121 114 10 90 102 110 98 105 127 91 109 95 113 107 97 106 118 93 111 111 118 118

61 42 52 63 63 43 59 53 56 32 68 45 59 47 61 54 66 71 72

Maximum

85 86 86 3

Minimum

87.91 85.45 85.82

79.67 79.18 80.64 79.18 80.91 84.55 77.73 80.45 81.58 81.18 87.55 76.00 77.48 79.64 79.73 73.09

101.36 100.91 97.55 6.91

M

13.33 13.49 13.31

8.77 17.84 14.55 10.53 12.13 24.46 10.44 19.83 11.61 21.90 14.59 15.21 11.75 17.53 9.60 17.14

11.13 9.18 10.03 2.77

SD

63 65 66

61 55 55 41 58 55 60 50 63 55 65 57 54 68 48 40

75 56 57 3

Minimum

112 102 105

103 109 99 114 101 113 114 116 100 103 104 116 88 96 90 81

119 124 103 10

Maximum

83.42 81.00 80.00

79.03 78.42 81.25 77.42 79.83 83.92 75.17 80.42 77.64 76.08 77.67 79.17 69.83 79.67 68.75 61.08

99.63 87.42 87.00 5.92

M

Unselected DCD (n = 12)

13.67 11.93 11.82

13.55 14.07 15.15 20.62 13.09 15.74 14.38 19.48 11.40 12.70 11.06 18.59 10.91 9.88 13.02 16.27

18.42 14.95 15.70 2.19

SD

75 71 72

72 69 74 59 71 58 60 72 69 64 67 70 79 76 68 79

90 58 63 3

Minimum

138 119 119

98 104 97 101 89 103 95 88 113 126 130 124 108 130 102 96

125 94 91 5

Maximum

M

91.18 89.91 90.36

83.94 85.64 84.82 81.36 80.67 86.82 74.27 80.91 93.09 93.09 92.45 93.73 92.30 98.55 90.18 88.18

101.36 76.09 77.36 3.27

SLI (n = 11)

18.30 15.17 14.49

8.95 10.48 7.81 14.40 5.71 12.85 12.02 6.28 12.55 18.37 14.86 16.27 8.05 16.19 10.25 6.18

9.97 10.08 8.31 0.65

SD

Note: BPVS-II = British Picture Vocabulary Scales, 2nd Edition; CELF-UK3 = Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals–UK 3; DCD = developmental coordination disorder; SLI = specific language impairment; STM = short-term memory; TROG = Test for Reception of Grammar; WM = working memory; WOND = Wechsler Objective Numerical Dimensions; WORD = Wechsler Objective Reading Dimensions. a. Scaled scores (M = 10, SD = 3).

Screening measures Raven’s Coloured Matrices Receptive vocabulary (BPVS-II) Receptive grammar (TROG) Recalling Sentences subtest of the CELF-UK3a Verbal STM: composite score Digit recall Word recall Nonword recall Verbal WM: composite score Listening recall Counting recall Backward digit recall Visuospatial STM: composite score Dot matrix Mazes memory Block recall Visuospatial WM: composite score Odd one out Mr. X Spatial span Attainment measures Numerical operations (WOND) Reading (WORD) Spelling (WORD)

Measure

DCD and Typical Language (n = 11)

Table 1 Descriptive Statistics of Standard Scores for Language, Memory, and Attainment for the DCD and Typical Language, Unselected DCD, and SLI Groups

8

Journal of Learning Disabilities

Table 2 Classification by Discriminant Function Analysis for Memory and Language Measures Correctly Classified as Variable Entered SLI (n = 11) vs. DCD and typical language (n = 11) Visuospatial STM Visuospatial WM Visuospatial STM and WM BPVS-II standard score SLI (n = 11) vs. unselected DCD (n = 12) Visuospatial WM Visuospatial and verbal WM BPVS-II standard score

SLI

Table 3 Pearson Correlations Between Composite Memory and Attainment Measures for the SLI, DCD and Typical Language, and Full DCD Groups Composite Memory Measures

DCD

Verbal Attainment Measure 7 (63.6%) 9 (81.8%) 9 (81.8%) 9 (81.8%)

8(72.7%) 9 (81.8%) 10 (90.9%) 10 (90.9%)

10 (83.8%) 11 (100%) 9 (81.8%)

10 (90.9%) 10 (83.3%) 10 (83.8%)

Note: BPVS-II = British Picture Vocabulary Scales, 2nd Edition; DCD = developmental coordination disorder; SLI = specific language impairment; STM = short-term memory; WM = working memory.

