Mar 16, 1991 - What findings in an infant or child indicate a need for audio- logic examination? 4. How are disorders of speech and language best managed?
Normal Speech and Language Development: An Overview James Coplan Pediatr. Rev. 1995;16;91-100 DOI: 10.1542/pir.16-3-91
The online version of this article, along with updated information and services, is located on the World Wide Web at: http://pedsinreview.aappublications.org
Pediatrics in Review is the official journal of the American Academy of Pediatrics. A monthly publication, it has been published continuously since 1979. Pediatrics in Review is owned, published, and trademarked by the American Academy of Pediatrics, 141 Northwest Point Boulevard, Elk Grove Village, Illinois, 60007. Copyright © 1995 by the American Academy of Pediatrics. All rights reserved. Print ISSN: 0191-9601. Online ISSN: 1526-3347.
: Normal Speech An Overview James
Coplan,
FOCUS
and
QUESTIONS
are speech and language defined and related? 2. How are the developmental sequences In the acquisition of speech and language modified by deafness or hearing disabIlity, developmental language disorders, mental retardation, autism, or sociocultural factors? 3. What findings in an infant or child indicate a need for audiologic examination? 4. How are disorders of speech and
best managed?
language
Development:
MD*
1. How
5.
Language
How do disorders of speech and language differ as to prognosis?
regardless of the language spoken by their adult caretakers. The earliest of these utterances is cooing, which consists of musical, open vowel sounds. Cooing should be well established within the first 4 to 6 weeks of life. This is followed in the first few months of life by bilabial sounds: blowing bubbles or the “raspberry.” By 5 months, laughing and a variety of monosyllables appear, such as “ba” or “ga” Between 6 and 8 months, infants produce polysyllabic babbling, which consists of the same syllable repeated over and over: “lalalalala,” ‘mamamamama,” ‘dadadadada,’ and so forth. During the latter half of the first year of life, infants begin to restrict their phonemic repertoire to coincide with the speech sounds of their adult caretakers. By 9 months, infants sporadically produce the truncated utterances “mama” or “dada” without comprehension of the meaning that adults have attached to these sounds. By 10 months of age, infants produce these utterances spontaneously and consistently to label the appropriate parent. By I 2 months, infants have acquired one or two words other than ‘mama, or ‘dada,’ or the names of other family members. Infants also will imitate words when presented by an adult in a stimulus-driven fashion. For example, the adult may hold up a ball and say, “Say ‘ball’ ! ‘Ball’!” Although the infant may repeat the adult’s utterance, this does not qualify as a word in the infant’s vocabulary because he or she is not producing it spontaneously. Vocabulary growth velocity accelerates steadily during the second year of life, starting at a rate of approximately one new word per week at 1 2 months of age and attaining a rate of one or more new words per day by 24 months of age. By 18 to 20 months, a toddler should be using a minimum of 20 words; the typical 24-month-old has a vocabulary of at least 50 words. Early during the second year of life most toddlers produce jargon. Jargon consists of strings of different sounds (as op‘
‘
Normal Language
Speech and Development
Language consists of any symbol system for the storage and exchange of information. It commonly is described in terms of auditory expressive and receptive ability (speech and listening comprehension, respectively). However, language also is conveyed visually. Normal infants attend to gestures and initiate various gestures to make their needs known; visual language development in deaf infants exposed to a formal sign system, such as American Sign Language (ASL), parallels the stages of oral language development in hearing infants;’ and reading and writing are obvious manifestations of visual language in older children and adults. Precursors to auditory and visual language are readily observable from earliest infancy. Because these precursors lack symbolic value, they are termed prelinguistic. AUDITORY LANGUAGE
Human sequence *Associate
EXPRESSIVE
infants
produce
a uniform
of prelinguistic Professor
Training
in Child University of New Center, Syracuse,
utterances,
of Pediatrics,
Director
in Review
Vol.
of
State
Development, York Health NY.
Science
Dr. Coplan also is affiliated with publishers of the Early Language Scale, of which he is the author.
Pediatrics
ARTICLE
16
PRO-ED, Milestone
No. 3
March
‘
1995
‘
‘ ‘
‘
‘
posed to the 9-month-old’s repetition of the same sound over and over), with rising and falling speech-like inflection. Parents may remark that their child seems to be ‘speaking a foreign language.’ The child’s jargon frequently will contain embedded words ( ‘lamanagabanadaddv. manabaganamommy, etc). The child is practicing the inflection patterns of speech with this jargon, referred to as prosody. By 24 months of age, toddlers are producing two-word phrases. These consist of spontaneous, novel combinations: ‘Want cookie!’ ‘Me down!’ and so forth. Children may memorize phrases such as “All gone,” but these are not novel combinations devised by the child. Jargon wanes and disappears rapidly around the time the child makes the transition from single words to two-word phrases. Pronouns appear in the second year of life, at first nonspecifically (ie, the child may reverse ‘me’ and ‘you’ ) and then specifically (‘ ‘me’ always referring to the speaker, and “you” referring to the listener). Vocabulary growth velocity continues to accelerate during the third year of life, ultimately reaching a rate of several new words per day. This increase in growth velocity is accompanied by a commensurate increase in vocabulary size; if a 30-month-old’s vocabulary is small enough for the parent to count accurately (90 dB) loss. The prevalence of permanent, partial hearing loss (eg, bilateral moderate loss, unilateral loss) is unknown. but probably is in the range of three to ten children per thousand. Etiologic factors are the same as for children who have bilateral severe-to-profound loss. Otitis media with effusion (OME) causes transient, mild-to-moderate hearing loss. Approximately 15% of infants experience 90 or more days of OME in the first year of life. Developmental language disorders (DLD) have been reported with increased frequency among such “otitis-prone” infants,4 but the attack rate for DLD specifically attributable to OME is unknown. ‘
RETARDATION
ing below -5 SD are considered to be profoundly mentally retarded. Most mentally retarded children are mildly retarded, and most of these are normal biologically: their mild MR reflects polygenic endowment for intelligence rather than any pathologic process. A lower limit for polygenic determination of intelligence probably exists, however, somewhere between -2 and -3 SD. Therefore, any child who has an IQ below 55 (ie, 3 SD below the population mean of 100) should be assumed to have an underlying pathologic process as the source for his or her MR. Most individuals demonstrate intellectual abilities very similar to those of their parents and siblings. Therefore, if a child’s IQ falls >3 SD below the estimated mean IQ of the other members of his or her nuclear family, an organic disability also should be suspected, even though the absolute value of the child’s IQ may be in the 60s or 70s.
