Large-Scale Network Coupling with the Fusiform Cortex ... - eNeuro

2 downloads 0 Views 915KB Size Report
Oct 3, 2017 - Amanda V. Utevsky1,2, David V. Smith3, Jacob S. Young4, Scott A. Huettel1,2*. 5 ...... transcranial magnetic stimulation (Fox et al., 2012; Luber and Lisanby, .... Berbaum, K.S., Franken, E.A., Dorfman, D.D., Rooholamini, S.A., ...
This Accepted Manuscript has not been copyedited and formatted. The final version may differ from this version.

Research Article: New Research | Cognition and Behavior

Large-Scale Network Coupling with the Fusiform Cortex Facilitates Future Social Motivation Network Coupling Facilitates Social Motivation 1,2

Amanda V. Utevsky

3

4

, David V. Smith , Jacob S. Young and Scott A. Huettel

1,2

1

Center for Cognitive Neuroscience, Duke University, Durham, NC 27708

2

Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, Duke University, Durham, NC 27708

3

Department of Psychology, Temple University, Philadelphia, PA 19122

4

Pritzker School of Medicine, University of Chicago, Chicago, IL 60637

DOI: 10.1523/ENEURO.0084-17.2017 Received: 15 March 2017 Revised: 20 September 2017 Accepted: 26 September 2017 Published: 3 October 2017

Author Contributions: AVU, DVS, JSY, and SAH designed research. AVU, DVS, JSY performed research. AVU, DVS, JYS analyzed data. AVU, DVS, SAH wrote the paper. Funding: HHS | NIH | National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) 100000025 F31-MH105137

Funding: HHS | NIH | National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) 100000025 F32-MH107175

Funding: HHS | National Institutes of Health (NIH) 100000002 NIMH RC1-88680

Funding: Duke Institute for Brain Sciences, Duke University (DIBS) 100005572

Conflict of Interest: Authors report no conflict of interest. This study was funded by a grant from the National Institutes of Health (NIMH RC1-88680), an Incubator Award from the Duke Institute for Brain Sciences (SAH), and by NIMH National Research Service Awards F31MH105137 (AVU) and F32-MH107175 (DVS). Correspondence should be addressed to Address for correspondence: Scott A. Huettel, Box 90999, Duke University, Durham, NC 27708, USA. E-mail: [email protected] phone: (919) 668-5286, fax: (919) 681-0815 Cite as: eNeuro 2017; 10.1523/ENEURO.0084-17.2017 Alerts: Sign up at eneuro.org/alerts to receive customized email alerts when the fully formatted version of this article is published.

Accepted manuscripts are peer-reviewed but have not been through the copyediting, formatting, or proofreading process. Copyright © 2017 Utevsky et al. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42

Large-Scale Network Coupling with the Fusiform Cortex Facilitates Future Social Motivation Abbreviated Title: Network Coupling Facilitates Social Motivation Amanda V. Utevsky1,2, David V. Smith3, Jacob S. Young4, Scott A. Huettel1,2* 1 Center for Cognitive Neuroscience, Duke University, Durham, NC, 27708 2 Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, Duke University, Durham, NC, 27708 3 Department of Psychology, Temple University, Philadelphia, PA, 19122 4 Pritzker School of Medicine, University of Chicago, Chicago, IL, 60637 AVU, DVS, JSY, and SAH designed research. AVU, DVS, JSY performed research. AVU, DVS, JYS analyzed data. AVU, DVS, SAH wrote the paper. *Address for correspondence: Scott A. Huettel Box 90999, Duke University Durham, NC 27708, USA [email protected] phone: (919) 668-5286 fax: (919) 681-0815 Number of Figures: 4 Number of Tables: 2 Number of words for Abstract: 233 Number of words for Significance Statement: 116 Number of words for Introduction: 637 Number of words for Discussion: 1759 Acknowledgments: This study was funded by a grant from the National Institutes of Health (NIMH RC1-88680), an Incubator Award from the Duke Institute for Brain Sciences (SAH), and by NIMH National Research Service Awards F31-MH105137 (AVU) and F32-MH107175 (DVS). The authors thank Christopher Coutlee and Cary Politzer for their assistance in task design and data collection. Conflict of Interest: Authors report no conflict of interest. Funding sources: National Institutes of Health (NIMH RC1-88680), the Duke Institute for Brain Sciences (SAH), NIMH National Research Service Awards F31-MH105137, F32MH107175 (DVS).

Page 1 of 38

43

Abstract

44

Large-scale functional networks – as identified through the coordinated activity of spatially

45

distributed brain regions – have become central objects of study in neuroscience because of

46

their contributions to many processing domains. Yet, it remains unclear how these domain-

47

general networks interact with focal brain regions to coordinate thought and action. Here,

48

we investigated how the default-mode network (DMN) and executive control network (ECN)

49

– two networks associated with goal-directed behavior – shape task performance through

50

their coupling with other cortical regions several seconds in advance of behavior. We

51

measured these networks’ connectivity during an adaptation of the monetary incentive

52

delay response-time task in which human participants viewed social and non-social images

53

(i.e., pictures of faces and landscapes, respectively) while brain activity was measured using

54

fMRI. We found that participants displayed slower response times (RTs) subsequent to

55

social trials relative to nonsocial trials. To examine the neural mechanisms driving this

56

subsequent-RT effect, we integrated independent components analysis (ICA) and a network-

57

based psychophysiological interaction (nPPI) analysis; this allowed us to investigate task-

58

related changes in network coupling that preceded the observed trial-to-trial variation in RT.

59

Strikingly, when subjects viewed social rewards, an area of the fusiform gyrus consistent

60

with the functionally-defined fusiform face area (FFA) exhibited increased coupling with the

61

ECN (relative to the DMN) – and the relative magnitude of coupling tracked the slowing of

62

RT on the following trial. These results demonstrate how large-scale, domain-general

63

networks can interact with focal, domain-specific cortical regions to orchestrate subsequent

64

behavior.

Page 2 of 38

65 66 67

Keywords: effective connectivity, executive control, default mode network, faces, goal-

68

directed behavior

69

70 71 72 73 74 75 76 77 78 79 80 81 82 83

Page 3 of 38

84

Significance

85

A diverse set of behaviors – from normal to pathological – has been linked to the responses

86

of large-scale functional networks. Yet, it remains unclear how these domain-general

87

networks shape subsequent processing in cortical regions with domain-specific function.

88

Here, we examine how two such networks — the default-mode network (DMN) and

89

executive control network (ECN) — connect functionally with other cortical regions to alter

90

performance in an incentive-compatible task. We found that differential coupling between a

91

prototypical face processing region and DMN and ECN tracked subsequent improvements in

92

performance to social stimuli. Our approach allowed us to examine direct coupling with

93

functional networks to future behavior, providing a significant step forward in

94

understanding how large-scale networks coordinate thought and action.

95 96 97

Page 4 of 38

98 99

Introduction Since the discovery of functionally correlated brain regions (Biswal et al., 1995),

100

large-scale functional networks have been considered fundamental features of brain activity

101

(Beckmann et al., 2005; Behrens and Sporns, 2012; Gusnard and Raichle, 2001). These

102

networks are highly reliable across large samples of participants (Biswal et al., 2010; Smith

103

et al., 2013) and are thought to reflect intrinsic properties of brain organization. Many of

104

these networks reflect sensory and perceptual processes instantiated within visual or

105

auditory regions (Smith et al., 2009). Still others contribute broadly to cognitive processing

106

across stimulus and task domains, including the default-mode (DMN), executive control

107

(ECN), and frontoparietal (FPN) networks (Dosenbach et al., 2007; Smith et al., 2009). These

108

functional networks have been linked to various aspects of behavior including demographic

109

variables (Cowdrey et al., 2012; Filippini et al., 2009), traits (Kucyi et al., 2014; Shannon et

110

al., 2011), and cognitive states (De Havas et al., 2012; Smith et al., 2009).

