USING e-ANNOTATION TOOLS FOR ELECTRONIC ...

9 downloads 0 Views 528KB Size Report
form better than same-aged individuals (Stephan, Chalabaev,. 46. Kotter-Grühn, & Jaconelli ... Dweck's (1999) lay theories of ability framework are based on. 56.
USING e-ANNOTATION TOOLS FOR ELECTRONIC PROOF CORRECTION TION Once you have Acrobat Reader open on your computer, click on the Comment tab at the right of the toolbar:

This will open up a panel down the right side of the document. The majority of tools you will use for annotating your proof will be in the Annotations section, pictured opposite. We’ve picked out some of these tools below:

1. Replace (Ins) Tool – for replacing text.

2. Strikethrough (Del) Tool – for deleting text.

Strikes a line through text and opens up a text box where replacement text can be entered. How to use it

Strikes a red line through text that is to be deleted. How to use it



Highlight a word or sentence.



Highlight a word or sentence.



Click on the Replace (Ins) icon in the Annotations section.



Click on the Strikethrough (Del) icon in the Annotations section.



Type the replacement text into the blue box that appears.

3. Add note to text Tool – for highlighting a section to be changed to bold or italic.

4. Add sticky note Tool – for making notes at specific points in the text.

Highlights text in yellow and opens up a text box where comments can be entered. How to use it

Marks a point in the proof where a comment needs to be highlighted. How to use it



Highlight the relevant section of text.





Click on the Add note to text icon in the Annotations section.

Click on the Add sticky note icon in the Annotations section.



Click at the point in the proof where the comment should be inserted.



Type the comment into the yellow box that appears.



Type instruction on what should be changed regarding the text into the yellow box that appears.

USING e-ANNOTATION TOOLS FOR ELECTRONIC PROOF CORRECTION TION

5. Attach File Tool – for inserting large amounts of text or replacement figures. Inserts an icon linking to the attached file in the appropriate pace in the text.

6. Drawing Markups Tools – for drawing shapes, lines and freeform annotations on proofs and commenting on these marks. Allows shapes, lines and freeform annotations to be drawn on proofs and for comment to be made on these marks.

How to use it •

Click on the Attach File icon in the Annotations section.



Click on the proof to where you’d like the attached file to be linked.



Select the file to be attached from your computer or network.



Select the colour and type of icon that will appear in the proof. Click OK.

How to use it • Click on one of the shapes in the Drawing Markups section. • Click on the proof at the relevant point and draw the selected shape with the cursor. • To add a comment to the drawn shape, move the cursor over the shape until an arrowhead appears. • Double click on the shape and type any text in the red box that appears.

J_ID: PSYP Customer A_ID: PSYP12780 Cadmus Art: PSYP12780 Ed. Ref. No.: PSYP-2015-0232.R2 Date: 25-October-16

Stage:

Page: 1

Psychophysiology, 00 (2016), 00–00. Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Printed in the USA. C 2016 Society for Psychophysiological Research Copyright V DOI: 10.1111/psyp.12780

1 2 3

AQ1

4 5 6 7 8

Effects of implicit theories of ability and stereotype-inconsistent information on handgrip strength in older adults: A regulatory fit perspective

 MELANIE EMILE,a,b AINA CHALABAEV,c SERGE S. COLSON,a,b JEROME VAULERIN,a,b CHARLENE FALZON,a,b AND FABIENNE D’ARRIPE-LONGUEVILLEa,b a

Laboratoire Motricite, Humaine, Education, Sport, Sante (LAMHESS EA 6312), Nice Sophia-Antipolis University, Nice, France Laboratoire Motricite, Humaine, Education, Sport, Sante (LAMHESS EA 6312), Toulon University, Toulon La Garde, France SENS, Universite Grenoble Alpes, Saint Martin d’He`res, France

b c

9

10

Abstract

11

20

This study examined whether stereotype-inconsistent information interacts with implicit theories of ability to affect handgrip strength in older adults. Eighty-two retired older adults (13 men and 69 women) from 61 to 89 years old (Mage 5 75.8 years; SD 5 6.9) performed maximum voluntary contractions (MVC) during a handgrip task in a design manipulating implicit theories of ability and stereotype-inconsistent information related to physical decline with aging. Participants were randomly assigned to one of three groups: incremental condition, entity condition, or control group. The results showed that in the incremental condition the stereotype-inconsistent information improved the peak MVC, the average MVC, the peak rate of force development (RFD), and RFD in the initial 50 ms of the MVC. This study therefore demonstrated that individuals with an incremental mindset who are exposed to stereotype-inconsistent information can boost their physical performance. These findings are discussed from the perspective of regulatory fit (i.e., when task framing is congruent with the individual’s goal).

21

Descriptors: Elderly, Incremental mindset, Stereotype threat, Rate of force development, Maximum voluntary contraction

12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19

22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37

The decrease in physical abilities with age is usually explained as an innate and inevitable decline in physiological functions, resulting notably in sarcopenia (e.g., Cruz-Jentoft et al., 2014). Sarcopenia and the decrease in physical abilities can be simply and rapidly assessed through the measure of handgrip strength (Lauretani et al., 2003). A study by Taekema, Gussekloo, Maier, Westendorp, and de Craen (2010) indicated that handgrip strength predicts functional, psychological, and social health in older populations (and low handgrip strength is therefore considered as an accurate indicator of frailty in older adults (Gale, Martyn, Cooper, & Sayer, 2007). Handgrip strength depends on age, sex, and ethnicity, as well as height, body mass index (BMI), nutritional status, and physical exercise (e.g., Dodds, Kuh, Aihie Sayer, & Cooper, 2013; Norman, Stob€aus, Gonzales, Schulzke, & Pirlich, 2011; Stenholm et al., 2012). In other words, handgrip strength depends