Three percent of children are mentally retarded (Table I ); all children who are mentally retarded are Ianguage-delayed. Mental retardation (MR) is defined as significantly subaverage general intellectual function plus delayed adaptive skills, arising in the first 5 years of life. Adaptive skills include self-feeding, selfgrooming, and self-dressing. General intellectual function is determined by an age-appropriate standardized test that measures problem-solving and language abilities: ‘significantly subaverage’ is defined as more than 2 standard deviations (SD) below the mean. “Mild” MR is defined as -2 to -3 SD. Because intelligence tests are standardized to a mean score of 100, with I SD equaling 15 points, mild MR is equivalent to an intelligence quotient (IQ) of 69 to 55. Moderate MR -3 to -4 SD (IQ 54 to 40), severe MR -4 to -5 SD (IQ 39 to 25), and persons scor-
‘
-
‘
‘
TABLE
2. Indications
for
Audiologic
Evaluation
Neonatal Intensive Care Birthweight < I 500 g Birthweight > 1500 g if complicated by any of the following: Prolonged neonatal depression (Apgar score 0-3 at 5 mm; no spontaneous persisting to 2 h of age) Hyperbilirubinemia at level exceeding indication for exchange transfusion Mechanical ventilation for > 10 d
Congenital
infection
toxoplasmosis,
with agents
known
or suspected
of causing
hearing
respiration
loss
by
(cytomegalovirus,
10 mm;
rubella,
hypotonia
syphilis,
herpes)
Anomalies Mesodermal: Abnormalities of first or second branchial arch (auricles, mandible) Ectodermal: Abnormalities of neural crest (eg, iris heterochromia, albinism, piebaldism) Presence of syndrome known to be associated with hearing loss (eg, Usher syndrome, Family
history
Bacterial
of congenital
infectious
Ototoxic
disease
therapy
Neurodegenerative trauma,
Speech,
language, Coplan,
ill
when
Review
with
loss
or global
t)/.
and
/6
pathways
lie in radiation
to be associated through
with
temporal
loss loop
(mumps,
measles,
Epstein-Barr
virus)
diuretics)
field hearing
loss
bone
hearing
developmental Anon.
Joint
3
Mate/i
No.
hearing
to aminoglycosides,
fracture
child’s
with
limited
known
regarding
1987,
not
auditory
especially
concern
to be associated
but
disorder
Parental
Adaptedfrom
known
(including
drugs
Radiation
P(’(/I(lt)i(S
hearing
syndrome)
meningitis
Other
Head
or of childhood-onset
Waardenburg
delay Committee
1995
on Infant
Hearing
1990
Position
Statement.
A SHA.
1991;33(suppl
5):3-6
93
LANGUAGE Language
and
Speech
Pathologic entities giving rise to MR include single gene, chromosomal, and teratogenic disorders; sporadic syndromes of unknown etiology; perinatal events; and hypoxic, traumatic, infectious, or toxic postnatal insults.
DEVELOPMENTAL DISORDERS
LANGUAGE
DLD are a clinically heterogeneous group of disorders characterized by selective impairment of speech and/or language development, with relative sparing of other developmental domains. General intelligence usually is normal, or if delayed, not sufficiently so to account for the degree of Ianguage impairment observed. Hearing, oromotor function and social-emotional development are normal, and there is no history to suggest environmental deprivation. Unlike MR.
interacting with the child’s sex or unknown environmental factors as the probable cause of DLD.
of shorter speech segments ( ‘I wu wu wwwwant to go with you!’ ‘) or a complete inability to mitiate a word, referred to as ‘blockage.’ Stuttering is accompanied by signs of distress, such as facial grimacing or change in respiratory rate. The prevalence of stuttering peaks at 4% between 2#{189} and 4 years of age and declines to about I % among older children and adults. The etiology of stuttering is multifactorial. There is a strong genetic component, with two thirds of adult stutterers coming from families that have a positive history of stuttering. As with DLD, however, there is no clear mendelian pattern, but rather the suggestion that polygenic loading increases the risk for stuttering, which may be triggered by interaction with one or more unknown environmental factors. ‘
.
.
.
.
.
.
‘
‘
AUTISM
Autism is characterized by delayed and deviant language development, impaired affective development, monotonously repetitious behaviors with an insistence on maintenance of routines, and an onset before 30 months of age. Prevalence estimates for autism conventionally are quoted as two to four children per 10 000 (0.02% to 0.04%). This figure, however, primarily reflects young children whose autism is moderate to severe. Many of the classic diagnostic features of autism wane with time, although the affected individual remains autistic and significantly impaired. At present, however, no satisfactory criteria exist for diagnosing
DYSARTHRIA
Unrecognized hearing loss may exert long-term effects by disrupting CNS development, not just at the level of auditory perception, but at the level of language processing. there are no universally accepted standards for diagnosing DLD. Prevalence estimates vary, therefore, depending on the definitions used and the ages at which subjects have been tested. A reasonable estimate is that DLD affects 5% to 10% of preschool children. Affected boys outnumber affected girls by approximately 3:1. DLD sometimes can be traced to a specific underlying factor, such as sex chromosome aneuploidy, fragile x syndrome, neonatal intracranial hemorrhage, fetal alcohol effects, head trauma, or human immunodeficiency virus encephalopathy. Rarely, DLD is associated with epileptogenic spike-wave activity in one or both temporal lobes without any outward manifestation of seizure activity. Fluctuating hearing loss due to otitis media with effusion in the first 12 months of life has been implicated as the cause of DLD in some children. In the majority of cases, however, the etiology of DLD remains unknown. Family history for speech delay frequently is positive, usually without a clear pattern of mendelian inheritance, suggesting polygenic inheritance of a predisposition for DLD 94
autism in middle childhood, adolescence, or adulthood. Furthermore, autism can occur with any degree of severity from mild to profound; some individuals who do not satisfy all of the formal diagnostic criteria for autism clearly lie on the spectrum of autistic disorders. A more realistic prevalence estimate, taking into account children who have partial expression of the autistic syndrome, is 20 per 10 000 (0.2%). Autism can be caused by most of the same agents that give rise to MR. An association has been reported between autism and fragile X syndrome,6 but this may reflect ascertainment bias.7
Dysarthria refers to physical handicap of the muscles of speech production; symbolic aspects of language function are intact. Dysarthria in children usually is encountered as one component of generalized, static, upper motor neuron encephalopathy, cornmonly known as cerebral palsy (CP). The prevalence of CP is approximately three children per thousand, and approximately one third of affected children will manifest dysarthria. With the possible exception of very-low-birthweight infants, most cases of CP are not due to perinatal events, but stem from prenatal factors, such as intrauterine infection or congenital malformation of the central nervous system.8 M#{246}biussyndrome (facial diplegia) rarely occurs as an isolated developmental feature outside of the setting of CP.