BPVS-II standard scores for comparison. Table 2 presents the results of separate analyses for visuospatial short-term memory, visuospatial working memory, and BPVS-II standard score and an analysis with both visuospatial composite memory measures entered. Wilks’s lambda was significant for each of these functions: visuospatial short-term memory, λ(1) = .800, p = .037; visuospatial working memory, λ(2) = .627, p = .003; BPVS-II, λ(1) = .354, p < .001; visuospatial short-term and working memory, λ(2) = .604, p = .008. BPVS-II was the best discriminator, as expected given that the groups were selected on the basis of this measure. Performance on the visuospatial working memory task was sufficient to correctly assign group membership for 82% of both the SLI and DCD and typical language groups. This figure rose to 91% for the DCD and typical language group when the visuospatial short-term memory measure was included. This outcome establishes that visuospatial memory scores were effective at discriminating children in the two atypical groups, with lower scores typically characterizing the children with DCD. To evaluate the extent to which the memory measures differentiated the SLI group from an unmatched DCD group, a set of discriminant function analyses was conducted comparing the SLI group with the unselected DCD group, with the results presented in Table 2. In a stepwise discriminant function analysis with all of the composite memory measures entered, there were two significant functions: visuospatial working memory entered alone, λ(1, 1, 21) = .403, p < .001, and visuospatial and verbal working memory entered, λ(2, 1, 21) = .329, p < .001. With both working memory measures entered, 100% of

SLI (n = 11) Numeracy Reading Spelling DCD and typical language (n = 11) Numeracy Reading Spelling Full DCD (n = 23) Numeracy Reading Spelling

Visuospatial

STM

WM

STM

WM

.47 .38 .38

.42 .41 .35

.31 –.09 –.03

.74** .03 –.01

.72** .52 .51

.72** .65** .58

.69* .50 .45

.47 .71** .71**

.60** .54** .48*

.54** .30 .33

.62* .27 .26

.53** .43* .39

Note: DCD = developmental coordination disorder; SLI = specific language impairment; STM = short-term memory; WM = working memory. *p < .05. **p < .01.

the SLI group and 83% of the DCD group were correctly classified. This proved more accurate than the BPVS-II alone, which correctly differentiated 82% of the SLI group and 83% of the DCD group. It should be noted that in a corresponding discriminant function analysis comparing the SLI with the combined DCD groups (n = 23), visuospatial working memory was the only significant variable, with 91% of SLI children and 83% of DCD children correctly assigned.

Working Memory Profiles and Learning The two clinical groups in the present study were characterized by different working memory profiles: SLI deficits were found for verbal but not visuospatial measures of short-term and working memory, whereas the DCD groups had across-the-board deficits in both verbal and visuospatial short-term and working memory. The relationship between these working memory profiles and measures of learning was evaluated in a series of correlations. These analyses must be considered exploratory and be interpreted with caution because of the small number of participants and reduced score range typical of clinical groups. Table 3 presents correlation coefficients between the composite memory scores and attainment measures for the SLI, DCD and typical language, and full DCD (combined typical language and unselected, n = 23) groups. There was a striking difference between groups:

Alloway, Archibald / DCD and SLI

Correlations were generally higher and significant for the DCD groups than the SLI group, although this difference was most notable between the DCD and typical language and SLI groups. In addition, all of the memory measures were significantly correlated with at least one attainment measure for the DCD groups, whereas only the correlation between visuospatial working memory composite and numeracy reached significance for the SLI group. Consistent with their uniformly low working memory profile, correlations for the DCD groups were similar across the cognitive measures, ranging from approximately 0.5 to 0.7 for the DCD and typical language group and from 0.3 to 0.6 for the full DCD groups for both the verbal and visuospatial domains, short-term and working memory, and literacy and numeracy. There were indications of a domain difference for the SLI group, but the relationship appeared to be driven by areas of deficit rather than strength: Low but uniform correlations were found between verbal memory and attainment measures (.35–.47), whereas visuospatial memory was unrelated to two of the attainment measures (reading and spelling, r = .01–.09) and slightly (visuospatial short-term memory) or strongly (visuospatial working memory) correlated with numeracy (r = .31 and .74, respectively). It appears that the SLI group did not depend on their stronger visuospatial memory skills in literacy learning.

Discussion In this study, we compared the performance on shortterm and working memory measures of children with two different developmental pathologies, SLI and DCD. Children with SLI showed marked impairments on verbal but not visuospatial measures of short-term and working memory, whereas children with DCD had general deficits across verbal and visuospatial short-term and working memory. Performance on the visuospatial and verbal working memory measures successfully discriminated 100% of SLI and 83% of an unselected DCD group, a classification rate higher than a standard language measure. Visuospatial working memory alone was sufficient to classify at least 82% of a matched sample of DCD and SLI groups. The pattern of correlations between the memory and learning measures also differed between groups. Consistent with their uniformly low working memory profile, all of the memory measures were correlated with at least one test of attainment for the DCD group. The relationship between the memory and learning measures was less clear for the SLI group, with low correlations between the attainment measures and verbal memory and a high correlation between

9

numeracy and visuospatial working memory. One explanation for the absence of stronger relationships between memory performance and attainment measures could be that the range of scores for these atypical groups was restricted. Their depressed scores, coupled with the small sample sizes, could account for the absence of correlations found by other authors. This possibility should be tested in future studies. In addition to the language and motor differences that characterize children with DCD and SLI, the present findings indicate that differences exist also in their immediate memory profiles. The flat profile of DCD deficits in short-term and working memory across domains contrasted with the relatively domain-specific SLI impairments in verbal short-term and working memory. Although the groups achieved similar performance levels on measures of literacy and numeracy, they exhibited different associations between the memory and attainment measures. Perhaps because of the uniformly low memory skills, short-term and working memory were moderately associated with attainment measures across domains in the DCD group. The more interesting case was provided by the SLI group. Despite strengths in visuospatial memory, measures of visuospatial memory were not associated with any of the attainment measures except numeracy. In fact, short-term and working memory in the SLI group showed little relationship to reading and literacy, and the most consistent associations were with the poor verbal memory skills. It may be that verbal working memory measures are more closely associated with scholastic attainment than the visuospatial working memory measures included in the present study. The verbal short-term and working memory tasks in the present study did not, however, act as additional language measures, as has been suggested by some researchers (MacDonald & Christiansen, 2002; Snowling, Chiat, & Hulme, 1991). All of the groups performed at similarly low levels on these tasks, even though the groups differed widely in language status: the DCD and typical language group had average language skills, the unselected DCD group had low-normal language skills, and the SLI group had impaired language skills. These findings point to a commonality in deficits in these clinical groups, which may account for their similar learning profiles to some extent. Certainly, it is unclear from the present findings if the strengths in visuospatial memory in the SLI group place them at any noticeable advantage. It is commonly held that there is considerable heterogeneity among children who present with either DCD or SLI. In the present study, the children in the DCD group exhibited the widest score range on all of the screening measures, with minimum scores on the language measures