on factors that are nonmodifiable (sociodemographic predictors) or relatively stable (BMI, lifestyle habits). In this article, we adopt a situational perspective and argue that strength performance may partly depend on the social context in which the evaluations take place, particularly the stereotypical context. In line with this perspective, studies have shown an increase in strength performance when older adults were told that they perform better than same-aged individuals (Stephan, Chalabaev, Kotter-Gr€uhn, & Jaconelli, 2013) or when counterstereotypic information was given to women (Chalabaev et al., 2013). These studies suggest that strategies focused on preventing the influence of negative stereotypes (e.g., stereotype threat) may be sufficient to improve physical performance. The goal of this study was therefore to extend this line of research by investigating the conditions in which stereotype-inconsistent information may be efficient. More particularly, we examined how such information interacts with lay theories of ability. Dweck’s (1999) lay theories of ability framework are based on the assumption that an individual’s conception of his or her ability in a task or domain is an important predictor of behavior. Two implicit theories of ability have been identified: people who hold entity beliefs consider ability as stable and immutable, whereas those who hold incremental beliefs perceive ability as amenable to change. According to Dweck’s social-cognitive model (Dweck, 1986; Dweck & Leggett, 1988), how people perceive ability has

This study was supported by a Ph.D. grant from the Provence-Alpes C^ote d’Azur Regional Council. We are grateful to all those who have contributed to this research, notably the executive staff and employees of the Cannes Bel Age Association for older adults and the Regional Center of Biology and Sport Medicine (CRBMS). Address correspondence to: Melanie Emile, Universite Nice SophiaAntipolis, Faculte des Sciences du Sport – Laboratoire Motricite, Humaine, Education, Sport, Sante (EA 6312), 261, boulevard du Mercantour – BP 3259 – 06205 Nice cedex 3, France. E-mail: melanie. [email protected] 1

ID: rajae Time: 21:27 I Path: //chenas03/Cenpro/ApplicationFiles/Journals/Wiley/PSYP/Vol00000/160150/Comp/APPFile/JW-PSYP160150

38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 61 62

J_ID: PSYP Customer A_ID: PSYP12780 Cadmus Art: PSYP12780 Ed. Ref. No.: PSYP-2015-0232.R2 Date: 25-October-16

64 65 66 67 68 69 70 71 72 73 74 75 76 77 78 79 80 81 82 83 84 85 86 87 88 89 90 91 92 93 94 95 96 97 98 99 100 101 102 103 104 105 106 107 108 109 110 111 112 113 114 115 116 117 118 119 120 121 122 123 124

Page: 2

M. Emile et al.

2 63

Stage:

important implications for behavioral, cognitive, and affective variables. Studies in sports psychology conducted with children and young adults have consistently reported that an entity mindset is associated with maladaptive templates like decreased performance (Ommundsen, 2001) and decreased intrinsic motivation (Biddle, Wang, Chatzisarantis, & Spray, 2003), increased anxiety (Ommundsen, 2001), and ability attributions for failure (Spray, Wang, Biddle, & Chatzisarantis, & Warburton, 2006), whereas an incremental mindset is related to positive outcomes like increased performance (e.g., Da Fonseca et al., 2008; Sarrazin et al., 1996) and greater intrinsic motivation (e.g., Cury, Elliot, Da Fonseca, & Moller, 2006; Wang & Biddle, 2003). Indeed, an incremental view of ability is associated with the belief of effort efficacy and cognitive self-regulation, and has been found to foster a task or learning goal orientation in which individuals focus on improvement, learning, and developing new skills (Dweck, 1999; Ommundsen, Roberts, Lemyre, & Miller, 2005). Past studies have revealed that incremental beliefs may act as a buffer against the effects of negative stereotypes on cognitive performance (e.g., Aronson, Fried, & Good, 2002; Good, Aronson, & Inzlicht, 2003). In the present study, we examined how incremental versus entity beliefs interact with information that nullifies negative stereotypes to affect handgrip strength. We predicted that nullifying stereotypes during test performance would be more efficient among incremental theorists than entity theorists. This prediction was based on the concept of regulatory fit, which occurs when task framing is congruent with the goal or mindset of the individual (Higgins, 2000). In this situation, people engage more and perform better because their goal matches the demands and opportunities of the context. In the motor domain, girls were shown to perform better and to be less extrinsically motivated when asked to focus on avoiding mistakes, because this task framing matched their stereotype-induced avoidance motivation (Chalabaev, Dematte, Sarrazin, & Fontayne, 2014). Regulatory fit effects were also observed with regard to lay theories of ability (Sue-Chan, Wood, & Latham, 2012), with the authors showing that participants encouraged to focus on avoiding mistakes performed better when they held entity beliefs rather than incremental beliefs. This task framing matched the entity theorists’ motivation to avoid failure in order to demonstrate competence, but not the incremental theorists’ motivation to improve in order to attain a positive outcome (Van-Dijk & Kluger, 2004). Although the regulatory fit account has mainly been examined in younger adults, recent studies by Barber and colleagues have also supported the regulatory focus account of stereotype threat in older adults’ memory performance (Barber & Mather, 2013; Barber, Mather, & Gatz, 2015). Based on these results, we assumed that the performance deficits typically associated with stereotype threat would be reduced under regulatory fit, and we further predicted that information that nullifies the stereotype of an inevitable physical decline with aging would generate regulatory fit among incremental theorists but not entity theorists. Such information would be likely to match the sensitivity to progress and improvement that characterizes incremental beliefs, but not the belief that abilities are innate, which implies an inevitable decline with physiological aging. Previously, a study by Emile, Chalabaev, Stephan, Corrion, and d’Arripe-Longueville (2013) showed that an incremental mindset was associated with the positive belief that physical activity is beneficial for older adults’ health, whereas an entity mindset was associated with the negative belief that physical activity is risky. We examined these hypotheses in the context of older adults’ performance on a handgrip task. Several studies have indicated that

handgrip strength predicts functional and cognitive health among older adults (Raji et al., 2005; Taekema et al., 2010), notably frailty (Gale et al., 2007). Therefore, investigating whether performance on this test is affected by the social context in which the evaluations take place has considerable implications with regard to health practices.