STUTTERING
NONCAUSES
Physiologic disfluency, marked by transient loss of normal rate and rhythm of speech, is a normal phenomenon among children between 2#{189} and 4 years of age. Physiologic disfluency typically involves repetition of whole words (“I want I want... I want to go with you!”) and is unaccompanied by subjective signs of distress in the child. Stuttering, or dysfluency, involves repetition
LANGUAGE
...
Pediatrics
OF
SPEECH
OR
DELAY
Birth
order, uncomplicated twinning, ‘laziness,’ bilingualism, and tonguetie all are invoked from time to time as causes of speech or language delay; in fact, none of these actually delays speech or language development. Later-born children speak no later than first-borns. Normal twins do not speak significantly later than normal single-born children. If a par‘
‘
in Review
Vol. /6
No. 3
Marc/i
1995
LANGUAGE Language and Speech ent or sibling is speaking for the child, this is the result, not the cause, of the child’s delay! Although toddlers may be noncompliant, they are developmentally incapable of being ‘lazy. Advice such as ‘Don’t give him what he wants until he says the word for it’ is unhelpful and only creates conflict between a speechdelayed child and his or her parents. Children reared bilingually may intermix vocabulary and syntax freely for the first 24 to 30 months of life, but developmental constants, such as vocabulary size, length of utterance, linguistic complexity, and intelligibility, are normal, and by 36 months the child essentially is a fluent bilingual speaker. ‘
‘ ‘
‘
‘
Pathogenesis Pathophysiology
and
Classical neurology consists of deducing brain function from the pattern of deficits and residual abilities after destruction of discrete regions of the brain; most of what we know about brain-language interrelationships is based on studies of adult stroke victims. Several obstacles exist to deciphering the pathophysiology of language disorders in children. Infants usually sustain diffuse rather than discrete insults (eg, intrauterine viral infection, fetal alcohol effects). Language functions are less well localized in the child’s brain compared with that of the adult; even an anatomically discrete lesion in a young child may not have as specific effects as the same lesion in an adult. Infants and very young children do not have a history of normal language development from which the clinical investigator may draw structural-functional inferences by comparing residual abilities with the subject’s premorbid state. Finally, identical lesions in infants and older children produce different effects, depending on the point in the developmental sequence when the lesion is sustained.9 The effects of hearing loss on language development are mediated via two pathways. The obvious pathway is via the effect of hearing loss on speech perception: The less well a child can hear, the more difficult it is to learn to speak. A less obvious but Pediatrics
in Review
Vol.
/6
No.
3
Marc/i
probably equally important pathway is via distortion of central nervous system (CNS) maturation during a critical period for language development. A critical period is an interval early in the development of a biological system during which the system is maximally sensitive to a specific outside stimulus, the effect of which may be to ‘trigger the course of further normal development or [to] produce an irrevocable result not modifiable in subsequent development.’ 10 Such a period exists for early postnatal maturation of auditory pathways in animals, and a similar phenomenon almost certainly occurs in human infants. Unrecognized hearing loss may exert long-term effects on disrupting CNS development, not just at the level of auditory perception, but at the level of language processing. For example. the mean age at diagnosis of profound congenital deafness in the United States is 24 months. Most deaf infants, therefore, are denied language stimulation of any kind during the critical first 2 years of life. Deaf infants born to deaf parents show better language development than similar infants born to hearing parents, owing in part to deaf parents providing their infants with appropriate language stimulation via ASL from the moment of birth. ‘
.
.
.
‘
Stuttering persistence increase
‘
‘
‘
‘
(dysfluency) for more than 6 months, onset or beyond age 5 years, or a positive family history the likelihood of stuttering.
