Articles in PresS. J Neurophysiol (February 8, 2012). doi:10.1152/jn.01051.2011
1 2
Bridging the gap: global disparity processing in the human
3
visual cortex
4 5
Benoit R. Cottereau
6
Stanford University, Stanford, CA, USA
7
Suzanne P. McKee
8
Smith-Kettlewell Eye Research Institute, San Francisco, CA, USA
9
Anthony M. Norcia
10
Stanford University, Stanford, CA, USA
11 12 13
Corresponding Author: Benoit R. Cottereau
14 15
Adress: Department of Psychology | Jordan Hall, Building 01-420 | Stanford
16
University | 450 Serra Mall | Stanford, CA 94305
17
Phone number: 650-725-2440
18
e-mail address:
[email protected]
Fax number: 650-725-1876
19 20
Running Title: Global disparity processing in the Human Visual System
21 22
Acknowledgments
23
This work was supported by National Eye Institute grants R01 EY018875, the Smith-
24
Kettlewell Eye Research Institute and a Walt and Lilly Disney Amblyopia Research
25
Award from Research to Prevent Blindness.
Copyright © 2012 by the American Physiological Society.
26 27
Abstract
28
The human stereoscopic system is remarkable in its ability to utilize widely
29
separated features as references to support fine depth discrimination. In a search for
30
possible neural substrates of this ability, we recorded high-density EEG and used a
31
distributed inverse technique to estimate population-level disparity responses in five
32
ROIs: V1, V3A, hMT+, V4 and LOC. The stimulus was a central modulating disk
33
surrounded by a correlated ‘reference’ annulus presented in the fixation plane. We
34
varied a gap separating the disk from the annulus parametrically from 0 to 5.5
35
degrees as a test of long-range disparity integration. In the V1, LOC and hMT+ ROIs,
36
the responses with gaps larger than 0.5 degrees were equal to those obtained in a
37
control condition where the surround was composed of uncorrelated noise (no
38
reference). By contrast, in the V4 and V3A ROIs, responses with gaps as large as 5.5
39
degrees were still significantly higher than the control. As a test of the spatial
40
distribution of the disparity-reference information, we manipulated the properties of
41
the stimulus by placing noise between the center and the surround or throughout the
42
surround. The V3A ROI was particularly sensitive to disparity noise between the
43
center and annulus regions, suggesting an important contribution of disparity edge
44
detectors in this ROI.
45 46
Key words: binocular vision, disparity, EEG source modeling, long-range
47
interactions.
48 49 50
Introduction
51
The general characteristics of disparity-tuned neurons in V1 are now well-
52
established, thanks to more than three decades of single cell studies in awake-
53
behaving primates (Poggio and Fischer, 1977; Cumming and DeAngelis, 2001;
54
Prince et al. 2002; Parker, 2007; Roe et al, 2007). Some V1 neurons are remarkably
55
sensitive to small changes in disparity, certainly sensitive enough to account for
56
primate stereoacuity (Prince et al, 2000). Yet surprisingly, awake-behaving primates
57
are unable to use the responses of these neurons to detect small changes in the
58
disparity of an isolated target. Monkeys, like humans, require a second reference
59
target in the visual field to respond precisely to changes in the disparity of a test
60
target (Prince et al., 2000; Westheimer, 1979; McKee et al., 1990). Prince et al.
61
(2000) concluded that a reference target was needed to remove ambiguity about
62
whether the change in disparity was produced by a stimulus change or a change in
63
convergence.
64
If disambiguation were the primary function of reference targets, then any
65
reference would suffice. However, psychophysical evidence shows that there are
66
constraints on what serves as an optimum reference. For example, Farell (2006)
67
found that stereoacuity for a Gabor target was best when the reference grating had
68
the same orientation as the test. McKee et al. (1990) measured stereoacuity as a
69
function of the distance separating test and reference lines, and found that
70
stereoacuity with a reference was better than unreferenced stereoacuity for distances
71
up to 5 degrees. Similarly, Read et al. (2010) found that stereoacuity for a referenced
72
disk was superior to an unreferenced disk for distances as great as 10 degrees.
73
These psychophysical constraints suggest that the calculation of disparity differences
74
depends on specialized neural wiring, rather than on a generalized differencing
75
operation that occurs between any two targets in the visual field.