125

Method

131

126 127 128 129 130

Participants

132

Members of seniors sports clubs in the southeast of France were directly contacted or were contacted through newsletters and email. Individuals were eligible to participate if they met the following criteria: (a) 60 years or older, (b) physically active, (c) medical certificate allowing the practice of adapted physical activities, and (d) independent living in the community. The exclusion criteria comprised any bone, muscle, or tendon trauma of the upper limbs (e.g., from shoulder to fingers). From a total of 98 older adults assessed for eligibility, 16 individuals were excluded because of health problems (i.e., osteoarthritis, carpal tunnel pain, shoulder or wrist tendonitis). The final sample was composed of 82 individuals (i.e., 13 men and 69 women) from 61 to 89 years old (Mage 5 75.8 years; SD 5 6.9) who met the criteria. Among them, 41.8% had not completed high school, and 58.2% had. All participants were informed of the study purpose and risks and gave informed written consent prior to enrollment. The protocol was institutionally approved and conformed to the Declaration of Helsinki.

133

Procedure

150

The participants were randomly assigned to one of three groups following a randomization procedure by computerized random numbers: (1) the entity condition (N 5 32), (2) the incremental condition (N 5 30), or (3) the control condition (N 5 20). They were invited into a private room and informed that the study purpose was to investigate muscle strength production during a handgrip task and that their performance would be measured twice. Based on a meta-analysis showing that stereotype threat effects are stronger when implicitly activated in older adults (Lamont, Swift, & Abrams, 2015), it was assumed that the handgrip task would implicitly induce age-stereotype threat. The experimenter, who was blind to the hypotheses and the group allocation, asked them to read a document providing handgrip task instructions. The participants were then familiarized with the equipment and performed submaximal and maximal contractions with sufficient rest between each contraction until they were accustomed to the task. Following this, they read the implicit theories of ability instructions, which differed according to the experimental group. Reading the instructions took fewer than 10 s. The participants in the incremental condition read that their strength was malleable and could be improved by training (i.e., incremental mindset), whereas those in the entity condition read that their strength was innate, fixed in time, and could not be improved even if they trained hard (i.e., entity mindset). Participants in both groups were told that this information was based on scientific evidence. After these instructions, all participants performed the first handgrip trial. Before performing the second trial, the experimenter provided stereotype-inconsistent information to the experimental groups by informing them that the test was free of age-related biases and that adults of all ages performed similarly. At the end of the handgrip tests, participants of the experimental groups were asked to complete several

151

ID: rajae Time: 21:27 I Path: //chenas03/Cenpro/ApplicationFiles/Journals/Wiley/PSYP/Vol00000/160150/Comp/APPFile/JW-PSYP160150

134 135 136 137 138 139 140 141 142 143 144 145 146 147 148 149

152 153 154 155 156 157 158 159 160 161 162 163 164 165 166 167 168 169 170 171 172 173 174 175 176 177 178 179 180 181

J_ID: PSYP Customer A_ID: PSYP12780 Cadmus Art: PSYP12780 Ed. Ref. No.: PSYP-2015-0232.R2 Date: 25-October-16

Stage:

Implicit theories, age stereotypes, handgrip strength

189

manipulation-check questionnaires. We assessed their beliefs about the malleability of physical ability (i.e., entity theory of ability vs. incremental theory of ability). In the control condition, no message related to their strength or stereotype-inconsistent information was given between the first and the second trials. Last, the participants were debriefed verbally and asked about the purpose of the study as a suspiciousness check (e.g., “What did you think this study was about?”).

190

Measures

191

Handgrip strength. The MicroFET 4 dynamometer (Hoggan Health Industries, West Jordan, UT) configured for wireless operation with a USB receiver connected to a computer was used to measure handgrip strength (in Newtons). In accordance with the standardized position recommended by the American Society of Hand Therapists (Fess & Moran, 1981), participants were seated with their dominant shoulder adducted and neutrally rotated, the elbow flexed at 908, and the forearm and wrist in neutral position. Hand dominance was determined by asking each participant if she/ he was right- or left-handed. Participants first performed familiarization trials with the instruction to “squeeze as hard and as fast as possible” to maximum force. We used a 3-s maximal voluntary contraction (MVC) for testing, since it is sufficient to obtain a maximum force value without exposing the participant to adverse effects like the increased blood pressure or heart rate associated with prolonged isometric muscle contraction. One MVC trial was performed before and after the stereotype-inconsistent information with the experimenter providing verbal encouragement to reach maximal exertions (Jung & Hallbeck, 2004). From these trials, the highest instantaneous force (peak MVC), the averaged maximal voluntary contraction (aMVC; the 500-ms average force produced over the peak MVC), and the rates of force development (RFD; the ability to develop force rapidly) were computed. The RFD values (N.s21) were calculated as the slope of the force-time curve over the time intervals of 0–50 and 0–100 ms relative to the onset of contraction (Aagaard, Simonsen, Andersen, Magnusson, & Dyhre-Poulsen, 2002). Also, peak RFD was computed as the highest slope value that occurred over the initial 200 ms of the force-time curve. Relative RFD values normalized with respect to peak MVC (expressed as %MVC.s21) were calculated for the different time intervals (RFD 50 N, RFD 100 N and peak RFD N). The normalized RFD provided information on the participant’s capacity to rapidly develop force, thus eliminating the influence of maximal force production.