DLD
is seen after focal brain dambut lesions in the Wernicke or the Broca area during infancy do not produce syndromes analogous to their adult counterparts, and such ‘pure” forms of language disability are virtually nonexistent on a developmental basis.’2 Volumetric magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) studies have demonstrated subtle abnormalities of the perisylvian cortex in some boys whose language development is I I
‘
delayed. Abnormalities have been described or theorized in the brain stem, limbic system,’3 temporal lobes, and cerebellum’4 to account for the features of autism. At present, however, there are no known neuroanatomic or neu/995
rophysiologic abnormalities that are either necessary or sufficient to give rise to autism. MR is the consequence of widespread abnormalities of cerebral architecture, neurotransmission, or myelination. In some cases the pathophysiology of MR has been elucidated, as in the essential facilitation of cerebral development by thyroid hormone or the disruption of neuronal migration caused by prenatal exposure to ethanol. Defective dystrophin-mediated regulation of cerebral development may account for the 30% prevalence of MR among boys who have Duchenne muscular dystrophy. In most cases, however, the pathophysiology of MR remains unknown. The pathogenesis of language delay in MR also is unclear. The ‘cognitive hypothesis’ holds that language is a direct expression of general cognitive development, and language delay is regarded as a symptom of MR itself. The ‘correlational hypothesis’ holds that Ianguage and cognition both represent outward manifestations of more fundamental cerebral processes, such as the ability to infer rules and hierarchical relationships from primary experience. According to this model, any insult sufficiently diffuse to im-
pair these fundamental impair both language
processes will and cognitive
ability simultaneously.’5 Both of these models predict that language delay will be proportionate to the degree of global cognitive delay, which is the case for individuals who have uncomplicated MR. The less intuitive ‘modular hypothesis’ holds that language and cognition are separate functions (“modules”), with cognition being neither sufficient nor necessary for the development of language.’6 Cognition may exist in the absence of language, as in an adult following a left temporal lobe stroke. Conversely, language may develop in relative isolation from cognition, as in the superficial “cocktail party ‘
‘
95
LANGUAGE Language
and Speech
chatter” of the child who has hydrocephalus or Williams syndromesyntax and grammar are correct, but speech is shallow and of limited content. Hyperlexia (the ability to decode printed matter by rote at a level far above the person’s other cognitive abilities), frequently seen in autism, also may represent an example of modular development of language. The pathogenesis and pathophysiology of stuttering remain obscure; an abnormality of interhemispheric
Frequency
in
communication long has been suspected)7 In adults, stroke has resuited in the sudden disappearance of lifelong stuttering or the sudden emergence of stuttering. Ablation of tissue in one hemisphere, freeing contralateral speech centers from the interference of dyssynchronous input, has been postulated to explain the former; disruption of interhemispheric pathways has been suggested to explain the latter. Dysarthria arises because of impairment to the upper
Cycles
per
Second
C) CD
C) C’)
a)
C Cl) Cl)
0 -J
C) C CD a)
I
FIGURE
1.
i.s. /)IOtt(’d
0)11 t/i(’
vertical thresholil wit/ziti
Si:nsitivitV
range
horizo,ztal
axis. Human of hearing) this
rouge,
for
/iiiinaii
C,.VIS
Izeoiring
roitiges
speech
Frequency
in decibels
from
120 i/B (the
to
I)14t c/ifjerent
hearing.
Wfl/)Iitilde
250
to) 8000
threshold .co)undS
(dB) Hz
ofpain). have
different
in cycles/sec (Hertz: is plotted i,zver.selv (liii! from 0 i/B (the
Hionan
speech
Hz) 0)0
tIle
is centered
.frequellcV/a?li/)litilde
Voiced .soiiiid.s ( ‘ ‘e, ‘ ‘ ‘ 1, ‘ ‘ ‘ ‘a, ‘ ‘ etc) ii,.e low-trequeicv, high-aitiplitude stimuli ( -5() i/B at 250 to 500 Hz). U,it’oiced sounds, or sihilants, sac/i as ‘‘f’’’’s (1,1(1 tI, ‘ ‘ are high-frequency, low-aln/)litui/e stimuli ( -20 i/B at 4000 Hz). The hearing Io).S5 of the i/u/u plotteo/ on the chart (X left ear, 0 rig/it ear) eliini,iiited perceptio)n of sibilants, thus severe/s degrading her ability to) understand speech. (Il(IFTI(li’fl.Sti(.S.
Relative
.sparuzg
created
the false
.
/,eliavior
Copyright 96
Clinical
Aspects
The hearing-impaired infant manifests auditory expressive and receptive delay; visual language is normal. Deaf infants COo and babble fairly normally until approximately 6 months of age. Thereafter, the amount of vocal output and the range of sounds in the child’s phonemic repertoire dwindle. Infants who have a hearing threshold of greater than 90 dB at and above 1000 cycles/sec have an extremely difficult time acquiring speech; auditory comprehension is equally impaired. Congenital moderate-to-severe HL (50 to 90 dB) is associated with impaired intelligibility, reduced verhal output, and reduced auditory comprehension. The hearing-impaired child may begin improvising his or her own form of sign language. The child often is labelled erroneously as having a behavior problem’ until the correct diagnosis belatedly is reached (Figure 1). The mentally retarded child manifests delay in all language areas: auditory expressive, auditory receptive, and visual. Cooing and babbling may be reduced in amount as well as delayed. Subsequent auditory expressive milestones resemble those of a normal but much younger child. Parents of a mentally retarded 3-year-old often assert that their child “understands everything we tell him.” Close questioning usually reveals that the parents are breaking everything down into a series of one-step cornmands or sequences that have been memorized as a unit. Although this may be highly functional for the child. it indicates a receptive Ianguage level of no greater than I 8 to 24 months. Delayed emergence of index finger pointing is a highly sensitive indicator of global language delay. Adaptive skills such as use of a spoon or dressing and undressing are delayed, as are play skills. Gross motor development. however, usually is normal. The child who has DLD presents with variable degrees of expressive and receptive impairment. Visual language, adaptive, play. and personal/ social development are normal. Ex‘
z
/,,,.
motor neurons or brainstem motor nuclei that govern the muscles of the face, oropharynx, and larynx.
o)t Ionc-fro’queiio#{149}v
o)f
il?i/)?e.S.SiO)1i
J)rO)i)lei?i
Anu’rioa,i
‘ ‘
iia.s
Academy
heariiig
(30-dB
threshold
at 250
Hz
in the
left
.
ear)
hearin#{231}. This child was 43 lflo)litIis o)ld befoic i/iai,’no.sei/ correct/v as Iieari,ii’ loss. Froni Cop/an J. of Pediatrics: asci/ b /n’rFnis.sioi. 1iO)rlflilI
Pediatrics
in Review
‘
t”O/.
16
No.