111

Yet, for effective behavior in a particular task, these large-scale domain-general

112

networks must alter ongoing task-specific processing in focal brain regions. To claim that

113

such interactions (i.e., between large-scale networks and specific brain regions) are critical

114

for behavior change, several conditions should be met. First, a large-scale network should be

115

identifiable during task performance independently of other concurrent activation; that is,

116

networks should be able to be extracted regardless of other processes occurring in focal

117

brain regions that might contribute to that task (i.e., without relying on seed-based analyses;

118

Cole et al., 2010; Smith et al., 2014b). Second, the coupling between a given network and a

119

given focal brain region should systematically vary across task conditions according to the

120

relative engagement of the task (Friston et al., 1997; O’Reilly et al., 2012). Third, those Page 5 of 38

121

changes in coupling (e.g., effective connectivity (Friston, 2011)) should predict the

122

characteristics of subsequent behavior, to provide evidence that the coupling contributes to

123

effective task performance. If these conditions are met, there would be strong evidence that

124

coupling between a functional network and a focal brain region contributes to a specific

125

cognitive process.

126

In the current study, we examined effective connectivity between large-scale

127

networks and focal brain regions while subjects played a response-time game to receive

128

social and nonsocial rewards in a modified monetary incentive delay (MID) task (Knutson et

129

al., 2000). We have previously used this task to produce meaningful variability in reaction

130

time behavior that reflects relative motivation (Clithero et al., 2011). We focused on the

131

DMN and ECN since both have been linked to task performance, engagement, and other

132

markers of executive function and preparatory behavior. Behaviorally, we found that

133

participants exhibited slower response times (RTs) subsequent to social relative to

134

nonsocial trials, reflecting a change in motivation according to social stimulus type. We

135

investigated whether coupling with DMN and ECN contributed to this subsequent-RT effect

136

by combining independent components (ICA) and psychophysiological interactions (PPI)

137

analyses. This network PPI (nPPI) pipeline allowed us to examine how DMN and ECN

138

contribute to other cortical regions to shape subsequent motivated behavior up to several

139

seconds later. Strikingly, we found that DMN and ECN differentially coupled with a region in

140

the fusiform gyrus (FG) when subjects viewed the social rewards, and that changes in this

141

coupling tracked the effect of stimulus type on subsequent RT. This region of the FG is

142

consistent with the functionally-defined fusiform face area (FFA) – a region classically

143

associated with face processing (Kanwisher et al., 1997; McCarthy et al., 1997).

Page 6 of 38

144

These findings highlight functional network interactions that guide subsequent

145

changes in reaction time behavior. While the cognitive mechanisms underlying the

146

behavioral effect of previous stimulus type on subsequent reaction time require future

147

research, we propose that this effect may be driven by increased attentional interference of

148

social rewards relative to nonsocial rewards or a subsequent change in task motivation.

149

Collectively, these results indicate that functional networks associated with goal-directed

150

behavior can interact with focal brain regions to support future motivated behavior.

151

Materials and Methods

152

Participants

153

A group of 50 self-reported heterosexual males completed the task (mean age: 23.8

154

years, range 18-32); this sample size was established prior to data collection. All participants

155

were screened prior to data collection to rule out prior or current psychiatric or neurological

156

illness. We excluded four participants because of data quality issues (see Preprocessing), and

157

we excluded five participants because of a malfunctioning button box in the scanner. These

158

exclusions left a final sample of 41 participants (mean age: 24.1 years, range: 18-32). All

159

participants gave written informed consent as part of a protocol approved by the

160

Institutional Review Board of Duke University Medical Center.

161

Stimuli and Tasks

162

The experiment consisted of four components: 1) a training session outside the

163

scanner; 2) a modified monetary incentive delay task (MID; Knutson et al., 2000, 2001); 3) a

164

passive viewing task with results previously published (Young et al., 2015); 4) two

Page 7 of 38

165

additional post-scan rating tasks. All participants received variable cash payment depending

166

on their performance.

167

In advance of the neuroimaging session, participants memorized the associations of

168

four fractal images with different potential rewards (see an example in Figure 1). On the day

169

of the scan, subjects were tested on the fractal images’ associations to ensure they

170

remembered the meaning of each image; if participants identified every association correctly

171

on the first attempt (n = 31), they received an extra $5 in addition to their study

172

compensation. All participants successfully learned the image associations before entering

173

the scanner.

174

Each trial of the modified MID task began with a fractal image presented as a cue for

175

1500 ms (Figure 1). The different cues indicated the different potential benefits of a fast

176

response on that trial: [1] reducing the delay until the presentation of a face image of a

177

variable attractiveness rating (Social Delay; SD), [2] increasing the attractiveness of a face

178

image presented after a variable duration (Social Attractiveness; SA), [3] reducing the delay

179

until the presentation of a landscape image of a variable attractiveness rating (Nonsocial

180

Delay; ND), [4] increasing the attractiveness of a landscape image presented after a variable

181

duration (Nonsocial Attractiveness; NA).

182

Following the cue, a fixation cross appeared for a variable duration (interstimulus

183

interval; 1500 – 4500 ms), followed by a target (a white triangle) that appeared on the

184

screen for 750 ms. Participants then responded by pressing a button box with their right

185

index finger before the target disappeared. A feedback shape then appeared for 750 ms; that

186

shape was colored green if the participant’s response was sufficiently fast (a win), and was

187

colored blue if the response was too slow (a loss). After the feedback image disappeared,

Page 8 of 38

188

participants waited a delay period that varied according to the cue and whether the subject

189

won or lost that trial. Lastly, the reward image (either a face or a landscape) was shown for

190

2000 ms, and there was an intertrial interval (ITI) that was jittered between 1500 and 4500

191

ms (with the 2000 ms reward image duration, the minimum and maximum time delays

192

between reward onset and subsequent cue onset was 3500 and 6500 ms, respectively).

193

Each run of the modified MID task consisted of 36 trials, nine images of each SD, SA,

194

ND, and NA in randomized order. Across all runs included in the analyses, the four types of

195

transitions (e.g., social followed by social, social followed by nonsocial, etc.) each occurred on

196

approximately 25±1% of trials, reflecting effective randomization of stimulus types across

197

trials. Social images were photographs of young adult women and were cropped to show

198

only the face (images were drawn from Smith et al. 2010). Nonsocial images were

199

photographs of landscapes, and did not contain any body or facial features. All images were

200

resized to uniform dimensions. Attractiveness (high/medium/low) of images was

201

determined by ratings by an independent group. Additionally, performance thresholds were

202

determined independently for each subject by an algorithm that adapted the task difficulty

203

so that accuracy would be approximately 60% (observed hit rate = 60.7%).

204

After completing the four runs of the modified MID task, participants completed an

205

unrelated passive-viewing task described elsewhere (Young et al., 2015). Following this

206

second scanner task, participants completed two tasks outside the scanner. In the first task,

207

participants rated the attractiveness of each image they viewed in the modified MID task on

208

a scale of 1 to 7. Each participant’s highest-rated face and landscape images were then used

209

in a second task involving forced choices between a landscape image and a face image

210

presented simultaneously.

Page 9 of 38

211

All tasks were programmed and displayed using the Psychophysics Toolbox (Version

212

3; Brainard, 1997) for MATLAB (Mathworks Inc.).