182 183 184 185 186 187 188

192 193 194 195 196 197 198 199 200 201 202 203 204 205 206 207 208 209 210 211 212 213 214 215 216 217 218 219 220 221 222 223 224 225 226 227 228 229 230 231 232 233 234 235 236 237 238 239 240

Implicit theories of ability. Entity and incremental theories of ability were measured using the Conceptions of the Nature of Athletic Ability Questionnaire (Sarrazin et al., 1996). Given that we were interested in older adults’ beliefs about physical activity, we replaced the term sport by physical activity (Moreno, GonzalezCutre, Sicilia, & Spray, 2010; Ommundsen, 2003). We also adapted the questionnaire to muscle strength. The tool included six items: three items measured beliefs related to the stability of physical abilities, corresponding to the adoption of an entity theory (e.g., “Muscle strength in physical activities changes little even if much effort is made”), and three items measured beliefs related to the improvement of physical abilities, corresponding to the adoption of an incremental theory (e.g., “We have to train hard to build muscle strength in physical activities”). Participants answered on a 7-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (completely disagree) to 7 (completely agree). Each subscale presented good reliability (aincremental theory 5 .78, aentity theory 5 .81).

Page: 3

3 Stereotype-inconsistent information. We used one item (“Muscle strength does not depend on age”) to evaluate how much participants endorsed the stereotype-inconsistent information related to age and muscle strength. Participants answered on a 7-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (completely disagree) to 7 (completely agree). The item was coded such that higher scores indicated greater ease of endorsement.

241

Data Analysis

248

The significance of the Condition 3 Time interaction across all the handgrip measures was evaluated via a 3 3 2 multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA). Significant effects were followed up with univariate analyses and appropriate post hoc comparisons. For each dependent variable, a repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) was tested with group (i.e., entity theory vs. incremental theory vs. control group) as the between-participants factor and time (i.e., prestereotype-inconsistent manipulation vs. poststereotypeinconsistent manipulation vs. no stereotype-inconsistent manipulation) as the within-participant factor. Fischer LSD (least significant difference) post hoc tests were performed to analyze the experimental conditions, for each measurement time. The significance level was set at 5%. For the manipulation check, Student’s t tests were used to compare the means of the measured mindsets in the mean comparisons of the incremental and entity groups.

249

Results

264

242 243 244 245 246 247

250 251 252 253 254 255 256 257 258 259 260 261 262 263

Manipulation Check

265

ANOVAs revealed that participants in the incremental theory group had significantly higher incremental mindsets (M 5 6.34; SD 5 1.28) than those assigned to the entity theory group (M 5 2.20; SD 5 0.97, p < .001) and the control group (M 5 5.00; SD 5 2.08, p < .001), F(1,82) 5 48.10, p < .001, g2 5 .54, while participants in the entity theory group had higher entity mindsets (M 5 5.90; SD 5 1.06) than those in the incremental theory group (M 5 2.60; SD 5 1.27, p < .001) and the control group (M 5 3.62; SD 5 2.24, p < .001), F(1,82) 5 43.34, p < .001, g2 5 .53. Mean scores for endorsement of the stereotype-inconsistent information were significantly different between participants in the control group (M 5 5.13; SD 5 1.01) and those in the incremental theory group (M 5 6.35; SD 5 1.13, p < .001) and the entity theory group (M 5 6.26; SD 5 1.06, p < .001), F(1,82) 5 14.32, p < .001, g2 5 .26. These results show that the manipulations were successful in changing the participants’ mindsets. They also indicate that under normal baseline conditions the participants had on average moderate incremental and entity beliefs and that they endorsed stereotype-inconsistent information.

266 267 268 269 270 271 272 273 274 275 276 277 278 279 280 281 282 283 284 285

Handgrip Strength

286

Means and standard deviations of strength measures in the three groups are presented in Table 1. We performed an ANOVA to compare the scores at T1. We found no significant differences for the peak MVC, average MVC, peak RFD, peak RFD N, RFD 50 ms, RFD 50 N, RFD 100 ms, and RFD 100 N between the experimental groups. To test for omnibus effects across the dependent variables, a 3 (Group) 3 2 (Time) MANOVA was computed and revealed a significant Condition 3 Time interaction, Wilks’ k 5 .51, F(9,18) 5 3.16, p < .001, g2 5 .28. Follow-up univariate analyses were thus conducted for each of the eight handgrip variables.

287

ID: rajae Time: 21:27 I Path: //chenas03/Cenpro/ApplicationFiles/Journals/Wiley/PSYP/Vol00000/160150/Comp/APPFile/JW-PSYP160150

288 T1 289 290 291 292 293 294 295 296

J_ID: PSYP Customer A_ID: PSYP12780 Cadmus Art: PSYP12780 Ed. Ref. No.: PSYP-2015-0232.R2 Date: 25-October-16

Page: 4

Stage:

M. Emile et al.

4 Table 1. Means and Standard Deviations Before (T1) and After (T2) Induction of Stereotype-Inconsistent Information Entity beliefs Strength parameters Peak MVC (N) Peak RFD (N.s21) Peak RFDN (%MVC.s21) RFD 50 (N.s21) RFD 50N (%MVC.s21) RFD 100 (N.s21) RFD 100N (%MVC.s21) aMVC (N)

Incremental beliefs

Control group

T1

T2

T1

T2

T1

T2

201.1 6 90.7 1193.5 6 636.3 594.3 6 325.9 752.1 6 571.4 381.4 6 257.0 798.9 6 515.2 392.3 6 205.8 198.0 6 80.1

207.3 6 88.51 1196.5 6 715.1 536.2 6 208.7 722.6 6 625.4 340.6 6 244.6 812.2 6 605.4 359.5 6 207.4 201.2 6 72.2

213.1 6 94.0 1116.7 6 800.9 495.8 6 193.8 758.0 6 579.1 342.5 6 208.3 817.3 6 603.8 361.8 6 174.4 214.6 6 113.4

231.6 6 92.7 1361.9 6 787.8 576.4 6 184.1 1040.5 6 704.1 437.6 6 221.3 997.4 6 650.2 414.2 6 174.9 233.5 6 113.8

164.41 6 29.2 952.8 6 552.3 585.7 6 308.8 498.0 6 407.1 236.8 6 202.4 487.1 6 481.1 207.4 6 145.2 158.4 6 28.5

154.0 6 25.3 777.4 6 614.8 507.5 6 349.8 590.1 6 389.8 263.1 6 190.0 441.2 6 303.3 224.0 6 144.2 148.8 6 32.2

Note. The values at T1 were not significantly different between the experimental conditions. N 5 Newton; N.s21 5 Newtons per second; %MVC.s21 5 percentage of MVC per second.