3
Mare/i
/995
LANGUAGE Language and Speech autistic child may be indifferent to parental entreaties one minute, but climb into the lap of the examining physician the next. The child may treat other persons as if they are objects. going through the examiner’s pockets. for example. without ever regarding the examiner face to face. Repetitous or ritualistic behaviors include rigid food preferences, fascination with bits of string or spinning objects. fixed daily routines, or repetitious unimaginative play. Autism
pressive deficits commonly include impaired intelligibility and delayed emergence of sentence structure. Speech may be ellortful and reduced in tOtLl amount. The child may begin tO invent his or her own sign Ianguage in an etlort to communicate. Receptive language may appear normal, hut difficulties with short-term auditory memory. auditory discrimination. or coiiiplex auditory comprehension tasks frequently become cvident when the child is old enough to cooperate with suitable testing. Occasionally a child will present having auditory agilosia: the absence of behavioral response to sound, even though peripheral hearing is normal. Auditory agnosia is accompanied by L severe expressive deficit. Auditory agnosia may be associated with epileptogenic teinporal lobe activity on electroencephalography ( EEG) and may he lifelong or of sudden onset in a previously normal child (“acquired epileptic aphasia.’ or LandauKlefiner syndroiiie). The autistic child manifests delayed and deviant language, impaired affecti ye development, and repetitious behaviors with an insistence on preservation of routines.19 Language is deviant or ‘atypical’ in the sense that many of the language features are not normal for a child of any age. The hallmark i)t the autistic type of language disorder is impaired pragmatics-lailure to use language as a medium of social interaction. The deficit in social interaction may be evident from infancy. with failure to make eye contact, to engage in reciprocal vocalization, or to point to desired objects. Speech consists mainly of rhetorical naming of objects and echolalia (repetition of the utterances of others). Echolalia may he immediate or delayed, typically including television commercials, segments of dialogue committed to memory from home videos, or other scripted material. Prosody (inElection) may be sing-song. robotlike, or stilted. The autistic child fails to give or receive affection in the usual manner. The child is not cuddly as an infant, does not like to be held, does not run to a parent for comfort after minor injuries. may not discriminate between parents and strangers, and has dillIculty reading body language. The
motor function and fine motor, adaptive, gross motor, play, and personal/ social development also should be investigated. Once speech or language delay has been detected, addi-
i’eo/iat,,i.s
/995
‘
‘
in Review
‘
Vo/.
/6
No.
3
Mare/i
tional developmental testing should be undertaken by a developmental pediatrician, psychologist. speech/ language pathologist, or some combination of these individuals, depending on the extent and nature of the child’s suspected delays. Any child who fails speech/language screening or fulfills other defined criteria should undergo formal audiologic testing, regardless of how well the child seems to hear on physical
examination
(Table
2).’
Birth order, uncomplicated twinning, laziness, bilingualism, and tongue-tie are not causes of delayed speech or language. exist
may
with
any
degree of severity General intel-
from mild to profound. ligence may be normal,
although
ap-
proximately 51)C% of all children who are autistic are mentally retarded as well. Terms such as Asperger syndrome, pervasive developmental disorder. and pragmatic-type developmental language disorder often are applied to children who have mild or fragmentary expression of the autistic syndrome; the wisdom of “lumping” versus ‘splitting’ such children into separate categories is controversial. In stuttering, clinical aspects usually are limited to loss of fluency with associated signs of subjective distress; symbolic aspects of Ianguage development are intact. Duration of disiluency for more than 6 months, onset or persistence of disfluency beyond 5 years of age, or a family history of stuttering all in‘
crease
the
‘
likelihood
that
disfluency
actually may herald stuttering. Likewise, in dysarthria, the defect is limited to speech production; receptive and visual language are intact. Difficulties
with
sucking
or
swallowing,
choking. or coughing during feeding may be the initial clues to significant oromotor impairment. The
best
test
for
detecting
speech
or language problems is a comprehensive history. encompassing auditory expressive, auditory receptive. and
visual
language
(Figure
2).
Oro-
Most hearing-impaired children have sufficient residual hearing to make gross responses to sound, even though perception is severely cornpromised; even profoundly deaf children commonly are missed by the examining physician because they ‘cheat’ by avidly using visual cues. The two most common causes of deafness are isolated recessive deafness and intrauterine CMV infection. The recurrence risk fbr the former is 25c% the recurrence risk for the latter is close to zero because the mother’s seroimmune status protects subsequent fetuses. Virologic and serologic ‘
‘
;
studies
for
CMV
should
be
obtained
routinely on newborn infants who have i ntrauteri ne growth retardation or microcephaly
of
unknown
cause;
these studies are of no diagnostic value beyond the first month of life. Ophthalmologic evaluation and a computed tomographic (CT) scan of the brain. searching for choreoretinitis and intracranial calcifications, respectively, occasionally support a presumptive diagnosis of intrauterine viral infection. In the absence of a proven etiology for the child’s hearing loss, evaluation by a medial geneticist is indicated. The parents and siblings of an infant who has hearing loss of unknown etiology also should be tested fIr hearing loss. G-banded karyotype and cytogenetic or DNA probe studies for fragile X are indicated in children who have mental retardation:#{176} autism, or developmental language disorder. Human
(HIV)
immunodeficiency
serology
should
virus
be considered 97
LANGUAGE Language and Speech
ELM
EARLY
SCALE-2
MONTHS
1
LANGUAGE
MILESTONE
SALE-2
22
24
26
28 Examin
32
34
36
2 1
Coo
Reciprocal
2
37
1
vocalization
[15A one-half 19 Three-fourths
by strangers
20A11/Almost
_________________
47
All
I
3Laugh LU f
o
r5 Mono. 8polysyllabic 7
#{149}
babbling L babbling
word
1
Alerts
to
2
P
6lnhibits TI
7Bell, 1-Step
B
laterally
to
Recognizes
Points
Driflk with
to objects S,on
Wt.wth Eat,/th
)
‘I
by use
‘I
commands
13
2/4)
Th’o
body
parts wlo gestureL’
I
On top Behind -Basde
-f-,-’.-’--4-----I---------
I
2/4)
I
AE
objects
tVisual
I
AR
V
Global Lang.
Intell.