213

Behavioral Analysis

214

Behavioral data were analyzed using MATLAB and IBM SPSS Statistics 20. A response

215

time (RT) difference was calculated for each participant by subtracting their mean RT on the

216

trials subsequent to nonsocial trials from their mean RT on trials subsequent to social trials.

217

Consequently, positive RT differences indicate shorter RTs subsequent to nonsocial trials

218

(relative to social trials), whereas negative differences indicate shorter RTs subsequent to

219

social trials (relative to nonsocial trials).

220

To control for factors other than prior stimulus category – such as attractiveness of

221

the prior trial’s reward image, the prior trial’s RT, and the subsequent trial’s stimulus type –

222

we incorporated these factors into a general linear model (GLM) for each subject. The GLM

223

included the following as regressors: (1) stimulus type on previous (t-1) trial, (2) RT on

224

previous trial, (3) attractiveness rating of the reward image of the previous trial, and (4)

225

stimulus type on current trial t. This analysis indicates the specific influence of each

226

regressor on the current trial’s RT, and allowed us to examine the unique influence of the

227

previous trial’s stimulus category (social or nonsocial) on current RT. In our analyses,

228

positive stimulus type beta weights reflect slower RTs on or following social trials, whereas

229

negative stimulus type betas weights reflect faster RTs on or following social trials.

230

Image Acquisition

231 232

Neuroimaging data were collected using a General Electric MR750 3.0 Tesla scanner equipped with an 8-channel parallel imaging system. We used a T2*-weighted spiral-in

Page 10 of 38

233

sensitivity encoding sequence (SENSE factor = 2), with slices parallel to the axial plane

234

connecting the anterior and posterior commissures (repetition time (TR): 1580 ms; echo

235

time (TE): 30 ms; matrix: 64 x 64; field of view (FOV): 243 mm; voxel size: 3.8 x 3.8 x 3.8

236

mm; 37 axial slices; flip angle: 70 degrees). The first eight volumes of each run were

237

removed to allow for magnetic stabilization. We additionally acquired whole-brain high-

238

resolution anatomical scans (T1-weighted FSPGR sequence; TR: 7.58 ms; TE: 2.93 ms;

239

matrix: 256 x 256; FOV: 256 mm; voxel size: 1 x 1 x 1 mm; 206 axial slices; flip angle: 12

240

degrees) to allow for coregistration and normalization.

241

Preprocessing

242

Our preprocessing used tools from the FMRIB Software Library package (FSL Version

243

4.1.8; http://www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/; Smith et al., 2004; Woolrich et al., 2009). We

244

corrected for head motion by realigning the time series to the middle volume (Jenkinson and

245

Smith, 2001), and then removed non-brain material using a brain extraction tool (Smith,

246

2002). We then corrected intravolume slice-timing differences using Fourier-space phase

247

shifting to align to the middle slice (Sladky et al., 2011). After spatially smoothing the image

248

using a 5-mm full-width-half-maximum isotropic Gaussian kernel, we applied a high-pass

249

temporal filter with a 100-second cutoff, and we normalized each 4-dimensional dataset to

250

the grand-mean intensity using a single multiplicative factor. Lastly, we spatially normalized

251

the functional data to the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) Template avg152 T1-

252

weighted template (3 mm isotropic resolution) using a 12-parameter affine transformation

253

implemented in FLIRT (Jenkinson and Smith, 2001).

254 255

As part of our preprocessing and quality control, we additionally examined three partially correlated measures of quality assurance: signal-to-fluctuation-noise ratio (SFNR; Page 11 of 38

256

Friedman and Glover, 2006), volume-to-volume head motion, and number of motion spikes

257

within the time series (motion spikes were identified by evaluating the root-mean-square-

258

error of each volume relative to the middle time point). Measures on each metric were

259

considered outliers if they exceeded the 75th percentile plus the value of 150% of the

260

interquartile range (i.e., a standard boxplot threshold); runs that were identified as outliers

261

were excluded from further analyses. Additionally, any participant who had fewer than two

262

good runs (out of four total runs) was excluded from further analyses. These criteria

263

eliminated four participants.

264

Neuroimaging Analysis

265

To best meet the conditions for identifying task-specific interactions between large-

266

scale networks and focal brain regions (see Introduction), our neuroimaging analyses

267

proceeded in two phases, each described in a separate section below. First, we used

268

independent components analysis (ICA; Beckmann & Smith, 2004) and spatial regression

269

(Filippini et al., 2009) to identify the large-scale neural networks of interests (DMN and ECN)

270

and to examine the networks’ levels of activation over the course of each run. Second, we

271

used generalized network psychophysiological interaction (nPPI) models (adapted from

272

McLaren et al., 2012) to identify brain regions whose coupling with the ECN and DMN

273

changed as a function of the effect of stimulus type on subsequent RT. Importantly, this ICA-

274

based nPPI approach follows the logic of ROI-based PPI analyses, with the critical difference

275

of examining connectivity with data-driven large-scale neural networks instead of a specific

276

seed region. Critically, we note that this nPPI pipeline allowed us to test the three necessary

277

conditions for inferences that functional networks alter task-specific processing in focal

278

brain regions. Page 12 of 38

279

Identifying Large-Scale Functional Networks

280

We used FSL’s Multivariate Exploratory Linear Decomposition into Independent

281

Components (MELODIC) Version 3.10 to identify large-scale functional networks in the

282

neuroimaging data (Beckmann and Smith, 2004). MELODIC ICA was implemented using

283

temporal concatenation, which looks for common spatial patterns of components across

284

participants’ data without assuming a specific or common time course across all

285

participants; we note that previous research has also used temporal concatenation-based

286

ICA on task-based fMRI data (Utevsky et al., 2014; Young et al., 2015). The preprocessed data

287

were whitened and projected into a 25-dimensional subspace (Ray et al., 2013; Utevsky et

288

al., 2014). The whitened data were decomposed into sets of vectors describing the temporal

289

and spatial signal variation, using a fixed-point iteration technique to optimize non-Gaussian

290

spatial source distribution (Hyvärinen, 1999). The estimated component maps were then

291

thresholded by dividing the maps by the standard deviation of the residual noise, then fitting

292

a Gaussian-Gamma mixture model to the histogram of the normalized intensity values

293

(Beckmann and Smith, 2004). This first step provided a data-driven means to identify

294

functional networks present during task performance; this allowed us to meet the first

295

condition for identifying task-specific interactions between large-scale networks and specific

296

brain regions.

297

All unthresholded spatial maps from the ICA were then submitted to a spatial

298

regression (part of FSL’s dual regression analysis) to estimate the time courses of each

299

network (Filippini et al., 2009; Leech et al., 2011). In this analysis, spatial maps are regressed

300

onto each participant’s functional data, resulting in a matrix of T (time points) x C

Page 13 of 38

301

(components) beta coefficients that characterize each subject’s time courses for each

302

network.

303

Characterizing Reward-Related Network Connectivity and Activation

304

We assessed task-dependent network coupling using a generalized nPPI model. The

305

generalized PPI, in contrast to a standard PPI, computes a separate PPI term for each task

306

condition. This approach has been shown to yield more accurate estimates of how

307

connectivity varies as a function of psychological context (McLaren et al., 2012). The

308

generalized nPPI analysis was carried out using FMRI Expert Analysis Tool (FEAT) Version

309

5.0.1.