297 298 299 300 301 302 303 304 305 306 307 308 309 310 311 312 313 314

F1

315 316 317 318 319

found, F(1,79) 5 4.55, p < .05, g2 5 .10. The peak RFD score significantly improved in the incremental theory group from pre- to poststereotype manipulation (p < .01). No significant effect was found in the entity theory or the control group.

320

Rate of force development 50 (RFD 50). A significant Group (entity vs. incremental vs. control) 3 Time (prestereotype manipulation vs. poststereotype manipulation vs. no stereotypeinconsistent manipulation) interaction effect on RFD 50 was also found, F(1,79) 5 3.26, p < .05, g2 5 .08. The RFD 50 score significantly improved in the incremental theory group from pre- to poststereotype manipulation (p < .01). No significant effect was found in the entity theory or the control group (see Figure 2). No significant Group 3 Stereotype manipulation interaction effects were found for peak RFD N, F(1,79) 5 2.08, p 5 .13, g2 5 .05; RFD 50 N, F(1,79) 5 2.84, p 5 .10, g2 5 .06; RFD 100, F(1,79) 5 1.22, p 5 .30, g2 5 .03; or RFD 100 N, F(1,79) 5 1.40, p 5 .25, g2 5 .03.

324

Discussion

337

Peak rate of force development (peak RFD). A significant Group (entity vs. incremental vs. control) 3 Time (prestereotype manipulation vs. poststereotype manipulation vs. no stereotypeinconsistent manipulation) interaction effect on peak RFD was also

In this research, we found evidence that nullifying the stereotypes related to physical decline with aging improved muscle strength and may have moderated the effects of implicit theories of ability in older adults. We had originally assumed that the mindset

338

Figure 1. Average maximal voluntary contraction by implicit theory of ability from pre- (T1) to post- (T2) stereotype manipulation. Error bars represent standards deviations. aMVC 5 average maximal voluntary contraction; N.s21 5 Newtons per second. ** p < .01: significant difference in the incremental theory group from pre- to poststereotype manipulation.

Figure 2. Rate of force development in the first 50 ms by implicit theory of ability conditions from pre- (T1) to post- (T2) stereotype manipulation. Error bars represent standards deviations. N.s21 5 Newtons per second. ** p < .01: significant difference in the incremental theory group from pre- to poststereotype manipulation.

Peak MVC. A significant Group (entity vs. incremental vs. control) 3 Time (prestereotype manipulation vs. poststereotype manipulation vs. no stereotype-inconsistent manipulation) interaction effect on muscle strength was found, F(1,79) 5 9.33, p < .001, g2 5 .19. More particularly, whereas the peak MVC score significantly improved in the incremental theory group from pre- to poststereotype manipulation (p < .01), it significantly decreased in the control group between the two handgrip trials (p < .05). In the entity theory, no significant interaction effect was found. Average MVC. A significant Group (entity vs. incremental vs. control) 3 Time (prestereotype manipulation vs. poststereotype manipulation vs. no stereotype-inconsistent manipulation) interaction effect on muscle strength was found, F(1,79) 5 10.6, p < .001, g2 5 .21. More particularly, whereas the average MVC score significantly improved in the incremental theory group from pre- to poststereotype manipulation (p < .001), it significantly decreased in the control group from pre- to poststereotype manipulation (p < .05). In the entity theory group, no significant interaction effect was found (see Figure 1).

ID: rajae Time: 21:27 I Path: //chenas03/Cenpro/ApplicationFiles/Journals/Wiley/PSYP/Vol00000/160150/Comp/APPFile/JW-PSYP160150

321 322 323

325 326 327 328 329 330 331 F2 332 333 334 335 336

339 340 341

J_ID: PSYP Customer A_ID: PSYP12780 Cadmus Art: PSYP12780 Ed. Ref. No.: PSYP-2015-0232.R2 Date: 25-October-16

Stage:

Implicit theories, age stereotypes, handgrip strength 342 343 344 345 346 347 348 349 350 351 352 353 354 355 356 357 358 359 360 361 362 363 364 365 366 367 368 369 370 371 372 373 374 375 376 377 378 379 380 381 382 383 384 385 386 387 388 389 390 391 392 393 394 395 396