Pass/Fail
to facial
tracking
expressions
Raw
(H,V)
Score
+
(NA)
+
Percentile
#{176}
-
(NA)
6Blinkstothreat
TI
Imit. Step
gest.
comm. 9lnitiates
2
3
Std. S. Equiv.
games
4
Points 5
to desired 6
(NA)
._____‘
Age Equiv.
w. gest. I gest. games 10
7
8
Only
i
objects 9
10
11
12
14
16 AdOlbenal
OI093.T5e3bJ.m.sColan.MD
FIGURE expressive,
I
parents
‘I
1
12
-]
Responds
MONTHS
5Bell,over&down bell
9Pointsto 1 105tp commands
3Recognizes
>
_
Smiles 2
Cl)
_
1
I
-a 4
_
I
Ban
gesture
ball
I 1
Cp
described
w/o
Me spoon) Mom Meb.)l/Momspoon1 Mom b&)/M.cop
/1.5!
‘I
objects
‘L
command 2/3)
to named
(213)
laterallyto
Use
5jfl
1I Points
to voice
I4Orients
and
2/4) Cup Ba)) Spoon C,ayon
L
Conversations
17
sounds
“No”
diagonal
Prepositions
words 11
Recognizes
Name
__________
(Not”Mama/Dada”)
Orients 3
AnyI 16
4-6 Single
Objects:
I
wds
l4aMe/youe:
voice
I I
Single
Correct
10
.
18
_____________
sentences
1350+
I___________
“Mama/Dada”: Any “Mama/Dada”: 9First
I
I
I
‘1Tells2wants ‘22Wd
18 oogwotthd
20
if age
22 foTm)aBBa2lTn.y
(NA)
or developmental
24
26
be ootmed
level
28 from
PRO-ED.
is 12 months
30
32
8100
Shod
or (ass
34
Cmok
Clod..
36
huCin.
TX 18758
2.
The Ear/v Language Milestone Scale, second edition (ELM Scale-2), functions as a structured history of auditors’ auditory receptive. atid visual language development from birth to 36 months of age and of intelligibility of speech froni /8 to 48 months of age. Tile ELM Scale-2 can be used as a screening tool or as a semniquantitati%’e measure of language development. ELM Scale-2 score sheet from Coplan J. The Early Language Milestone Scale. Ed 2 (ELM Scale-2). PRO-ED, 8700 Shoal Creek Blvd. Austin, Tex. © 1983, 1993, used by permission.
in speech-delayed preschool children because speech delay may be an early indicator of HIV encephalopathy. ‘ Boys who have speech delay plus gross motor delay but who do not
have
clearly
increased
deep
ten-
don reflexes should undergo creatine kinase determination to rule out Duchenne muscular dystrophy. Electroencephalography should be undertaken in the rare child who has auditory agnosia; otherwise, the study is of little benefit in the evaluation of speecManguage delay. Imaging studies such as cranial CT or MRI seldom are informative, except in the presence of focal neurologic abnormalities or dysmorphic features suggestive of structural brain abnormality (eg, hypertelorism, midfacial hypoplasia, aberrant hair patterning). 98
Management Management
education may be all oral or ‘total communication’ (sign language plus speech). The ability of a congenitally hearing-impaired child to read lips usually is grossly overestimated by wishful adults. Normal-hearing parents may be averse to the introduction of a hearing aid or sign language because these become visible markers of the child’s disability. The physician needs to be aware of the century-old controversy between “oral” and ‘manual’ approaches23 and ensure that decisions regarding education are dictated by the child’s needs. The physician should encourage the parents to vent their feelings, while emphasizing that the primary goal of therapy is to foster effective commu‘
for
all
children
‘
who
have delayed speech or language includes making the correct developmental diagnosis; arriving at an underlying medical diagnosis; rendering appropriate genetic counseling (eg, recurrence risk); assuring that the child receives appropriate habilitative services; and assuring that the child, his or her parents, and his or her siblings receive appropriate supportive care so that family functioning is not compromised unduly. A trial of amplification usually is indicated for the child who has permanent hearing loss and will require the expertise of an audiologist. Cochlear implantation offers exciting possibilities for many children.22 Special Pediatrics
‘
‘
nication. iii
Res’ieiv
Speech,
to the extent Vol.
/6
No.
3
that Marc/i
it /995
LANGUAGE
Language is possible, will be an outgrowth of such an approach. Language delay in the mentally retarded child must be viewed within the overall context of the MR. Enrollment in a special education program, rather than isolated speech therapy. is indicated. The transition from
home-based
i nfant
to a classroom-based
takes The
place
program
around
physician
stimulation
36 months
needs
to be
group
often
are
the
child
who
has
DLD
is to
communicate effectively. For many children, manual sign language or an augmentative communication device such as a picture board or computer that has the capability of synthesizing speech offers effective short-term palliation of the expressive deficit.24 Parents often express the fear that if they let their child ‘depend’ on signing, the child never will learn to speak. This fear is understandable, ‘
but
‘
ungrounded.
Therapy for autism should be directed at enhancing communication and social skills. Therapy for autistic children is embroiled in controversy over facilitated communication (FC), a technique whereby an assistant supports the autistic person’s hand as the autistic person types at a conventional keyboard or computer. In theory. the assistant provides only support or actually pulls the autistic person’s hand back from the keyboard after a keystroke; the autistic person is said to generate all of the output. Such output often differs greatly from the autistic person’s verbal language. adaptive skills, or level of general intelligence as ascertained by standardized measures. FC is supported primarily by emotionally cornpellin anecdotes but scant controlled data: Facilitator influence has been implicated in studies in which the autistic person and facilitator respond to discrepant stimuli (eg, delivered via headphones). The “facilitated” output often represents the correct Peo/iatric.s
in Review
Vi)!.
/6
No.
3
‘In
At
helpful.