10

Journal of Learning Disabilities

falling below those of the SLI group. The SLI group, on the other hand, had no concomitant motor impairments. Thus, the DCD group represented a more heterogeneous group than the children with SLI in the present study. Nevertheless, the substantial heterogeneity among the DCD groups did not drastically alter performance on a variety of cognitive, memory, and attainment measures. A subset of children with DCD and typical language skills and an unselected DCD group were found to perform at comparably low levels on verbal short-term and working memory, visuospatial short-term and working memory, and reading, spelling and numeracy tasks (see also Alloway 2007; Alloway & Temple, 2007). These findings suggest that the current focus on heterogeneity with respect to memory skills and learning in developmental pathologies may be misplaced. It may be possible to relax selection criteria to include larger sample groups without introducing excessive variability. Given the average language profile of the DCD and typical language group, one possibility is that these children may also be classified as having a nonverbal learning disability (see Rourke & Tsatsanis, 1996). The profile of this disability includes average verbal IQ but substantially poorer nonverbal IQ skills. Children with this disability also struggle in tasks involving nonverbal problem solving and visual-spatial-organizational skills. On the surface, they appear to have a certain level of linguistic competence, as evidenced by their use of various sentence structures, strong memorization skills, and aboveaverage abilities in single-word reading and spelling. However, their competence in language is superficial, and they struggle most in tasks requiring the use of contextual information or sophisticated social competence. Closer inspection of the performance of children in the DCD and typical language group in the present study revealed striking contrasts between the two profiles. Specifically, they did not appear to have a deficit in nonverbal IQ skills (as measured by Raven’s Coloured Matrices); rather, they had marked impairments in single-word reading and spelling (as measured by the Reading and Spelling subtests of the Wechsler Objective Reading Dimensions, respectively) and memory (in both sentence memory and working memory, assessed by the Recalling Sentences subtest of the CELF-UK3 and the AWMA, respectively). This suggests that although the DCD and typical language group had relatively poor nonverbal skills compared with the SLI group, they exhibited distinctive skills from a nonverbal learning disability cohort. SLI and DCD differ in their principal domains of impairment, language and motor coordination, respectively. The present results indicate that in addition, these groups differ in their working memory profiles. Measures of verbal and visuospatial working memory, taken

together, are excellent group discriminators. Children with DCD performed at low levels on both verbal and visuospatial measures of short-term and working memory, whereas children with SLI exhibited deficits in verbal memory only. Despite different associations between working memory and measures of attainment for the children with DCD and those with SLI, the groups performed similarly in numeracy, reading, and spelling. It appeared that verbal memory measures were more closely related to learning outcomes for both atypical groups, although visuospatial working memory was strongly related to numeracy as well.

Note 1. The pattern of performance growth as a function of age, which plots the z scores for each year group from 4 to 11 years in the AWMA sample, was compared with the WMTB-C sample. The tests indicate broadly similar developmental functions, with performance increasing across each year group.

References Alloway, T. P. (2006). Working memory and children with developmental co-ordination disorders. In T. P. Alloway & S. E. Gathercole (Eds.), Working memory and neurodevelopmental conditions (pp. 161–187). New York: Psychology Press. Alloway, T. P. (2007a). The Automated Working Memory Assessment. London: Pearson Assessment. Alloway, T. P. (2007b). Working memory, literacy and numeracy in children with developmental coordination disorder. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 96, 20–36. Alloway, T. P., Gathercole, S. E., Adams, A. M., Willis, C., Eaglen, R., & Lamont, E. (2005). Working memory and other cognitive skills as predictors of progress towards early learning goals at school entry. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 23, 417–426. Alloway, T. P., Gathercole, S. E., Willis, C., & Adams, A. M. (2004). A structural analysis of working memory and related cognitive skills in early childhood. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 87, 85–106. Alloway, T. P., Gathercole, S. E., Willis, C., & Adams, A. M. (2005). Working memory and special educational needs. Educational and Child Psychology, 22, 56–67. Alloway, T. P., & Temple, K. J. (2007). A comparison of working memory profiles and learning in children with developmental coordination disorder and moderate learning difficulties. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 21, 473–487. American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. Archibald, L.M.D., & Gathercole, S. E. (2006a). Short-term and working memory in children with specific language impairments. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 41, 675–693. Archibald, L.M.D., & Gathercole, S. E. (2006b). Visuospatial immediate memory in specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, & Hearing Research, 49, 265–277. Archibald, L.M.D., & Gathercole, S. E. (2007). The complexities of complex span: Specifying working memory deficits in SLI. Journal of Memory and Language, 57, 177–194.