310

The run-level model included six task regressors: social cue (duration = 1.5 – 4.5 s),

311

nonsocial cue (duration = 1.5 – 4.5 s), hits (duration = 0.75 s), misses (duration = 0.75 s),

312

social reward outcome (duration = 2 s), and nonsocial reward outcome (duration = 2 s). We

313

additionally included time courses of both the DMN and ECN that were produced by the

314

spatial regression. Because of the minimum ITI of 1500 ms and the potential confound of the

315

cue presentation occurring between the outcome and subsequent target phases of the task,

316

we examined whether there was any collinearity between the outcomes and cues across the

317

runs included in our analysis. For each run included in our analyses, we calculated the

318

correlation values between the face and land outcome regressors and the face and land cue

319

regressors – and then averaged these correlation values across all runs. The mean

320

correlation values between face or land cues and face or land outcomes ranged from r = -

321

0.25 to r = -0.17 (minimum r = -0.28; maximum r = -0.10), indicating that our run-level

322

analyses were able to isolate and model cues and outcomes independently, with minimal

323

collinearity between regressors. Page 14 of 38

324

For our network interaction analysis, nPPI regressors were formed by multiplying the

325

DMN and ECN time courses (zeroed to the mean of the time course), separately, by the social

326

outcome and nonsocial outcome regressors (zeroed to the minimum value of the task time

327

course) (McLaren et al., 2012); this yielded four nPPI regressors: (1) DMN*social, (2)

328

DMN*nonsocial, (3) ECN*social, (4) ECN*nonsocial. To control for motion in the scanner, we

329

additionally included motion spikes and motion parameters as regressors. Lastly, to control

330

for the influence of other networks and potential artifacts on our generalized nPPI, we

331

included the time courses of the remaining 23 components from the ICA. The nPPI analysis

332

allowed us to examine task-specific coupling between ECN and DMN with other regions in

333

the brain, fulfilling the second condition for identifying the task-specific interactions of

334

interest.

335

Subject-level analyses for the generalized nPPI were run using FEAT and

336

implementing FMRIB’s Local Analysis of Mixed Effects (FLAME 1), and examined activation

337

across runs within each participant. Group-level analyses included each subject’s demeaned

338

betas from the prior stimulus-category regressor (see Behavioral Analysis); this allowed us

339

to examine whether network coupling predicts the characteristics of subsequent RT, and

340

fulfill the third condition for identifying task-specific interactions of interest. The group-level

341

analysis additionally included the main effect of group and three motion-related parameters

342

(SFNR, volume-to-volume head motion, and number of motion spikes within the time

343

series). All resulting z-statistic images were thresholded using a cluster-forming threshold of

344

2.3 and a corrected cluster-significance threshold of p < 0.05. Although this threshold

345

combined with FSL’s FLAME 1 protects against false positives, we note that all of our results

346

also survived permutation-based testing (Eklund et al., 2016). In these supplemental tests,

Page 15 of 38

347

statistical significance was assessed in a nonparametric fashion via FSL’s randomise; this tool

348

uses Monte Carlo permutation-based testing with 10,000 permutations and D = 0.05,

349

corrected for multiple comparisons across the whole brain (Nichols and Holmes, 2002;

350

Winkler et al., 2014).

351

Brain images and activations are displayed using MRIcroGL

352

(http://www.mccauslandcenter.sc.edu/mricrogl/; Rorden et al., 2007). All coordinates are

353

reported in Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space.

354

Results

355

Previous stimulus category influences current behavior

356

Social rewards, such as images of individuals or interactions with others, provide a

357

useful tool for examining the effects of both social context and non-consumable rewards on

358

motivated behavior. However, relatively little is known about the effects of social and

359

nonsocial rewards on future motivated behavior, and how the brain orchestrates future

360

motivated action. To examine the effect of social rewards on subsequent motivated actions,

361

we calculated a subsequent-RT effect by subtracting RTs following nonsocial trials from RTs

362

following social trials. When averaging across value and delay trials, we found an overall

363

effect of previous trial stimulus type on current trial RT: participants exhibited increased

364

(slower) RTs subsequent to social trials (M = 0.313 s, SD = 0.008 s) compared to subsequent

365

to nonsocial trials (M = 0.306 s, SD = 0.008 s; t(40) = 2.63, p = 0.01, d = 0.41; Figure 2A). This

366

pattern replicated when examining hit trials only (social: M = 0.317 s, SD = 0.009 s;

Page 16 of 38

367

nonsocial: M = 0.309 s, SD = 0.008 s; t(40) = 2.63, p = 0.01, d = 0.41). Thus, participants were

368

slower after performing a social trial compared to a nonsocial trial.

369

We next ran a general linear model (GLM) to control for properties of the previous

370

and current trials (see Behavioral Analysis). Across participants, current stimulus category

371

was the strongest predictor of current RT (t(40) = -4.31, p < .0001, d = 0.67), reflecting that

372

participants were faster to respond during a social trial than during a nonsocial trial. The

373

next strongest predictor was the previous trial’s stimulus category (t(40) = 2.46, p = 0.01, d =

374

0.38), reflecting that participants were slower to respond following a social trial than

375

following a nonsocial trial. Neither previous trial’s RT nor previous trial’s reward

376

attractiveness rating significantly predicted the current trial’s RT (Figure 2B; Table 1). We

377

note that the effect of previous stimulus type on current RT was still significant even after

378

accounting for the large effect of current stimulus type, reflecting a distinct role of previous

379

stimulus type unaccounted for by the other variables included in the GLM. These results

380

indicate that of the measured properties of the previous trial, prior stimulus type had the

381

strongest effect on current RT.

382

Network coupling with fusiform gyrus tracks effect of stimulus category on subsequent

383

behavior

384

Our behavioral results indicated that current-trial stimulus type influences motivated

385

behavior on the subsequent trial (which occurred seven seconds or more later), such that

386

participants were slower to respond following social trials compared to following nonsocial

387

trials. However, it remains unclear how interactions in the brain during this previous

388

stimulus outcome affect the subsequent RT. To investigate this, we examined how effective

Page 17 of 38

389

connectivity with the ECN – a network implicated in cognitive control and goal-directed

390

behavior – and the DMN – a network linked with task engagement orchestrate this change in

391

future motivated behavior. We predicted that this subsequent-RT effect would be guided, in

392

part, by changes in the coupling between these large-scale functional networks and domain-

393

specific brain regions when viewing social images compared to when viewing nonsocial

394

images. In particular, we predicted that the subsequent-RT effect would be driven by

395

changes in the relative coupling of the ECN and the DMN during the reward outcome phase

396

of the previous trial. Our analysis pipeline allowed us to meet the criteria to claim that

397

interactions between large-scale networks and specific brain regions are critical for behavior

398

changes (see Introduction).

399

After running the ICA, we identified the DMN and ECN maps by running spatial

400

correlations between the unthresholded maps from our ICA and the DMN and ECN maps

401

from Smith and colleagues (2009). From our 25 components, we selected the maps that best

402

matched the DMN (R = 0.776; other components: Rmean = -0.006, Rmin = -0.179, Rmax = 0.124)

403

and ECN (R = 0.64; other components: Rmean = 0.019, Rmin = -0.092, Rmax = 0.296) maps from

404

Smith and colleagues (2009) (see Table 2). For ease of visualization, thresholded maps (Z >

405

4) are shown in Figure 3.