Marc/i
received than
the
by
stimu-
‘
‘
to
stimulus rather
lus received by the autistic person.2628 A standardized test protocol is needed to separate those instances in which FC may be genuine from those in which facilitator control may be operating. ‘Does FC work?’ is probably not a fruitful question. Rather,
alert
to the
facilitator,
usually
The child who has DLD should be referred for speech/language therapy. As with the child who has a severe hearing loss, the first therapeutic goal for
the
of age.
signs of family decompensation (physical abuse, parental separation, unresolved grieving or anger, dysfunctional behavior in siblings). Family therapy or participation in a support
response
the
‘
questions
whom
does
present,
appear
FC
work
however,
to be and
how?”
physicians
car-
ing for autistic persons face a terrible dilemma. Rejection of FC (because it lacks a plausible neuropsychological explanation or objecti ye empirical support) may condemn the autistic child to an unnecessarily isolated existence. Conversely, uncritical acceptance of FC may raise parents’ hopes falsely, inspire parental feelings of inadequacy if they cannot achieve the same results as those touted by their child’s facilitator at school, or cause harm
in medicolegal
standardized
settings.28
protocol
A
to assess
the
Most
preschool
experience
complete
I or 2 years
mastering
mitted
as evidence
in a court
be adof
law.
Stuttering requires referral to a speech/language pathologist. Admonitions by the physician or parent that the child should simply ‘slow ‘
down’
‘
are
of no
value
and
are
po-
tentially harmful. Anecdotal reports, primarily from the adult literature, have suggested benefit from an array of drugs,
including
benzodiazepines,
calcium channel blockers, parasympathomimetics, and haloperidol.29 At present, however, most therapy in children is behavioral.
Prognosis Prognosis for the child who has HL varies, depending on the age at onset of
HL,
age
at introduction
of amplifi-
cation and speech/language therapy, degree of HL, and presence of other disabilities. The prognosis for speech and language development in the mentally retarded child is governed by the degree of MR. Even with optimal care, the gap between the mentally retarded child and his or her age mates will widen steadily with the passage of time. /995
resolution
of
by
equal
difficulty
in
written
language.3#{176} The author has coined the term ‘the prelearning disabled child’ to describe the situation in which the child’s speech is no longer a developmental concern, but the child has not yet fallen far enough behind academically to meet regulatory guidelines ‘
‘
for academic intervention rate with a diagnosis of
commensulearning dis-
ability. Prognosis for the autistic child varies with the severity of the autism and the degree of any accompanying MR. Severely autistic children or those who have MR and autism gen-
cant qualitative ha and delayed
not
have
their developmental disorder. For many others, however, difficulty with acquisition of spoken language is replaced after a honeymoon period of
produced
should
who
develop
erally
FC25
children
reasonably clear speech by entry to kindergarten or first grade. Some children who have DLD DLD
validity of FC case by case will be of inestimable value in sorting through these issues. Until such a protocol is available, allegations of sexual abuse by
and Speech
continue
to demonstrate
dis-
abling autistic tendencies for life. Nonretarded children who are mildly to moderately
are replaced a tendency
autistic
show
by verbal literalism to dwell at length on
scure conversational tact and social skills though
the
signifi-
improvement. Echolaecholalia gradually
child
and ob-
topics. Eye conimprove, al-
still
has
great
difficulty reading social cues. Diagnosticians encountering a high-functioning autistic adolescent for the first time may find it impossible to reason backward from the child’s improved state to arrive at the correct developmental the
diagnosis.
autistic
Often,
features
the
become,
milder the
more
difficult it becomes to obtain appropriate educational services, and the greater the risk that the child will be diagnosed incorrectly as having oppositional behavior disorder or an emotional disturbance. Seventy-five
percent
of preschool
children who stutter experience complete or partial remission of symptoms during elementary school. The chance for remission is reduced greatly if a parent or sibling also stutters. The prognosis who have dysarthria the
Some never
severity
children acquire
of the
for children is governed by motor
disability.
who have dysarthria functional speech and 99
LANGUAGE and Speech
Language remain output
dependent device
on a mechanical of some
may range from a simple board to a microcomputer speech capabilities.
kind,
which
picture that has
REFERENCES I . Petitto
Marentette PF. Babbling in the evidence for the onR)geny of language. Science. I 991:251:1493-1496 Coplan J. Gleason JR. Unclear speech: recognition and significance of unintelligible speech in preschool children. Pediatrics. l9tg:K2 (p1 2):447-452 Ruben Ri. The ontogeny of human hearing. Acta Otolarviigol. 1992: 1 I 2:192-196 Finitzo T, Gunnarson AD. Clark JL. Auditory deprivation and early conductive loss froni otitis niedia. Topics in Laiigzage 1)i.vorders. I 990: 1 1:29-42 Ornitz EM. Naruse H. Introduction to biological research on infantile autism. In: Naruse H, Omits EM, eds. Neurobiology of Imijantile Autisni. New York. NY: Excerpta Medica; 1992:3-39 Hagerman Ri. Silverman AC: Froigile X .Sy,ulronie: f)iagiio.sis. Treat,neiit. aiid Researc/i. Baltimore. Md: Johns Hopkins University Press: 1991 Fisch GS. Is autism associated with the fragile X syndronie! Am J Med Gemiet. 1992:43:47-55 Nelson KB. Relationship of intrapartum and delivery room events to long-term neurologic outcome. (‘liii Perinatol. I 989: I6:995-I (X)7 Feldman HM, Holland AL, Kemp SS. Janosky JE. Language development after unilateral brain injury. !3raimi Lang. 992: 42:89-102 Colombo J. The critical period concept: research, methodology. and theoretical issues. Psycho! Bull. 1982:91:260-275 ThaI Di. Marchman V. Stiles J. et al. Early lexical development in children with focal brain injury. Brain Lang. I 99 1:40: 49 1-527 Tuchman RF. Rapin I. Shinnar S. Autistic and dysphasic children. I: Clinical characteristics. l’edatrics. I 99 1 :88: 12 1 1-1 2 I 8 DeLong GR. Autism, amnesia. hippocampus, and learning. Neurosci Biobeliav Rt/: I 992:16:63-70 Holroyd 5, Reiss AL. Bryan RN. Autistic features in Joubert syndrome: a genetic disorder with agenesis of the cerehellar vermis. Bio/ Psyc/iiatrv. I 99 1:29:287-294 Bates E. Language development. Curr Opimi Neurobio/. I992:2: I80-I 85 Yamada JE. Laura: A Case For The Modu/aritv Of i_.oiiigiici,’e. I 990: Cambridge. Mass: MIT Press Aram DM. Meyers SC. Ekelman BL. Fluency of conversational speech in children with unilateral brain lesions. Bmain IA1!lg. I 990:38: 1 05- I 2 I Coplan J. Deafness: ever heard of it? Delayed recognition of permanent hearing loss. Pediatrics. 1987:79:206-213 Rapin I. Autistic children: diagnosis and clinical features. Pediatrics. 199 I :87( part 2):751-760 (entire supplement devoted to a review of autism) manual
2.