76
Here, we used high-density EEG source-imaging to study how the neural
77
response to changes in disparity was affected by the separation of the disparity-
78
modulated stimulus from a fixed reference disparity. In human subjects, we
79
measured population responses in five visual ROIs (V1, V4, V3A, hMT+, and LOC).
80
Our previous work has shown that the presence of an abutting reference stimulus
81
greatly enhances the response to disparity modulation in extra-striate areas
82
(Cottereau et al. 2011a, Cottereau et al., 2012). How does this enhancement depend
83
on the distance to the reference stimulus and how does this dependence compare to
84
comparable psychophysical measurements? Our measurements demonstrate that for
85
large separations between center and surround (i.e. greater than 0.5 degrees), areas
86
V1, LOC and hMT+ have responses equivalent to those of the control condition
87
where the surround was composed of uncorrelated noise. In contrast, areas V3A and
88
V4 have significantly larger responses than the control for gaps up to 5.5°. These
89
areas therefore contain neural populations that integrate disparity information over
90
long distances and these populations may support long-range perceptual
91
judgements. In a second experiment, we introduced noise into the space between
92
center and surround or throughout the surround. We showed that responses in area
93
V3A are specifically affected by the presence of noise between the center and the
94
surround. This result suggests that neurons acting as disparity edge detectors are
95
present in this ROI.
96 97
Methods and Materials
98
The basic stimulus (see Figure 1) consisted of a central disk surrounded by an
99
annulus; both were composed of dynamic random dots that changed positions every
100
47 msec (21.25 Hz).
The diameter of the central disk was 4 deg. Its disparity
101
alternated between 12.4 arcmin (uncrossed) and 0 arcmin every 470 msec (i.e. the
102
disparity was driven by a square wave at 2.12 Hz) in a steady-state Visual Evoked
103
Potential (SSVEP) paradigm. Vergence cannot follow disparity changes at this
104
temporal frequency, so it cannot null out the changes in stimulus disparity (Rashbass
105
and Westheimer, 1961; Erkelens and Collewijn, 1985). The disparity of the annular
106
surround, when binocularly correlated, remained constant during a trial. For all
107
studies, the EEG stimulus presentation lasted 10.35s (that is 22 cycles of the disk
108
disparity modulation); the first 940ms (i.e. the first two cycles) of the data record was
109
discarded to avoid start-up transients, leading to 9.41s trials (i.e. 20 cycles of the disk
110
disparity modulation). Trials for all conditions were run interspersed in random order
111
in 10-minute blocks. The blocks were repeated four times, producing a total of 20
112
trials per condition.
113 114 115
%%%%%% INSERT FIGURE 1 ABOUT HERE %%%%%%%%%%%%
116 117
We used two different types of stereoscopes for these studies: a mirror
118
stereoscope, and a beam-splitter based system. In the mirror stereoscope, the stimuli
119
were presented on a pair of matched SONY monitors (Multiscan GS 220) viewed at a
120
distance of 77 cm. At this viewing distance, the outer dimensions of the screens were
121
23 deg high by 18 deg wide. The monitors and mirrors were angled for natural
122
convergence at this viewing distance. The contrast of the bright dots was 90%. In the
123
interval between trials, the screen was filled with bright static dots at zero disparity to
124
prevent changes in light adaptation. In the beam-splitter system, orthogonally
125
polarized images from two matched Sony Trinitron monitors (Model 110GS), were
126
combined via a beam-splitter and viewed through appropriately oriented polarized
127
filters placed immediately in front of the eyes. Each eye could see the image on only
128
one screen; the viewing distance was 82 cm and the contrast of the bright dots was
129
90%. Each separate experiment described below was, of course, conducted on a
130
single stereoscope; this first experiment was performed in the mirror stereoscope,
131
and the second one in the beam-splitter system. We checked whether the difference
132
in stereoscopes affected our most sensitive measure, namely psychophysical
133
stereoacuity thresholds. We found that the thresholds were not significantly different
134
for the two types of stereoscopes. As will be apparent in the results below, the
135
general pattern of EEG responses for comparable conditions was also unaffected by
136
the change in stereoscopes.