Alloway, Archibald / DCD and SLI Arvedson, P. J. (2002). Young children with specific language impairment and their numerical cognition. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 45, 970–982. Baddeley, A. D. (1996). Exploring the central executive. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49A, 5–28. Baddeley, A. D. (2000). The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 417–423. Baddeley, A. D., Gathercole, S. E., & Papagno, C. (1998). The phonological loop as a language learning device. Psychological Review, 105, 158–173. Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. In G. A. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (Vol. 8, pp. 47–89). New York: Academic Press. Bavin, E. L., Wilson, P., Maruff, P., & Sleeman, F. (2005). Spatio-visual memory of children with specific language impairment: Evidence for generalized processing problems. International Journal of Language and Communicative Disorders, 40, 319–332. Bishop D. (1982). Test for Reception of Grammar. London: Medical Research Council. Bishop, D.V.M., & Adams, J. A. (1990). A prospective study of the relationship between specific language impairment, phonological disorders and reading retardation. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 31, 1027–1050. Botting, N., & Conti-Ramsden, G., (2001). Non-word repetition and language development in children with specific language impairment (SLI). International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 36, 421–432. Bull, R., & Scerif, G. (2001). Executive functioning as a predictor of children’s mathematics ability. Shifting, inhibition and working memory. Developmental Neuropsychology, 19, 273–293. Cain, K., Oakhill, J., & Bryant, P. (2004). Children’s reading comprehension ability: Concurrent prediction by working memory, verbal ability and component skills. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 31–42. Catts, H., Fey, M., Tomblin, J., & Zhang, X. (2002). A longitudinal investigation of reading outcomes in children with language impairments. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 45, 1142–1157. Choudhury, N., & Benasich, A. A. (2003). A family aggregation study: The influence of family history and other risk factors on language development. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 46, 261–271. Conrad, R., & Hull, A. J. (1964). Information, acoustic confusion and memory span. British Journal of Psychology, 55, 429–432. Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). Individual differences in working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal learning and Verbal Behavior, 19, 450–466. De Jong, P. F. (1998). Working memory deficits of reading disabled children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 70, 75–96. Dewey, D., Kaplan, B. J., Crawford, S. G., & Wilson, B. N. (2002). Developmental coordination disorder: Associated problems in attention, learning, and psychosocial adjustment. Human Movement Science, 21, 905–918. Dollaghan, C., & Campbell, T. F. (1998). Nonword repetition and child language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 41, 1136–1146. Donlan, C., & Gourlay, S. (1999). The importance of non-verbal skills in the acquisition of place-value knowledge: Evidence from normally-developing and language-impaired children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 17, 1–19. Duff, S. C., & Logie, R. H. (1999). Storage and processing in visuospatial working memory. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 40, 251–259.