3. 4.
5.
6.
7.
).
9.
10.
II.
12.
13.
14.
15. 16.
17.
18.
19.
/00
20.
21.
22.
LA.
mode:
23.
24.
25.
Schaefer GB. Bodensteiner JB. Evaluation of the child with idiopathic mental retardation. Pediatr Cliii North Am. I 992: 39:929-943 Condini A, Axia G. Cattteland C. et al. Development of language in I 8 -30 month old HIV-l-infected but not ill children. All)S. 991:5:735-739 Osherger Mi. Chute PM. Pope ML, ct al. Pediatric cochlear implant candidacy issues. Am J Otol. I 99 1 : I 2(suppl):80-88 Winefield R. Never the Tivaimi S/ia/I Meet: Bell. Gal/audet, oi,i(/ tbu Communications Debate. 1987: Washington. DC: Gallaudet University Press Goldstein U. Hockenberger EH. Significant progress in child language intervention: an I I year retrospective. Res I)ev Di.sahi/. 1991:12:401-424 Prior M. Cummins R. Questions about
facil itated conliiiunicatioii J Autism?, I)ev I)isord. 26.
,nd autisni. 1992:22:331-338
Hudson A. Melita B. Ariiold report: a case study asscssing of faci litated conlnlunicatioil
.
N. Brief thc validity I Autis,,i
l)ev 1)isord. I 993:23: 165- I 73 27. Wheeler DL. Jacobsen JW. Paglieri RA. Schwartz AA. An cxpcri nscntal asscssmcnt of faci Iit1.itcd coniiiiunication. Memit Retard. 1993:31:49-60 28. Hostler SL. Allaire JH. Chrisloph RA. Childhood sexual abusc reported by facilitated coiaillunication. Pediatrics. 1993:91:1190-1192 29. Brady JP. The pharm1.icology of’ stuttering: a critical review. Ani J Psychiatry. 199 I 148:1309-1316 30. Scarborough HS. Vcry early language deficits in dyslcxic children. (/ii/d I)ev. 1990:61:1728-1743
PIR QUIZ 5.
A mother
expresses
22-month-old gether words.
concern
boy She
is not reports
that
her
putting tothat he
can produce about I 0 words a rich but incomprehensible
and has jargon;
he gets what he wants by pointing and vocalizing. Uls history records hyperbilirubinemia in the neonatal 3tfl()d, following normal pregnancy
and
delivery (birthweight. 3.1 kg). were two episodes of otitis in the first year that seemed
There media
to respond
readily
to antibiotic
apy. Developmental stones have been its. The
patient
the family,
one
(male/female). with his twin, cabulary together household Farsi). nation
ther-
motor milewithin normal is the
second
lim-
child
in
of fraternal twins He shares his jargon who has a larger vo-
and is beginning to put two or three words. The is bilingual (English! Findings on physical examiare normal. Which of the fbI-
lowing is most likely the mothers concern? A. A normal variant
to account
for
in language
development. B.
Effect
C.
Effect of living household. Partial deafness
D.
of having
a twin. in a bilingual after
hyperbiliru-
after
otitis
binemia.
E. Partial
deafness
media. 6.
8.
In the previous case. following would you
to which give the
est priority7 A. Audiometry. B. Cytomegalovirus
titers.
C.
Examination pathologist.
D.
Magnetic
E. Reexamination
7. A 22-month-old boy is referred to you because his mother is concerned that his speech development seems to be delayed. He had several words by the age of 14 to 16 months, but has not added any new ones. She also is concerned that he has become difficult to manage, preferring to spend a lot of time alone, intent Oil repetitious manipulations of small objects and resentful of inlrusions. His lather feels that there is no problem ( ‘ ‘My mother says I was a strong-willed child. to’ ) the parents are having considerable strife over this difference of opinion. The boy was born at 38 weeks’ gestation (hirthweight 2.8 kg). had hyperbiliruhinemia in the first week, and had three episodes of otitis media in the first year without apparent complicatiofls. You find no abnormality on physical examination, which is dullcult to perform because you cannot get the patient’s attention. much less his cooperation. Your tentative diagnosis is: A. Auditory agnosia. B. Autism. C. Conduct disturbance due to faniily discord. D. Conduct disturbance in a partially deaf child. E. Degenerative disease of the central nervous system. In the previous case. all of the fbIlowing are true except: A. Chromosomal analysis often is indicated. B. Many children who have this condition are mentally retarded. C. Special education is indicated. D. The pathophysiology of this condition is unknown. E. This condition often is the result of emotional trauma.
of the high-
by a speech resonance imaging. in 2 to 3 months.
/‘(I(/i(,t,.i(.c
ill
Rel’IeIl
‘o/.
/6
No.
3
Maic/i
/995
Normal Speech and Language Development: An Overview James Coplan Pediatr. Rev. 1995;16;91-100 DOI: 10.1542/pir.16-3-91
Updated Information & Services
including high-resolution figures, can be found at: http://pedsinreview.aappublications.org
Permissions & Licensing
Information about reproducing this article in parts (figures, tables) or in its entirety can be found online at: http://pedsinreview.aappublications.org/misc/Permissions.shtml
Reprints
Information about ordering reprints can be found online: http://pedsinreview.aappublications.org/misc/reprints.shtml