137 138
Subjects
139
Twenty-one subjects (14 males, 7 females, age range 20-69 years)
140
participated in the recording sessions. Thirteen subjects (9 males, 4 females) were
141
involved in the first EEG experiment, 12 subjects (8 males, 5 females) participated in
142
the second one and 4 subjects (2 males, 2 females) did the psychophysical
143
measurements. All the subjects were volunteers, with normal stereopsis and normal
144
or corrected-to-normal visual acuity. All subjects were given instructions and detailed
145
information about the experiments. They provided written informed consent before
146
participating in the study in accordance with Helsinki Declaration; the human subjects
147
review committee of Smith-Kettlewell Eye Research Institute approved the study.
148 149
Main Experiments
150
1. Different gaps
151
In six interspersed conditions, we measured the SSVEP to uncrossed disparity
152
modulation of the central disk. The outer ring of the surround had an eccentricity of
153
16 degrees (dot density: 20 dots/deg^2). The distance between the outer ring of the
154
center and the inner ring of the surround varied from 0 to 5.5 degrees (see Figure 1).
155
In a control condition, the surround consisted of uncorrelated disparity noise (without
156
a gap). This stimulus was first used by Prince et al (2000) and has been used in our
157
previous studies (Cottereau et al., 2011a; Cottereau et al., 2012) to measure a
158
response driven mainly by neurons tuned to absolute disparity.
159
For the six gap conditions, we presented a pair of nonius lines and a
160
binocularly visible fixation point superimposed on the center of the disk-annulus
161
stimulus. Subjects were asked to keep the nonius lines aligned during a recording
162
trial. We eliminated the nonius lines and the fixation point for the uncorrelated
163
surround condition to remove all immediate reference stimuli. In our last study
164
(Cottereau et al., 2012), we tested if the absence of nonius line and fixation point
165
during the stimulation produced a fixation bias for an equivalent condition. A
166
psychophysical nonius alignment test demonstrated that if this bias existed, it was
167
negligible and not sufficient to influence our results. The direct comparison between
168
the EEG responses obtained with and without fixation point for this same condition
169
pointed toward the same conclusion (Cottereau et al., 2012).
170 171
2. Noise surround
172
In a second experiment, we tested the influence of the nature of the space
173
between center and surround on the EEG responses. The outer diameter of the
174
surround annulus was diminished to 7.48° (dot density: 30 dots/deg^2). In the first
175
two conditions, the distance between the outer ring of the center and the inner ring of
176
the surround was 1°. The stimulus dimensions for these two conditions were chosen
177
so that the area of the surround annulus and the area between center and surround
178
were equal. The space between the center and surround was either empty (i.e. a
179
gap) in condition 1 or filled with uncorrelated noise in condition 2. In a third condition,
180
the surround touched the center but was filled with a mixture composed of 50% of
181
correlated dots and 50% of uncorrelated noise. The noise amount was therefore the
182
same in conditions 2 and 3. Nonius lines and a fixation point were displayed in the
183
three conditions (see the ‘different gaps’ section).
184 185 186
3. Psychophysical Thresholds We
also
measured
disparity
modulation
thresholds psychophysically.
187
Observers judged in which of two intervals the disparity of a 4 deg central disk was
188
modulated by a tiny amount in an uncrossed direction. The stimuli were presented for
189
472 msec, equal to one period of our EEG presentations. The stimuli were presented
190
in a mirror stereoscope and viewed at a distance of 86 cm. Four different modulation
191
values were presented interspersed in a random sequence in a 100 trial block. We
192
fitted a Weibull function to the percentages correct for these four values and
193
estimated threshold as the disparity modulation corresponding to 81.6% correct. We
194
measured thresholds for each subject and condition in two to four separate blocks
195
(200 - 400 trials total); the thresholds were averaged from the blocks and are plotted
196
as sensitivities (1/ threshold) in Figure 3.