Page: 5

5

imposed by stereotype-inconsistent information would be strong enough to overwhelm an individual’s own implicit beliefs of ability, and the findings were partly consistent with our hypotheses. Older adults assigned to the incremental mindset condition obtained higher scores for some of the components of muscle strength production (i.e., peak MVC, average MVC) and for their ability to rapidly develop force (i.e., peak RFD, RFD 50) after exposure to the stereotype-inconsistent information, whereas those in the entity mindset condition and in the control group were not significantly influenced by this information. These results corroborate the hypothesis that information nullifying the stereotype of an inevitable physical decline with aging generates regulatory fit among incremental theorists but not entity theorists. One explanation is that stereotype-inconsistent information is congruent with the sensitivity to progress and improvement that characterizes incremental beliefs, but not with the belief that abilities are innate and therefore decline with physiological aging. In line with this idea, Emile et al. (2013) found that an incremental mindset was associated with the positive belief that physical activity is beneficial for older adults’ health, whereas an entity mindset was associated with the negative belief that physical activity is risky for older adults’ health. These results also extend recent findings supporting the regulatory fit account of stereotype threat in older adults’ memory performance to the domain of motor performance (Barber & Mather, 2013; Barber et al., 2015). Another interesting set of results was that nullifying stereotypes among incremental mindset holders did not have the same influence on all the strength measures. We found that providing stereotypeinconsistent information improved both the ability to produce maximal force (i.e., peak MVC, average MVC) and the ability to develop force rapidly (peak RFD, RFD 50) of the participants. However, no effect on RFD 100 or normalized RFD scores was observed (i.e., peak RFD N, RFD 50 N, RFD 100 N). The increase in peak MVC and average MVC in the incremental mindset participants might be explained by the increased motivation that would result from regulatory fit. This idea is consistent with the research showing that feedback framed to the regulatory focus is likely to increase motivation (Dweck & Leggett, 1988; Jarzebowski, Palermo, & van de Berg, 2012; Motyka et al., 2014). This interpretation also agrees with previous work showing that motivation resulting from verbal encouragement leads to maximal voluntary contraction increase (Jung & Hallbeck, 2004; McNair, Depledge, Brettkelly, & Stanley, 1996). A complementary explanation is that an incremental theory of ability combined with stereotype-inconsistent information unconsciously improves neural drive to the muscles (Andersen & Aagaard, 2006; Van Cutsem, Duchateau, & Hainaut, 1998). Chalabaev et al. (2013) reported that stereotype threat inhibited RFD by influencing the preparatory processes associated with the upcoming motor task. In the present study, it can thus be speculated that the enhanced RFD 50 scores of the incremental mindset participants after stereotypeinconsistent information is ascribable to a neural adaptation. It has indeed been observed that the early phase of voluntary RFD (< 50 ms) depends predominantly on neural factors, whereas the late phase (> 50 ms) relies more on contractile muscle properties (Andersen & Aagaard, 2006; Folland, Buckthorpe, & Hannah, 2014). This

speculation is further reinforced by the lack of significant change in RFD 100, as it is unlikely that changes within the muscle could have occurred during the short duration of the experiment. Therefore, the increased peak RFD score (e.g., 0–200 ms) is mainly due to the higher early-phase RFD score. Given that no effects were observed for the normalized RFD scores, we cannot fully assert that the increase in peak RFD and RFD 50 were not influenced by the enhanced maximal force production. But within the methodology used in the present investigation, we can suggest that providing stereotype-inconsistent information to active older adults has the potential to improve handgrip muscle strength through neural adaptations. Further research is nevertheless needed to provide evidence of the neural mechanisms underlying the increased muscle strength and RFD scores observed here. Some limitations of the current research need to be considered and open promising research avenues. First, our sample was mainly composed of older women, who may have had gender stereotypes in addition to aging stereotypes (e.g., Pinquart & S€orensen, 2001). Future research could thus replicate this study with a more genderbalanced sample to better control for this confounding effect and examine the potential gender differences. Second, the study was designed to test the effectiveness of stereotype-inconsistent information depending on lay theories of ability. Future studies could examine whether lay beliefs also interact with stereotype threat. Our study revealed that stereotype-inconsistent information combined with incremental theory was successful, but it was unsuccessful combined with entity theory. Why did entity theory resist the stereotypeinconsistent information? An intervention study with an explicit stereotype threat could be designed to examine the relationship between entity theory and stereotype threat. Third, our design did not allow us to determine whether the incremental group’s performance gain was due to the incremental belief or to the stereotypeinconsistent information. Indeed, mentioning that strength could be improved with training may have been sufficient to increase performance. Future research should examine this question by, for example, adding a second control group to manipulate theories of ability without manipulating stereotype-inconsistent information. Last, this study measured the effects of variables on muscle strength at a point in time. It would be interesting to examine these effects over time through a longitudinal experimental study using several messages of age-related stereotype-inconsistent information delivered repeatedly. In summary, from the perspective of public health promotion of exercise for the elderly, the results of this study are encouraging because they suggest that individuals with an incremental mindset who are exposed to stereotype-inconsistent information can boost their physical performance, providing support to the regulatory fit perspective. We are not aware of other studies that have specifically considered the role of stereotype-inconsistent information in modulating the effects of implicit theories of ability on muscle strength. Messages that promote an incremental mindset and stereotypeinconsistent information should perhaps be emphasized. This study thus paves the way for future research aiming at a better understanding of implicit theories of ability and stereotype-inconsistent information as sources of greater physical activity in older adults.

397 398 399 400 401 402 403 404 405 406 407 408 409 410 411 412 413 414 415 416 417 418 419 420 421 422 423 424 425 426 427 428 429 430 431 432 433 434 435 436 437 438 439 440 441 442 443 444 445 446 447 448 449 450

451 452 453 454 455 456

References Aagaard, P., Simonsen, E. B., Andersen, J. L., Magnusson, P., & DyhrePoulsen, P. (2002). Increased rate of force development and neural drive of human skeletal muscle following resistance training. Journal of Applied Physiology, 93, 1318–1326. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00283.2002

Andersen, L. L., & Aagaard, P. (2006). Influence of maximal muscle strength and intrinsic muscle contractile properties on contractile rate of force development. European Journal of Applied Physiology, 96, 46–52. doi: 10.1007/s00421-005-0070-z

ID: rajae Time: 21:27 I Path: //chenas03/Cenpro/ApplicationFiles/Journals/Wiley/PSYP/Vol00000/160150/Comp/APPFile/JW-PSYP160150

457 458 459 460

J_ID: PSYP Customer A_ID: PSYP12780 Cadmus Art: PSYP12780 Ed. Ref. No.: PSYP-2015-0232.R2 Date: 25-October-16

Page: 6

M. Emile et al.

6 461 462 463 464 465 466 467 468 469 470 471 472 473 474 475 476 477 478 479 480 481 482 483 484 485 486 487 488 489 490 491 492 493 494 495 496 497 498 499 500 501 502 503 504 505 506 507 508 509 510 511 512 513 514 515 516 517 518 519 520 521 522 523 524 525 526 527 528 529 530 531 532 533 534 535