11

Dunn, L. M., Dunn, L. M., Whetton, C. W., & Burley, J. (1997). The British Picture Vocabulary Scales (2nd ed.). Windsor, UK: NFER Nelson. Edwards, J., & Lahey, M. (1998). Nonword repetitions of children with specific language impairment: Exploration of some explanations for their inaccuracies. Applied Psycholinguistics, 19, 279–309. Ellis Weismer, S. (1991). Hypothesis testing abilities of language impaired children. Journal of Speech & Hearing Research, 34, 1329–1338. Ellis Weismer, S., Evans, J., & Hesketh, L. (1999). An examination of working memory capacity in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42, 1249–1260. Ellis Weismer, S., Tomblin, J. B., Zhang, X., Buckwalter, P., Gaura Chynoweth, J., & Jones, M. (2000). Nonword repetition performance in school-age children with and without language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 43, 865–878. Fazio, B. B. (1996). Serial memory in children with specific language impairment: Examining specific content areas for assessment and intervention. Topics in Language Disorders, 17, 58–71. Flax, J. F., Realpe-Bonilla, T., Hirsch, L. S., Brzustowicz, L. M., Bartlett, C. W., Tallal, P. (2003) Specific language impairment in families: Evidence for co-occurrence with reading impairments. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 46, 530–543. Gathercole, S. E., Alloway, T. P., Willis, C., & Adams, A. M. (2006). Working memory in children with literacy and mathematical difficulties. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 93, 265–281. Gathercole, S., & Baddeley, A. (1990). Phonological memory deficits in language disordered children: Is there a causal connection? Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 336–360. Gathercole, S. E., & Pickering, S. J. (2001). Working memory deficits in children with special educational needs. British Journal of Special Education, 28, 89–97. Graham, N. C. (1980). Memory constraints in language deficiency. In F. M. Jones (Ed.), Language disability in children (pp. 69–94). Baltimore, MD: University Park Press. Henderson, S. E., & Sugden, D. A. (1992). Movement Assessment Battery for Children. Sidcup, UK: Psychological Corporation. Henderson, S. E., & Sugden, D. A. (1996). Movement Assessment Battery Checklist. Sidcup, UK: Psychological Corporation. Hill, E. L. (2001). Non-specific nature of specific language impairment: A review of the literature with regard to concomitant motor impairments. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 36, 149–171. Iversen, S., Berg, K., Ellertsen, B., & Tonnessen, F. E. (2005). Motor coordination difficulties in a municipality group and in a clinical sample of poor readers. Dyslexia, 11, 217–231. Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1992). A capacity theory of comprehension: Individual differences in working memory. Psychological Review, 99, 122–149. Kadesjo, B., & Gillberg, C. (1999). Developmental coordination disorder in Swedish 7-year-old children. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 38, 820–828. Kaplan, B. J., Wilson, B. N., Dewey, D. M., & Crawford, S. G. (1998). DCD may not be a discrete disorder. Human Movement Science, 17, 471–490. Lee, K. M., & Kang, S. Y. (2002). Arithmetic operation and working memory: Differential suppression in dual tasks. Cognition, 83, 63–68.

12

Journal of Learning Disabilities

Leonard, L. (1998). Children with specific language impairments. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. MacDonald, M. C., & Christiansen, M. H. (2002). Reassessing working memory: A comment on Just & Carpenter (1992) and Waters & Caplan (1996). Psychological Review, 109, 35–54. Mandich, A., & Polatajko, H. J. (2003). Developmental coordination disorder: Mechanisms, measurement and management. Human Movement Science, 22, 407–411. Mayringer, H., & Wimmer, H. (2000). Pseudoname learning by German-speaking children with dyslexia: Evidence for a phonological learning deficit. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 75, 116–133. McLean, J. F., & Hitch, G. H. (1999). Working memory impairments in children with specific mathematics learning difficulties. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 74, 240–260. Miller, C. A., Kail, R., Leonard, L. B., & Tomblin, J. B. (2001). Speed of processing in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44, 416–433. Nation, K., Adams, J. W., Bowyer-Crane, C. A., & Snowling, M. J. (1999). Working memory deficits in poor comprehenders reflect underlying language impairments. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 73, 139–158. Nelson, L., Kamhi, A., & Apel, K. (1987). Cognitive strengths and weaknesses in language-impaired children: One more look. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 52, 36–45. Passolunghi, M. C., & Siegel, L. S. (2001). Short term memory, working memory, and inhibitory control in children with specific arithmetic learning disabilities. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 80, 44–57. Pickering, S. J., & Gathercole, S. E. (2001). Working Memory Test Battery for Children. London: Harcourt Assessment. Pickering, S. J., & Gathercole, S. E. (2004). Distinctive working memory profiles in children with varying special educational needs. Educational Psychology, 24, 393–408. Piek, J. P., & Dyck, M. J. (2004). Sensory-motor deficits in children with developmental coordination disorder, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and autistic disorder. Human Movement Science, 23, 475–488. Quinn, J. G. (1994). Towards a clarification of spatial processing. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 47A, 465–480. Raven, J. C., Court, J. H., & Raven, J. (1986). Raven’s Coloured Matrices. London: H. K. Lewis. Records, N., & Tomblin, B. (1994). Clinical decision making: Describing the decision rules of practicing speech-language pathologists. American Speech-Language-Hearing Association, 37, 144–156. Rourke, B., & Tsatsanis, K. (1996). Syndrome of nonverbal learning disabilities: Psycholinguistic assets and deficits. Topics in Language Disorders, 16, 30–44. Semel, E., Wiig, E. H., & Secord, W. A. (1995). Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals–3. San Antonio, Texas: Psychological Corporation. Shah, P., & Miyake, A. (1996). The separability of working memory resources for spatial thinking and language processing: An individual differences approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 125, 4–27. Siegel, L. S., & Ryan, E. B. (1989). The development of working memory in normally achieving and subtypes of learning disabled children. Child Development, 60, 973–980.