197 198
EEG signal acquisition and preprocessing
199 200
The electroencephalogram (EEG) data were collected with 128-sensor
201
HydroCell Sensor Nets (Electrical Geodesics, Eugene OR) and were band-pass
202
filtered from 0.1 to 200 Hz. Following each experimental session, the 3D locations of
203
all electrodes and three major fiducials (nasion, left and right peri-auricular points)
204
were digitized using a 3Space Fastrack 3-D digitizer (Polhemus, Colchester, VT). For
205
all observers, the 3D digitized locations were used to co-register the electrodes to
206
their T1-weighted anatomical MRI scans and to construct the EEG forward model
207
(see the “Forward modeling of the current sources” section below). Raw data were
208
evaluated off line according to a sample-by-sample thresholding procedure to remove
209
noisy sensors that were replaced by the average of the six nearest spatial neighbors.
210
On average, less than five per cent of the electrodes were substituted. These
211
electrodes were mainly localized in the front of the head or near the ears. The
212
substitutions had a negligible impact on our results, as recordings at occipital and
213
parietal locations mainly drive our estimates of the responses within the visual areas.
214
After this operation, the EEG was re-referenced to the common average of all the
215
sensors. Within each 9.41s trial, the data were segmented into five 1.82s long
216
epochs (i.e. each of these epochs was exactly 4 cycles of the disk disparity
217
modulation). EEG epochs that contained a large percentage of data samples
218
exceeding a noise threshold (depending on the subject and ranging between 25 and
219
50 micro volts) were excluded from the analysis on a sensor-by-sensor basis. This
220
was typically the case for epochs containing artifacts such as blinks or eye
221
movements. Finally, a Fourier analysis was applied on every remaining epoch using
222
a discrete Fourier transform with a rectangular window. For each frequency bin, the
223
Fourier coefficients were then averaged across all the epochs and all the trials.
224
These average Fourier coefficients were therefore obtained from up to 100 (5 epochs
225
* 20 trials) values.
226 227
Structural and Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (MRI)
228 229
Structural and functional MRI scanning was conducted at 3T (Siemens Tim
230
Trio, Erlangen, Germany) using a 12-channel head coil. We acquired a T1-weighted
231
MRI dataset (3-D MP-RAGE sequence, 0.8 x 0.8 x 0.8 mm3 and a 3-D T2-weighted
232
dataset (SE sequence at 1 x 1 x 1 mm3 resolution) for tissue segmentation and
233
registration with the functional scans. For fMRI, we employed a single-shot, gradient-
234
echo planar imaging (EPI) sequence (TR/TE = 2000/28 ms, flip angle 80, 126
235
volumes per run) with a voxel size of 1.7 x 1.7 x 2 mm3 (128 x 128 acquisition matrix,
236
220 mm FOV, bandwidth 1860 Hz/pixel, echo spacing 0.71 ms). We acquired 30
237
slices without gaps, positioned in the transverse-to-coronal plane approximately
238
parallel to the corpus callosum and covering the whole cerebrum. Once per session,
239
a 2-D SE T1-weighted volume was acquired with the same slice specifications as the
240
functional series in order to facilitate registration of the fMRI data to the anatomical
241
scan. The general procedures for these scans (head stabilization, visual display
242
system, etc.) are standard and have been described in detail elsewhere (Brewer et
243
al. 2005). The FreeSurfer software package (http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu) was
244
used to extract both gray/white and gray/cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) boundaries. These
245
surfaces can have different curvatures. In particular, the gray/white boundary has
246
sharp gyri (the curvature changes rapidly) and smooth sulci (slowly changing surface
247
curvature), while the gray/CSF boundary is the inverse, with smooth gyri and sharp
248
sulci. In order to avoid these discontinuities, we generated a surface partway
249
between these two boundaries that has gyri and sulci with approximately equal
250
curvature. This “midgray” cortical surface consisted in a triangular tessellation of
251
20,484 regularly spaced vertices and was used to define the visual areas and the
252
source space for the EEG current modeling (see the two sections below).
253 254
Visual area definition
255 256
Retinotopic field mapping using rotating wedges and expanding/contracting
257
rings produced Regions-of-Interest (ROIs) defined by the visual cortical areas V1,
258
V2v, V2d, V3v, V3d, V3A, and V4 in each hemisphere (Tootell and Hadjikhani 2001;
259
Wade et al. 2002). ROIs corresponding to hMT+ were identified using low contrast
260
motion stimuli similar to those described by Huk and Heeger (2002).