Stage:

Aronson, J., Fried, C. B., & Good, C. (2002). Reducing the effects of stereotype threat on African American college students by shaping theories of intelligence. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 113–125. doi: 10.1006/jesp.2001.1491 Barber, S. J., & Mather, M. (2013). Stereotype threat can both enhance and impair older adults’ memory. Psychological Science, 24, 2522–2529. doi: 10.1177/0956797613497023 Barber, S. J., Mather, M., & Gatz, M. (2015). How stereotype threat affects healthy older adults’ performance on clinical assessments of cognitive decline: The key role of regulatory fit. Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 70(6) 891–900. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbv009 Biddle, S. J., Wang, C. J., Chatzisarantis, N. L., & Spray, C. M. (2003). Motivation for physical activity in young people: Entity and incremental beliefs about athletic ability. Journal of Sports Science, 21, 973– 989. doi: 10.1080/02640410310001641377 Chalabaev, A., Brisswalter, J., Radel, R., Coombes, S. A., Easthope, C., & Clement-Guillotin, C. (2013). Can stereotype threat affect motor performance in the absence of explicit monitoring processes? Evidence using a strength task. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 35, 211–215. doi: 10.1123/jsep.35.2.211 Chalabaev, A., Dematte, E., Sarrazin, P., & Fontayne, P. (2014). Creating regulatory fit under stereotype threat: Effects on performance and selfdetermination among junior high school students. International Review of Social Psychology, 27, 119–132. Cruz-Jentoft, A. J., Landi, F., Schneider, S. M., Z u~ niga, C., Arai, H., Boirie, Y., . . . Cederholm, T. (2014). Prevalence of and interventions for sarcopenia in ageing adults: A systematic review. Report of the International Sarcopenia Initiative (EWGSOP and IWGS). Age and Ageing, 43, 748–759. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afu115 Cury, F., Elliot, A. J., Da Fonseca, D., & Moller, A. C. (2006). The socialcognitive model of achievement motivation and the 23 2 achievement goal framework. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 666–679. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.90.4.666 Da Fonseca, D., Cury, F., Fakra, E., Rufo, M., Poinso, F., Bounoua, L., & Huguet, P. (2008). Implicit theories of intelligence and IQ test performance in adolescents with generalized anxiety disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46, 529–536. doi: 10.1016/ j.brat.2008.01.006 Dodds, R., Kuh, D., Aihie Sayer, A., & Cooper, R. (2013). Physical activity levels across adult life and grip strength in early old age: Updating findings from a British birth cohort. Age and Ageing, 42, 794–798. doi: 10.1093/ageing/aft124 Dweck, C. S. (1986). Motivational processes affecting learning. American Psychologist, 41, 1040–1048. doi: 10.1037/003-066x.41. 10.1040 Dweck, C. S. (1999). Self-theories: Their role in motivation, personality, and development. Philadelphia, PA: Taylor & Francis. Dweck, C. S., & Leggett, E. L. (1988). A social cognitive approach to motivation and personality. Psychological Review, 95, 256–273. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.95.2.256 Emile, M., Chalabaev, A., Stephan, Y., Corrion, K., & d’ArripeLongueville, F. (2013). Aging stereotypes and active lifestyle: Personal correlates of stereotype internalization and relationships with level of physical activity among older adults. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 15, 198–204. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2013.11.002 Fess, E. E., & Moran, C. A. (1981). Clinical assessment recommendations. Philadelphia, PA: American Society of Hand Therapists. Folland, J. P., Buckthorpe, M. W., & Hannah, R. (2014). Human capacity for explosive force production: Neural and contractile determinants. Scandinavian Journal of Medicine Sciences in Sports, 24, 894–906. doi: 10.1111/sms.12131 Gale, C. R., Martyn, C. N., Cooper, C., & Sayer, A. A. (2007). Grip strength, body composition, and mortality. International Journal of Epidemiology, 36, 228–235. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyl224 Good, C., Aronson, J., & Inzlicht, M. (2003). Improving adolescents’ standardized test performance: An intervention to reduce the effects of stereotype threat. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 24, 645–662. doi: 10.1016/j.appdev.2003.09.002 Higgins, E. T. (2000). Making a good decision: Value from fit. American Psychologist, 55, 1217–1230. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.55.11. 1217 Jarzebowski, A., Palermo, J., & van de Berg, R. (2012). When feedback is not enough: The impact of regulatory fit on motivation after positive feedback. International Coaching Psychology Review, 7, 14–32.