406

Following network identification, we ran a nPPI analysis that used participant-

407

specific DMN and ECN time courses (estimated by the spatial regression analysis) as the

408

physiological regressors and presentation of social and nonsocial images as the

409

psychological regressors. This nPPI identified regions that are influenced by the ECN and

410

DMN in a task-dependent manner. We then tested whether these influences on cortex

411

predicted the effect of prior stimulus type on current RT. Our nPPI analysis indicated that

Page 18 of 38

412

effective connectivity between the fusiform gyrus (FG; peak: x = 38, y = -64, z = -20, p
social cues. Notably, there

425

were no regions exhibiting changes in connectivity with the ECN relative to the DMN in any

426

of the three contrasts tested. To further investigate this, we used the FG region showing

427

changes in ECN-DMN connectivity during the previous outcome phase as a mask during the

428

current cue phase, and examined whether the ECN-DMN connectivity from this region of

429

interest (ROI) tracked the effect of prior stimulus on current RT. Across the three contrasts

430

tested (social, nonsocial, nonsocial > social), there was no correlation between the ROI

431

connectivity estimates and the effect of previous stimulus type on current RT (social: r = -

432

0.12, p = 0.45; nonsocial: r = 0.01, p = 0.94; nonsocial > social: r = 0.23, p = 0.15). Collectively,

433

these results support the claim that changes in subsequent RT are associated with

Page 19 of 38

434

differential network connectivity with the FG during the previous outcome phase, and

435

cannot be attributed to other aspects of task performance.

436

No regions showed increased coupling with ECN (relative to DMN) during nonsocial

437

rewards that tracked the effect on subsequent RT, nor did any regions show increased

438

coupling with DMN (relative to ECN) during social or nonsocial rewards that tracked the

439

effect on subsequent RT. Additionally, there were no regions that exhibited changes in

440

coupling with the DMN or ECN when comparing social and nonsocial rewards (e.g., DMN-

441

social > DMN-nonsocial; ECN-social > ECN-nonsocial; and the inverse contrasts) that tracked

442

the effect on subsequent RT. Lastly, we ran a whole-brain GLM using the RT betas as a

443

covariate to examine whether any regions’ activation tracked the effect of stimulus type on

444

subsequent RT; we found that no regions tracked this effect using this traditional GLM. Thus,

445

coupling between large-scale networks and the FG that tracked the effect on subsequent RT

446

was only observed during the viewing of social rewards.

447

Discussion

448

Recent neuroscience research has highlighted the relevance of large-scale functional

449

networks to various aspects of behavior (Anticevic et al., 2010; Eichele et al., 2008; Kelly et

450

al., 2008; Rosen et al., 2015). While many of these studies have linked network activation

451

and connectivity to behavior, the contribution of these networks to motivated behaviors via

452

focal cortical regions has been relatively understudied. For example, although previous work

453

has found correlations between DMN and working memory (Piccoli et al., 2015; Sambataro

454

et al., 2010) or sustained attention (Bonnelle et al., 2011; Gui et al., 2015), understanding

455

how these distributed functional networks influence other cortical regions to influence

Page 20 of 38

456

behavior has remained a significant challenge. Here, we found that participants were slower

457

in a response time task after having performed a trial for a social reward relative to a non-

458

social reward (after accounting for the influence of the current trial cue type), reflecting a

459

change in motivated behavior according to previous social stimulus type (Clithero et al.,

460

2011). We then examined the neural mechanisms underlying this effect using nPPI analysis –

461

an adaptation of generalized PPI analysis (i.e., effective connectivity) that examines effective

462

connectivity with large-scale functional networks. This analysis pipeline allowed us to

463

identify changes in coupling between large-scale networks and focal brain regions that

464

predicted subsequent motivated behavior. Our results demonstrate that two goal-relevant

465

networks, the DMN and ECN, interact with FG in a manner that predicts trial-to-trial

466

adjustments in response time.

467

Our analysis on our modified MID task identified that participants exhibit slower

468

reaction times on trials subsequent to social outcomes, relative to trials subsequent to

469

nonsocial outcomes. These results are consistent with a difference in motivational processes

470

due to the social nature of the previous reward outcome, analogous to effects on motivation

471

observed in similar tasks with nonsocial stimuli (Clithero et al., 2011). There are at least two

472

potential explanations for this subsequent reaction time effect. The first hypothesis is that

473

social images are more distracting than nonsocial images, and participants may still covertly

474

attend to a prior social reward more so than they do a prior nonsocial reward. Previous

475

research indicates that social images interfere with visual attention to a greater degree than

476

nonsocial images (Ebitz et al., 2013); thus, it is possible that this differential interference

477

may affect subsequent trials differently, as well. A second and related potential explanation

478

is that participants’ decreased motivation can be attributed to the satisfaction of receiving a

Page 21 of 38

479

motivating reward on the previous trial, much akin to results seen in “satisfaction of search”

480

(SOS) research. In SOS research, participants performing visual search tasks tend to

481

discontinue their search after finding an initial item, and either miss or display slower

482

reaction times for subsequent items (Berbaum et al., 1990; Fleck et al., 2010). As applied to

483

our MID task, a more motivating reward outcome (social images) may hinder the

484

performance on the subsequent trial. Future research will be needed to explore the cognitive

485

mechanisms driving the observed subsequent-RT effect and to determine whether it is

486

specific to social versus nonsocial stimuli, or may generalize to items that are more

487

motivating versus less motivating.

488

Our findings expand on recent research examining the relevance of DMN and ECN

489

activation to behavior, potentially in task-relevant contexts. Specifically, we found that

490

increased ECN (relative to DMN) coupling with the FG is associated with enhanced

491

subsequent task performance. These results are consistent with previous studies

492

demonstrating different relationships between the DMN and ECN with task behavior: while

493

ECN activation is often associated with heightened task performance and behavior

494

(Dosenbach et al., 2007; Seeley et al., 2007), DMN activation is frequently linked to

495

decrements in behavior and engagement in both humans (Eichele et al., 2008; Weissman et

496

al., 2006) and non-human primates (Hayden et al., 2009, 2010; Heilbronner and Platt, 2013).

497

Importantly, however, our findings extend these previous results by demonstrating direct

498

coupling of these networks with a prototypical social-perception processing region in a task-

499

dependent manner. Our results additionally highlight that the opposing effects of DMN and

500

ECN are not limited to concurrent behavior, but also affect subsequent behavior with effects

501

observed seven or more seconds later. These findings support the idea that large-scale

Page 22 of 38

502

networks interact with lower-level perceptual regions to contribute to motivational

503

processes (Clithero et al., 2011) and shape later behavior.

504

Unlike previous studies examining large-scale networks (Brewer et al., 2011;

505

Ossandon et al., 2011; Seeley et al., 2007; Utevsky et al., 2014), our experiment demonstrates

506

network coupling that directly shapes subsequent response time. While prior studies report

507

associations between functional connectivity and behavior, estimates of functional

508

connectivity solely report on correlations in activation (not coupling) between regions,

509

which can be the result of various phenomena (Friston, 2011). Specifically, reported changes

510

in functional connectivity can arise from numerous causes, including: changes in

511

connectivity with another region, changes in observation noise (or signal-to-noise ratios),

512

and changes in the degree of neuronal fluctuations (for a review, see Friston, 2011). Thus,

513

changes in functional connectivity may not reflect changes in coupling between cortical

514

regions. In contrast to functional connectivity, we implemented an adaptation of a traditional

515

PPI analysis to measure effective connectivity between functional networks and other

516

cortical regions. Psychophysiological interaction analyses measure whether a psychological

517

context (e.g., outcome stimulus category) influences how one brain region or network (the

518

“seed”) contributes to another (the “target”) by examining whether an interaction between

519

the psychological context and the seed is identified in the target (Smith et al., 2016). Thus, a

520

PPI analysis can eschew the potential confounds of functional connectivity analyses and

521

reflects a change in neural coupling (e.g., effective connectivity). We note that our novel

522

approach of generalized nPPI – applying generalized PPI analyses to large-scale networks –

523

allows us to examine task-dependent contributions between networks and other cortical

Page 23 of 38

524

regions (Friston et al., 1997; Friston, 2011). In this way, our study extends prior work by

525

demonstrating specific task-dependent coupling of the DMN/ECN.