261
The Lateral Occipital Complex (LOC) was defined using a block-design fMRI
262
localizer scan. During this scan, the observers viewed blocks of images depicting
263
common objects (12s/block) alternating with blocks containing scrambled versions of
264
the same objects. The stimuli were those used in a previous study (Kourtzi and
265
Kanwisher, 2000). The regions activated by these scans included an area lying
266
between the V1/V2/V3 foveal confluence and hMT+ that we identified as LOC. This
267
definition covers almost all regions (e.g. V4d, LOC, LOp) that have previously been
268
identified as lying within object-responsive lateral occipital cortex (Kourtzi and
269
Kanwisher, 2000; Tootell and Hadjikhani, 2001).
270 271
Forward modeling of the current source
272 273
For each subject, the EEG source space was given by his “midgray” cortical
274
surface tessellation (see above) and consisted in 20,484 regularly spaced vertices.
275
The distance between connected vertices was on average 3.7 mm, with a standard
276
deviation of 1.5 mm, range .1–11 mm. Current dipoles were placed at each of these
277
vertices. Their orientations were constrained to be orthogonal to the cortical surface
278
to diminish the number of parameters to be estimated in the inverse procedure
279
(Hämäläinen et al., 1993). The FSL toolbox (http://www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/) was used
280
to segment from the individual T1 and T2 weighted MRI scans, contiguous volume
281
regions for the inner skull, outer skull and scalp. These MRI volumes were then
282
converted into inner skull, outer skull, and scalp surfaces (Smith 2002; Smith et al.,
283
2004) that defined the boundaries between the brain/CSF and the skull, the skull and
284
the scalp, the scalp and the air. The source space, the 3D electrode locations, and
285
the individually defined boundaries were then combined using the MNE software
286
package
287
characterize the electric field propagation using a 3-compartment boundary element
288
method (BEM) (Hämäläinen and Sarvas, 1989). The resulting forward model is linear
289
and links the activity of the 20,484 cortical sources to the voltages recorded by our
290
EEG electrodes.
(http://www.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/martinos/userInfo/data/sofMNE.php)
to
291 292
Inverse modeling constrained by the visual ROIs
293 294
Cortical current density estimates of the neural responses were obtained from
295
an L2 minimum-norm inverse of the forward model described above (Hämäläinen et
296
al., 1993). We used the definition of the visual ROIs to constrain these estimates by
297
modifying the source-covariance matrix. Our aim was to decrease the tendency of
298
the minimum-norm procedure to smooth activity over very large surfaces and across
299
different functional areas. Two modifications were applied: 1) we increased the
300
variance allowed within the visual areas by a factor of two relative to other vertices
301
and 2) we enforced a local correlation constraint within each area using the first and
302
second order neighborhoods on the cortical tesselation with a weighting function
303
equal to 0.5 for the first order and 0.25 for the second. This correlation constraint
304
therefore respects both retinotopy and areal boundaries and permitted us
305
dissociate the signals from different areas unlike other smoothing methods such as
306
LORETA that apply the same smoothing rule throughout cortex (Pascual-Marqui et
307
al., 1994). The details of this approach can be found in Cottereau et al., (In press).
to
308 309
ROI-based analysis of the Steady-state Visual Evoked Potential (SSVEP)
310 311
For each subject and condition, our inverse approach was applied to the
312
average Fourier coefficients (see the “EEG signal acquisition and preprocessing”
313
section) and led to an estimation of these coefficients for each source of the cortical
314
tessellation. Within each functionally defined ROI, the coefficients were then
315
averaged across all the sources belonging to the area. We were particularly
316
interested in their magnitude (i.e. their norm) at the odd (first and third) components
317
of the steady-state frequency (2.12 Hz and 6.36 Hz). To take into account the
318
difference of noise levels between the recordings from each of our subjects (Viallatte
319
et al., 2009), we computed the signal-to-noise ratio (SNR) and divided these values
320
by the root-mean square of the associated noise which is defined for a given
321
frequency f by the average amplitude of the two neighbor frequencies (i.e. f – δf and f
322
+ δf where δf gives the frequency resolution of the Fourier analysis). The
323
corresponding SNRs are presented in decibels (20 X log10).