Jung, M.-C., & Hallbeck, M. S. (2004). Quantification of the effects of instruction type, verbal encouragement, and visual feedback on static and peak handgrip strength. International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics, 34, 367–374. doi: 10.1016/j.ergon.2004.03.008 Lamont, R. A., Swift, H. J., & Abrams, D. (2015). A review and metaanalysis of age-based stereotype threat: Negative stereotypes, not facts, do the damage. Psychology and Aging, 30, 180–193. doi: 10.1037/ a0038586 Lauretani, F., Russo, C. R., Bandinelli, S., Bartali, B., Cavazzini, C., Di Iorio, A., . . . Ferrucci, L. (2003). Age-associated changes in skeletal muscles and their effect on mobility: an operational diagnosis of sarcopenia. Journal of Applied Physiology, 95, 1851–1860. doi: 10.1152/ japplphysiol.00246.2003 McNair, P. J., Depledge, J., Brettkelly, M., & Stanley, S. N. (1996). Verbal encouragement: Effects on maximum effort voluntary muscle action. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 30, 243–245. doi: 10.1136/ bjsm.30.3.243  & Spray, C. M. (2010). Moreno, J. A., Gonzalez-Cutre, D., Sicilia, A., Motivation in the exercise setting: Integrating constructs from the approach–avoidance achievement goal framework and selfdetermination theory. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 11, 542–550. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2010.06.003 Motyka, S., Grewal, D., Puccinelli, N. M., Roggeveen, A. L., Avnet, T., Daryanto, A., . . . Wetzels, M. (2014). Regulatory fit: A meta-analytic synthesis. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 24, 394–410. doi: 10.1016/j.jcps.2013.11.004 Norman, K., Stob€aus, N., Gonzalez, M. C., Schulzke, J. D., & Pirlich, M. (2011). Hand grip strength: outcome predictor and marker of nutritional status. Clinical Nutrition, 30, 135–142. doi: 10.1016/j.clnu.2010.09.010 Ommundsen, Y. (2001). Self-handicapping strategies in physical education classes: The influence of implicit theories of the nature of ability and achievement goal orientations. Psychology of Sports and Exercise, 2, 139–156. doi: 10.1016/S1469-0292(00)00019-4 Ommundsen, Y. (2003). Implicit theories of ability and self-regulation strategies in physical education classes. Educational Psychology, 23, 141–157. doi: 10.1080/01443410303224 Ommundsen, Y., Roberts, G. C., Lemyre, P. N., & Miller, B. W. (2005). Peer relationships in adolescent competitive soccer: Associations to perceived motivational climate, achievement goals and perfectionism. Journal of Sports Sciences, 23, 977–989. doi: 10.1080/ 02640410500127975 Pinquart, M., & S€ orensen, S. (2001). Gender differences in self-concept and psychological well-being in old age: A meta-analysis. Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 56, 195–213. doi: 10.1093/geronb/56.4.P195 Raji, M. A., Kuo, Y. F., Snih, S. A., Markides, K. S., Peek, M. K., & Ottenbacher, K. J. (2005). Cognitive status, muscle strength, and subsequent disability in older Mexican Americans. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 53, 1462–1468. doi: 10.1111/j.15325415.2005.53457.x Sarrazin, P., Biddle, S., Famose, J., Cury, F., Fox, K., & Durand, M. (1996). Goal orientations and conceptions of the nature of sport ability in children: A social cognitive approach. British Journal of Social Psychology, 35, 399–414. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8309.1996.tb01104.x Spray, C. M., Wang, C. K., Biddle, S. J., Chatzisarantis, N. L., & Warburton, V. E. (2006). An experimental test of self-theories of ability in youth sport. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 7, 255–267. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2005.05.001 Stenholm, S., Tiainen, K., Rantanen, T., Sainio, P., Heli€ ovaara, M., Impivaara, O., & Koskinen, S. (2012). Long-term determinants of muscle strength decline: Prospective evidence from the 22-year mini-Finland follow-up survey. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 60(1), 77–85. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2011.03779.x Stephan, Y., Chalabaev, A., Kotter-Gr€ uhn, D., & Jaconelli, A. (2013). “Feeling younger, being stronger”: An experimental study of subjective age and physical functioning among older adults. Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 68, 1–7. Sue-Chan, C., Wood, R. E., & Latham, G. P. (2012). Effect of a coach’s regulatory focus and an individual’s implicit person theory on individual performance. Journal of Management, 38, 809–835. Taekema, D. G., Gussekloo, J., Maier, A. B., Westendorp, R. G., & de Craen, A. J. (2010). Handgrip strength as a predictor of functional, psychological and social health. A prospective population-based study among the oldest old. Age and Ageing, 39, 331–337. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afq022

ID: rajae Time: 21:27 I Path: //chenas03/Cenpro/ApplicationFiles/Journals/Wiley/PSYP/Vol00000/160150/Comp/APPFile/JW-PSYP160150

536 537 538 539 540 541 542 543 544 545 546 547 548 549 550 551 552 553 554 555 556 557 558 559 560 561 562 563 564 565 566 567 568 569 570 571 572 573 574 575 576 577 578 579 580 581 582 583 584 585 586 587 588 589 590 591 592 593 594 595 596 597 598 599 600 601 602 603 604 605 606 607 608 609 610

J_ID: PSYP Customer A_ID: PSYP12780 Cadmus Art: PSYP12780 Ed. Ref. No.: PSYP-2015-0232.R2 Date: 25-October-16

Stage:

Implicit theories, age stereotypes, handgrip strength 611 612 613 614 615 616 617

Van Cutsem, M., Duchateau, J., & Hainaut, K. (1998). Changes in single motor unit behavior contribute to the increase in contraction speed after dynamic training in humans. Journal of Physiology, 513, 295–305. Van-Dijk, D., & Kluger, A. N. (2004). Feedback sign effect on motivation: Is it moderated by regulatory focus? Applied Psychology, 53, 113–135. doi: 10.1111/j.1464-0597.2004.00163.x

Page: 7

7 Wang, C. J., & Biddle, S. J. (2003). Intrinsic motivation towards sports in Singaporean students: The role of sport ability beliefs. Journal of Health Psychology, 8, 515–523. doi: 10.1177/ 13591053030085004

618 619 620 621 622

(RECEIVED August 11, 2015; ACCEPTED September 30, 2016)

624

ID: rajae Time: 21:27 I Path: //chenas03/Cenpro/ApplicationFiles/Journals/Wiley/PSYP/Vol00000/160150/Comp/APPFile/JW-PSYP160150

623

J_ID: PSYP Customer A_ID: PSYP12780 Cadmus Art: PSYP12780 Ed. Ref. No.: PSYP-2015-0232.R2 Date: 25-October-16

Stage:

Page: 8

AUTHOR QUERY FORM Dear Author, During the preparation of your manuscript for publication, the questions listed below have arisen. Please attend to these matters and return this form with your proof. Many thanks for your assistance.

Query References

Query

AQ1

Please confirm that given names (red) and surnames/family names (green) have been identified correctly.

Remarks

ID: rajae Time: 21:27 I Path: //chenas03/Cenpro/ApplicationFiles/Journals/Wiley/PSYP/Vol00000/160150/Comp/APPFile/JW-PSYP160150