526

Although recent meta-analytic work has demonstrated that PPI produces consistent

527

and specific patterns of connectivity (Smith et al., 2016; Smith and Delgado, 2017), it is

528

important to note that PPI results can be interpreted in two ways (Friston et al., 1997). First,

529

our effects could reflect a context-specific modulation of effective connectivity. In this case,

530

face presentations modulate the degree to which the DMN and ECN contribute to FG. Our

531

results focus on the difference between DMN and ECN contributions to FG which seem to

532

facilitate social motivation (Clithero et al., 2011). Alternatively, our effects could reflect a

533

modulation of stimulus-specific responses. In this case, the DMN and ECN influence how FG

534

responds to the presentation of the face; under this interpretation, our results suggest that

535

the degree to which DMN enhances face responses in FG is greater than that of ECN. In either

536

interpretation, the resulting effect on FG connectivity predicts behavior. A better

537

understanding of the mechanisms and causal relationship underlying our results may be

538

facilitated by other analytical approaches, such as dynamic causal modeling (DCM) (Friston

539

et al., 2003; Friston, 2011), although relatively less is understood regarding how the

540

biophysical models implemented in DCM apply to distributed functional networks (Buxton

541

et al., 1998; Friston et al., 2003). Additionally, other methodological approaches, such as

542

transcranial magnetic stimulation (Fox et al., 2012; Luber and Lisanby, 2014; Mueller et al.,

543

2014) or transcranial current stimulation (Hampstead et al., 2014; Keeser et al., 2011) may

544

also better inform the specific interactions between FG and the ECN and DMN.

545

One potential caveat to note is that our task design included a cue to indicate the current

546

trial’s stimulus type in between the previous trial’s outcome phase and the current trial’s

Page 24 of 38

547

response phase. Because the current trial’s stimulus type is the strongest predictor of the

548

current trial’s RT, there may be concerns over whether the neural coupling we observe can

549

be attributed to the cue, rather than the previous trial’s outcome. This concern is

550

ameliorated through both the randomization of trial types (i.e., the identity of the outcome

551

on the previous trial is uncorrelated with the trial type on the subsequent trial), as well as

552

the run-level models implemented in our PPI analysis that included separate regressors for

553

both the outcome phase (social and nonsocial outcomes, separately) and the cue phase

554

(social and nonsocial cues, separately). Including these separate regressors for each phase

555

allowed us to distinguish effects of the cue and outcome phases on connectivity. This

556

concern is additionally mitigated by the analyses examining connectivity during the cue

557

phase. A nPPI showed no changes in ECN-DMN connectivity during social cues, nonsocial

558

cues, or social > nonsocial cues when looking across the whole brain. A supplementary ROI

559

analysis using the FG region indicated that FG connectivity with the ECN (relative to the

560

DMN) during these three contrasts did not track the effect of the previous trial stimulus type

561

on subsequent RT (as illustrated by the lack of significant correlations between the

562

connectivity estimates and the effect of previous stimulus type on subsequent RT). However,

563

future research may benefit from a modified task design in which there are no informative

564

stimuli presented between a reward image and subsequent response.

565

As an additional caveat, we note that our paradigm and results leave room for

566

interpretive challenges. Because participants exhibited faster response times to view social

567

images compared to nonsocial images, we cannot discern whether our nPPI results from the

568

social-stimuli condition are due to the stimulus type itself (i.e. face images) or would

569

generalize to other highly motivating stimuli. While prior work linking FG to face processing

Page 25 of 38

570

supports the interpretation that our results are indeed due to that specific stimulus type

571

(Engell and McCarthy, 2014; Kanwisher et al., 1997; Kanwisher and Yovel, 2006; McCarthy et

572

al., 1997), future studies could compare within-domain images of varying attractiveness to

573

other categories of motivating stimuli (e.g., money). Such an analysis would speak to

574

whether these behavioral and neural results are unique to face image rewards, or are due to

575

participant differences in subjective value (Bos et al., 2013; Smith et al., 2014a).

576

Our results demonstrate task-dependent contributions between the DMN and ECN

577

and the FG that shape subsequent motivated behavior. These large-scale networks are

578

known to be disrupted in a variety of psychopathologies marked by impairments in

579

attention and reward processing, including autism spectrum disorder (Assaf et al., 2010;

580

Young et al., 2015), obsessive compulsive disorder (Stern et al., 2011, 2012), and major

581

depressive disorder (Grimm et al., 2011; Sheline et al., 2009), and so an improved

582

understanding of how they influence moment-to-moment behavior could have clinical

583

relevance and advance models of pathophysiology (Cuthbert and Insel, 2013; Insel et al.,

584

2010). Thus, this study marks a significant step toward better understanding and treatment

585

of disorders characterized by impaired social and reward processing.

Page 26 of 38

586

References

587 588 589

Anticevic, A., Repovs, G., Shulman, G.L., Barch, D.M., 2010. When less is more: TPJ and default network deactivation during encoding predicts working memory performance. NeuroImage 49, 2638–2648. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.11.008

590 591 592 593

Assaf, M., Jagannathan, K., Calhoun, V.D., Miller, L., Stevens, M.C., Sahl, R., O’Boyle, J.G., Schultz, R.T., Pearlson, G.D., 2010. Abnormal functional connectivity of default mode sub-networks in autism spectrum disorder patients. NeuroImage 53, 247–256. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.05.067

594 595 596

Beckmann, C.F., DeLuca, M., Devlin, J.T., Smith, S.M., 2005. Investigations into resting-state connectivity using independent component analysis. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. 360, 1001–1013. doi:10.1098/rstb.2005.1634

597 598 599

Beckmann, C.F., Smith, S.M., 2004. Probabilistic independent component analysis for functional magnetic resonance imaging. IEEE Trans. Med. Imaging 23, 137–52. doi:10.1109/TMI.2003.822821

600 601

Behrens, T.E., Sporns, O., 2012. Human connectomics. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 22, 144–53. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2011.08.005

602 603 604

Berbaum, K.S., Franken, E.A., Dorfman, D.D., Rooholamini, S.A., Kathol, M.H., Barloon, T.J., Behlke, F.M., Sato, Y., Lu, C.H., el-Khoury, G.Y., 1990. Satisfaction of search in diagnostic radiology. Invest. Radiol. 25, 133–140.