324 325
Cross-Talk
326 327
In a previous paper (Cottereau et al., 2011a), we described how we estimated
328
the theoretical crosstalk among visual areas for a specific EEG study. Crosstalk
329
refers to the neural activity generated in other areas that is attributed to a particular
330
ROI, due to the smoothing of the electric field by the head volume. In brief, for each
331
subject, we simulated the cross-talk by placing sources in one ROI and estimating
332
their contribution to other ROIs, using the same forward and inverse methods
333
described above. The global crosstalk matrix (i.e. averaged across all the subjects
334
who participated in our EEG experiments) is shown in Figure 2 for seven ROIs (V1,
335
V2, V3, V4, LOC, V3A & hMT+); the crosstalk magnitude shown in the matrix is
336
proportional to activity originating in the ROI where the crosstalk is being estimated.
337 338 339
%%%%%%%%%%% INSERT FIGURE 2 ABOUT HERE %%%%%%%%%%
340 341
From our simulations, it was apparent that there was significant crosstalk in areas V2
342
and V3 (the last two rows of the matrix). For this reason, we excluded these two ROIs
343
from our analysis and focused on V1, V3A, V4, hMT+, and the LOC. These areas are
344
more widely separated and their estimated activities are therefore more reliable.
345
Within this subset of ROIs, the biggest crosstalk comes from a V1 contribution to V3A
346
(55%), and a V3A contribution to V4 (50%). We discuss the influence of crosstalk
347
coming from other regions that were not defined using fMRI in the “Effect of the gap”
348
section of the results below.
349 350 351
Statistical analysis
352
Repeated measure analyses of variance (ANOVAs) were performed on the
353
SNRs at the odd harmonics. We specifically tested effects of conditions, of ROIs and
354
their interaction. Significant effects were followed-up by post-hoc pairwise Wilcoxon
355
sign-ranks tests. The validity of the ANOVAs (equal variance hypothesis) was
356
controlled using Mauchly’s sphericity test. The sphericity test showed no significant
357
effect, so we did not correct the p-values of the ANOVA.
358 359
Results
360 361
Psychophysics:
362 363
We measured stereoacuity thresholds for disparity modulation as a function of gap
364
size in 4 subjects. The associated sensitivities (i.e. 1 / Threshold) are displayed in
365
Figure 3.
366 367 368
%%%%%% INSERT FIGURE 3 ABOUT HERE %%%%%%%%%%%%
369 370
For all subjects, performance decreased when the gap was greater than one degree.
371
Interestingly, three subjects were better for small gap values than without any
372
separation, suggesting that they were uncertain about the location of the central disk
373
in the absence of the demarcation provided by the small gap. Indeed, the edge of the
374
disk was invisible to most observers at behavioral threshold in the zero gap condition;
375
instead a central region of the disk appeared to dip slightly but at an unpredictable
376
location from trial to trial. It is well known that uncertainty can reduce sensitivity
377
(Green & Swets, 1966; Pelli, 1985) and that the addition of features that remove
378
uncertainty about target location, such as the small gap surrounding the disk in this
379
case, can improve sensitivity. For subjects RF and SPM, we were also able to
380
measure a threshold for the control condition (uncorrelated surround). The two other
381
subjects were unable to detect the change in disparity in the absence of the disparity
382
reference. For all four subjects, the thresholds with a gap of 5.5 deg separating the
383
disk from the correlated surround (the reference) were significantly better than the
384
thresholds for the uncorrelated noise condition, but were elevated from the best value
385
by a factor of 3 to 9, as shown by the decline in sensitivity apparent in Figure 3. The
386
next section examines where these sensitivities are localized in visual cortex.
387 388
SSVEP data:
389 390
Figure 4 displays the amplitude spectrum for all the electrodes averaged across trials
391
and subjects. Two conditions are presented, one where no gap separated the center
392
and surround (0 deg) and another with a 4° gap.