Seigneuric, A., Ehrlich, M.-F., Oakhill, J. V., & Yuill, N. M. (2000). Working memory resources and children’s reading comprehension. Reading and Writing, 13, 81–103. Smyth, M. M., Pearson, N. A., & Pendleton, L. R. (1988). Movement and working memory: Patterns and positions in space. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 40A, 497–514. Smyth, M. M., & Scholey, K. A. (1996). The relationship between articulation time and memory performance in verbal and visuospatial tasks. British Journal of Psychology, 87, 179–191. Snowling, M., Chiat, S., & Hulme, C. (1991). Words, nonwords and phonological processes: Some comments on Gathercole, Willis, Emslie & Baddeley. Applied Psycholinguistics, 12, 369–373. Swanson, H. L. (1994). Short-term memory and working memory— Do both contribute to our understanding of academic achievement in children and adults with learning disabilities? Journal of Learning Disabilities, 27, 34–50. Swanson, H. L., & Berninger, V. W. (1995). Working memory as a source of individuals differences in children’s writing. In E. Butterfield (Ed.), Children’s writing: Toward a process theory of skilled writing (pp. 31–35). Greenwich, CT: JAI. Swanson, H. L., & Sachse-Lee, C. (2001). Mathematical problem solving and working memory in children with learning disabilities: Both executive and phonological processes are important. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 79, 294–321. Swanson, H. L., & Saez, L. (2003). Memory difficulties in children and adults with learning disabilities. In L. Swanson et al. (Eds.), Handbook of learning disabilities (pp. 182–198). New York: Guilford. Tomblin, J. B., Records, N. L., Buckwalter, P., Zhang, X., Smith, E., & O’Brien, M. (1997). Prevalence of specific language impairment in kindergarten children. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 40, 1245–1260. Visser, J. (2003).Developmental coordination disorder: A review of research on subtypes and comorbidities. Human Movement Science, 22, 479–493. Wechsler, D. (1993). Wechsler Objective Reading Dimensions. London: Psychological Corporation. Wechsler, D. (1996). Wechsler Objective Numerical Dimensions. London: Psychological Corporation. Wiig, E. H., & Semel, E. M. (1976). Language disabilities in children and adolescents. Columbus, OH: Charles E. Merrill. Williams, K. T. (1997). Expressive Vocabulary Test. New York: American Guidance Service. Wilson, P. H. (2005). Approaches to assessment and treatment of children with DCD: An evaluative review. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 806–823.

Tracy Packiam Alloway, PhD, is a senior research psychologist based at the University of Durham. She is interested in how working memory affects learning in both typical and atypical populations (attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, autistic spectrum disorder, developmental coordination difficulties [dyspraxia], and dyscalculia). Lisa Archibald is an assistant professor in the School of Communication Sciences and Disorders at the University of Western Ontario. She studies links between memory and language processes in individuals with communication disorders. In particular, she is interested in the extent to which limitations in working memory may impair language learning in children.

Suggest Documents