605 606 607

Biswal, B., Zerrin Yetkin, F., Haughton, V.M., Hyde, J.S., 1995. Functional connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using echo-planar mri. Magn. Reson. Med. 34, 537– 541. doi:10.1002/mrm.1910340409

608 609 610 611 612 613 614 615 616 617

Biswal, B.B., Mennes, M., Zuo, X.-N., Gohel, S., Kelly, C., Smith, S.M., Beckmann, C.F., Adelstein, J.S., Buckner, R.L., Colcombe, S., Dogonowski, A.-M., Ernst, M., Fair, D., Hampson, M., Hoptman, M.J., Hyde, J.S., Kiviniemi, V.J., Kötter, R., Li, S.-J., Lin, C.-P., Lowe, M.J., Mackay, C., Madden, D.J., Madsen, K.H., Margulies, D.S., Mayberg, H.S., McMahon, K., Monk, C.S., Mostofsky, S.H., Nagel, B.J., Pekar, J.J., Peltier, S.J., Petersen, S.E., Riedl, V., Rombouts, S.A.R.B., Rypma, B., Schlaggar, B.L., Schmidt, S., Seidler, R.D., Siegle, G.J., Sorg, C., Teng, G.-J., Veijola, J., Villringer, A., Walter, M., Wang, L., Weng, X.-C., Whitfield-Gabrieli, S., Williamson, P., Windischberger, C., Zang, Y.-F., Zhang, H.-Y., Castellanos, F.X., Milham, M.P., 2010. Toward discovery science of human brain function. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 107, 4734–4739. doi:10.1073/pnas.0911855107

618 619 620 621

Bonnelle, V., Leech, R., Kinnunen, K.M., Ham, T.E., Beckmann, C.F., Boissezon, X.D., Greenwood, R.J., Sharp, D.J., 2011. Default Mode Network Connectivity Predicts Sustained Attention Deficits after Traumatic Brain Injury. J. Neurosci. 31, 13442–13451. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1163-11.2011

Page 27 of 38

622 623 624

Bos, W. van den, Talwar, A., McClure, S.M., 2013. Neural Correlates of Reinforcement Learning and Social Preferences in Competitive Bidding. J. Neurosci. 33, 2137–2146. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3095-12.2013

625

Brainard, D.H., 1997. The Psychophysics Toolbox. Spat. Vis. 10, 433–436.

626 627 628

Brewer, J.A., Worhunsky, P.D., Gray, J.R., Tang, Y.-Y., Weber, J., Kober, H., 2011. Meditation experience is associated with differences in default mode network activity and connectivity. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 108, 20254–9. doi:10.1073/pnas.1112029108

629 630

Buxton, R.B., Wong, E.C., Frank, L.R., 1998. Dynamics of blood flow and oxygenation changes during brain activation: the balloon model. Magn. Reson. Med. 39, 855–64.

631 632 633

Clithero, J.A., Reeck, C., Carter, R.M., Smith, D.V., Huettel, S.A., 2011. Nucleus accumbens mediates relative motivation for rewards in the absence of choice. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 5, 87. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2011.00087

634 635 636

Cole, D.M., Smith, S.M., Beckmann, C.F., 2010. Advances and pitfalls in the analysis and interpretation of resting-state FMRI data. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 4, 8. doi:10.3389/fnsys.2010.00008

637 638 639

Cowdrey, F.A., Filippini, N., Park, R.J., Smith, S.M., McCabe, C., 2012. Increased resting state functional connectivity in the default mode network in recovered anorexia nervosa. Hum. Brain Mapp. 0. doi:10.1002/hbm.22202

640 641

Cuthbert, B.N., Insel, T.R., 2013. Toward the future of psychiatric diagnosis: the seven pillars of RDoC. BMC Med. 11, 126. doi:10.1186/1741-7015-11-126

642 643 644

De Havas, J.A., Parimal, S., Soon, C.S., Chee, M.W.L., 2012. Sleep deprivation reduces default mode network connectivity and anti-correlation during rest and task performance. NeuroImage 59, 1745–1751. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.08.026

645 646 647 648

Dosenbach, N.U.F., Fair, D.A., Miezin, F.M., Cohen, A.L., Wenger, K.K., Dosenbach, R. a T., Fox, M.D., Snyder, A.Z., Vincent, J.L., Raichle, M.E., Schlaggar, B.L., Petersen, S.E., 2007. Distinct brain networks for adaptive and stable task control in humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 104, 11073–11078. doi:10.1073/pnas.0704320104

649 650

Ebitz, R.B., Watson, K.K., Platt, M.L., 2013. Oxytocin blunts social vigilance in the rhesus macaque. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 110, 11630–11635. doi:10.1073/pnas.1305230110

651 652 653

Eichele, T., Debener, S., Calhoun, V.D., Specht, K., Engel, A.K., Hugdahl, K., von Cramon, D.Y., Ullsperger, M., 2008. Prediction of human errors by maladaptive changes in event-related brain networks. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 105, 6173–8. doi:10.1073/pnas.0708965105

654 655 656

Eklund, A., Nichols, T.E., Knutsson, H., 2016. Cluster failure: Why fMRI inferences for spatial extent have inflated false-positive rates. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 113, 7900–7905. doi:10.1073/pnas.1602413113

Page 28 of 38

657 658 659

Engell, A.D., McCarthy, G., 2014. Face, eye, and body selective responses in fusiform gyrus and adjacent cortex: an intracranial EEG study. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2014.00642

660 661 662 663

Filippini, N., MacIntosh, B.J., Hough, M.G., Goodwin, G.M., Frisoni, G.B., Smith, S.M., Matthews, P.M., Beckmann, C.F., Mackay, C.E., 2009. Distinct patterns of brain activity in young carriers of the APOE-ϵ4 allele. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 106, 7209–7214. doi:10.1073/pnas.0811879106

664 665 666

Fleck, M.S., Samei, E., Mitroff, S.R., 2010. Generalized “satisfaction of search”: adverse influences on dual-target search accuracy. J. Exp. Psychol. 16, 60–71. doi:10.1037/a0018629

667 668 669 670

Fox, M.D., Halko, M.A., Eldaief, M.C., Pascual-Leone, A., 2012. Measuring and manipulating brain connectivity with resting state functional connectivity magnetic resonance imaging (fcMRI) and transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS). NeuroImage 62, 2232–43. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.03.035

671 672 673

Friedman, L., Glover, G.H., 2006. Reducing interscanner variability of activation in a multicenter fMRI study: controlling for signal-to-fluctuation-noise-ratio (SFNR) differences. NeuroImage 33, 471–81. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.07.012

674 675

Friston, K.J., 2011. Functional and Effective Connectivity: A Review. Brain Connect. 1, 13–36. doi:10.1089/brain.2011.0008

676 677 678

Friston, K.J., Buechel, C., Fink, G.R., Morris, J., Rolls, E., Dolan, R.J., 1997. Psychophysiological and Modulatory Interactions in Neuroimaging. NeuroImage 6, 218–229. doi:10.1006/nimg.1997.0291

679 680

Friston, K.J., Harrison, L., Penny, W., 2003. Dynamic causal modelling. NeuroImage 19, 1273– 1302. doi:10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00202-7

681 682 683

Grimm, S., Ernst, J., Boesiger, P., Schuepbach, D., Boeker, H., Northoff, G., 2011. Reduced negative BOLD responses in the default-mode network and increased self-focus in depression. World J. Biol. Psychiatry 12, 627–637. doi:10.3109/15622975.2010.545145

684 685 686

Gui, D., Xu, S., Zhu, S., Fang, Z., Spaeth, A.M., Xin, Y., Feng, T., Rao, H., 2015. Resting spontaneous activity in the default mode network predicts performance decline during prolonged attention workload. NeuroImage. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.07.030

687 688

Gusnard, D.A., Raichle, M.E., 2001. Searching for a baseline: Functional imaging and the resting human brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2, 685–694. doi:10.1038/35094500

689 690 691

Hampstead, B.M., Brown, G.S., Hartley, J.F., 2014. Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation Modulates Activation and Effective Connectivity During Spatial Navigation. Brain Stimulat. 7, 314–324. doi:10.1016/j.brs.2013.12.006

Page 29 of 38

692 693

Hayden, B.Y., Smith, D.V., Platt, M.L., 2010. Cognitive control signals in posterior cingulate cortex. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 4, 223. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2010.00223

694 695 696

Hayden, B.Y., Smith, D.V., Platt, M.L., 2009. Electrophysiological correlates of default-mode processing in macaque posterior cingulate cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 106, 5948–5953. doi:10.1073/pnas.0812035106

697 698 699

Heilbronner, S.R., Platt, M.L., 2013. Causal Evidence of Performance Monitoring by Neurons in Posterior Cingulate Cortex during Learning. Neuron 80, 1384–1391. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2013.09.028

700 701

Hyvärinen, A., 1999. Fast and robust fixed-point algorithms for independent component analysis. IEEE Trans. Neural Netw. 10, 626–34. doi:10.1109/72.761722

702 703 704 705

Insel, T., Cuthbert, B., Garvey, M., Heinssen, R., Pine, D.S., Quinn, K., Sanislow, C., Wang, P., 2010. Research domain criteria (RDoC): Toward a new classification framework for research on mental disorders. Am. J. Psychiatry 167, 748–751. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09091379

706 707

Jenkinson, M., Smith, S., 2001. A global optimisation method for robust affine registration of brain images. Med. Image Anal. 5, 143–56.