393 394
%%%%%% INSERT FIGURE 4 ABOUT HERE %%%%%%%%%%%%
395 396
Over the low frequency range, the spectrum has multiple, narrow peaks that occur at
397
integer multiples of the stimulus frequency. Their amplitudes range from 3 to 20 times
398
the noise level (defined here by the amplitude at non-tagged frequencies). This result
399
demonstrates that cortical responses are clearly entrained by the steady-state
400
stimulation. The spectra also show a narrowband response at the dot refresh rate
401
(i.e. f2 = 21.25Hz). The brain is therefore also responding in a synchronous fashion
402
to the refresh of the local elements in the stimulus. When the center and surround
403
regions are abutted (no gap, Figure 4a), the spectrum is dominated by first and third
404
(odd) harmonics of the disparity update rate, which we refer to as 1f1 and 3f1. The
405
introduction of a 4° deg gap strongly reduces the odd harmonic component of the
406
response. Odd harmonics are produced by asymmetries in the responses to the two
407
states of the stimulation, i.e., when the disk appears behind the surround and when it
408
merges with it in the fixation plane. These asymmetries can be explained by the
409
disparity tuning function in early visual areas. One of our previous EEG studies
410
estimated these curves at the population level in V1, LOC, V4, hMT+ and V3A and
411
found that in all these ROIs, responses increase from 0’ to 16’ and then go back to
412
their baseline level (Cottereau et al., 2011a). Responses at 12’ are therefore stronger
413
than those at 0’, leading to an asymmetry. In the 4° gap condition, the amplitudes at
414
the odd harmonics and in the topography of the first harmonics suggest that, even
415
though the cortical activity is diminished in this condition, the response is still mainly
416
in the odd harmonics. The amplitude at the odd harmonics is therefore a good metric
417
to characterize the cortical responses to the different conditions.
418 419
Effects of the gap
420
The grand average topographic maps corresponding to the first harmonic
421
suggest that the underlying neural activity arises mainly in the parietal and occipital
422
cortex. This is confirmed by the surface-based average reconstruction (Figure 4,
423
right). Our question is a quantitative one --- how does disparity processing in different
424
cortical areas depend on the spatial separation of features. These cortical images are
425
shown here only for visualization purposes; quantitative analyses will be performed in
426
the five regions of interest (ROIs) defined by fMRI retinotopy and standard localizers.
427 428 429
%%%%%% INSERT FIGURE 5 ABOUT HERE %%%%%%%%%%%%
430 431
Figure 5 displays a composite measure of the odd-harmonic amplitudes in the
432
five ROIs as a function of the gap size. The responses obtained when the surround
433
was uncorrelated (control condition) are displayed by the filled areas (mean and
434
standard error). In V1 as well as in V4, LOC, hMT+ and V3A, the amplitudes in the
435
absence of a gap are larger than those obtained in the control condition, suggesting
436
that responses in these ROIs are enhanced by the presence of the correlated
437
surround. In our previous work (Cottereau et al., 2011a, Cottereau et al, 2012), the
438
V4, LOC, hMT+ and V3A ROIs were each found to be influenced by the addition of a
439
disparity reference, i.e., the surround. Based on our previous findings and single cell
440
studies (see e.g. Cumming and Parker 1999), we did not expect any surround
441
(relative disparity) effects in V1. The activity observed with the 0° gap could arise
442
from crosstalk from all the areas surrounding V1, but particularly the V3A area (see
443
the ‘Resolution of source-imaged SSVEP’ section above for more detail on cross-
444
talk) whose SNR for the no gap condition is particularly large.
445
Taken across all the gap values, the presence of a correlated surround
446
enhanced the SNRs at the odd harmonics when compared to the activity obtained
447
with the uncorrelated surround. This was confirmed by a repeated measures (2
448
surround conditions) x (5 ROIs) ANOVA where the two surround conditions
449
(correlated and uncorrelated) corresponded to the average SNRs across the 6 gap
450
values on the one hand and the SNR for the uncorrelated surround on the other
451
hand. This ANOVA supported a surround effect (p < 0.05) and also a significant
452
interaction (p < 0.01) between ROI and the nature of the surround. Post-hoc signed-
453
rank tests showed that only V3A (p < 0.01) and V4 (p < 0.05) activity was significantly
454
higher than the control when averaged across all gap conditions. The possibility that
455
the responses in V4 are due to cross-talk from V3A is ruled out by the results of the
456
second experiment (see the next section) that clearly demonstrate that the activity in
457
V4 and V3A are different.
458
Generally, the effect of the gap is to decrease the SNRs. This was confirmed
459
by an (6 gap conditions) x (5 ROIs) ANOVA that led to a main effect of gap size (p