708 709

Kanwisher, N., McDermott, J., Chun, M.M., 1997. The fusiform face area: A module in human extrastriate cortex specialized for face perception. J. Neurosci. 17, 4302–4311.

710 711 712

Kanwisher, N., Yovel, G., 2006. The fusiform face area: a cortical region specialized for the perception of faces. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 361, 2109–2128. doi:10.1098/rstb.2006.1934

713 714 715 716

Keeser, D., Meindl, T., Bor, J., Palm, U., Pogarell, O., Mulert, C., Brunelin, J., Moller, H.-J., Reiser, M., Padberg, F., 2011. Prefrontal Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation Changes Connectivity of Resting-State Networks during fMRI. J. Neurosci. 31, 15284–15293. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0542-11.2011

717 718 719

Kelly, A., Uddin, L.Q., Biswal, B.B., Castellanos, F.X., Milham, M.P., 2008. Competition between functional brain networks mediates behavioral variability. NeuroImage 39, 527– 37. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.08.008

720 721

Knutson, B., Adams, C.M., Fong, G.W., Hommer, D., 2001. Anticipation of Increasing Monetary Reward Selectively Recruits Nucleus Accumbens. J. Neurosci. 21, RC159-RC159.

722 723 724

Knutson, B., Westdorp, A., Kaiser, E., Hommer, D., 2000. FMRI visualization of brain activity during a monetary incentive delay task. NeuroImage 12, 20–7. doi:10.1006/nimg.2000.0593

725 726

Kucyi, A., Moayedi, M., Weissman-Fogel, I., Goldberg, M.B., Freeman, B.V., Tenenbaum, H.C., Davis, K.D., 2014. Enhanced Medial Prefrontal-Default Mode Network Functional

Page 30 of 38

727 728

Connectivity in Chronic Pain and Its Association with Pain Rumination. J. Neurosci. 34, 3969–3975. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5055-13.2014

729 730 731

Leech, R., Kamourieh, S., Beckmann, C.F., Sharp, D.J., 2011. Fractionating the default mode network: distinct contributions of the ventral and dorsal posterior cingulate cortex to cognitive control. J. Neurosci. 31, 3217–3224. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5626-10.2011

732 733 734

Luber, B., Lisanby, S.H., 2014. Enhancement of human cognitive performance using transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS). NeuroImage 85, Part 3, 961–970. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.06.007

735 736

McCarthy, G., Puce, A., Gore, J.C., Allison, T., 1997. Face-Specific Processing in the Human Fusiform Gyrus. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 9, 605–610. doi:10.1162/jocn.1997.9.5.605

737 738 739

McLaren, D.G., Ries, M.L., Xu, G., Johnson, S.C., 2012. A generalized form of context-dependent psychophysiological interactions (gPPI): A comparison to standard approaches. NeuroImage 61, 1277–1286. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.03.068

740 741 742 743

Mueller, J.K., Grigsby, E.M., Prevosto, V., Petraglia Iii, F.W., Rao, H., Deng, Z.-D., Peterchev, A.V., Sommer, M.A., Egner, T., Platt, M.L., Grill, W.M., 2014. Simultaneous transcranial magnetic stimulation and single-neuron recording in alert non-human primates. Nat. Neurosci. advance online publication. doi:10.1038/nn.3751

744 745

Nichols, T.E., Holmes, A.P., 2002. Nonparametric permutation tests for functional neuroimaging: a primer with examples. Hum. Brain Mapp. 15, 1–25.

746 747 748

O’Reilly, J.X., Woolrich, M.W., Behrens, T.E., Smith, S.M., Johansen-Berg, H., 2012. Tools of the trade: psychophysiological interactions and functional connectivity. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 7, 604–9. doi:10.1093/scan/nss055

749 750 751 752

Ossandon, T., Jerbi, K., Vidal, J.R., Bayle, D.J., Henaff, M.-A., Jung, J., Minotti, L., Bertrand, O., Kahane, P., Lachaux, J.-P., 2011. Transient Suppression of Broadband Gamma Power in the Default-Mode Network Is Correlated with Task Complexity and Subject Performance. J. Neurosci. 31, 14521–14530. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2483-11.2011

753 754 755 756

Piccoli, T., Valente, G., Linden, D.E.J., Re, M., Esposito, F., Sack, A.T., Salle, F.D., 2015. The Default Mode Network and the Working Memory Network Are Not Anti-Correlated during All Phases of a Working Memory Task. PLoS ONE 10, e0123354. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0123354

757 758 759

Ray, K.L., McKay, D.R., Fox, P.M., Riedel, M.C., Uecker, A.M., Beckmann, C.F., Smith, S.M., Fox, P.T., Laird, A.R., 2013. ICA model order selection of task co-activation networks. Front. Neurosci. 7. doi:10.3389/fnins.2013.00237

760 761

Rorden, C., Karnath, H.-O., Bonilha, L., 2007. Improving Lesion-Symptom Mapping. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 19, 1081–1088. doi:10.1162/jocn.2007.19.7.1081

Page 31 of 38

762 763 764

Rosen, M.L., Stern, C.E., Michalka, S.W., Devaney, K.J., Somers, D.C., 2015. Cognitive Control Network Contributions to Memory-Guided Visual Attention. Cereb. Cortex bhv028. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhv028

765 766 767

Sambataro, F., Murty, V.P., Callicott, J.H., Tan, H.-Y., Das, S., Weinberger, D.R., Mattay, V.S., 2010. Age-related alterations in default mode network: Impact on working memory performance. Neurobiol. Aging 31, 839–852. doi:10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2008.05.022

768 769 770 771

Seeley, W.W., Menon, V., Schatzberg, A.F., Keller, J., Glover, G.H., Kenna, H., Reiss, A.L., Greicius, M.D., 2007. Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. J. Neurosci. 27, 2349–2356. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.558706.2007

772 773 774 775

Shannon, B.J., Raichle, M.E., Snyder, A.Z., Fair, D.A., Mills, K.L., Zhang, D., Bache, K., Calhoun, V.D., Nigg, J.T., Nagel, B.J., Stevens, A.A., Kiehl, K.A., 2011. Premotor functional connectivity predicts impulsivity in juvenile offenders. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 108, 11241–11245. doi:10.1073/pnas.1108241108

776 777 778 779

Sheline, Y.I., Barch, D.M., Price, J.L., Rundle, M.M., Vaishnavi, S.N., Snyder, A.Z., Mintun, M.A., Wang, S., Coalson, R.S., Raichle, M.E., 2009. The default mode network and selfreferential processes in depression. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 106, 1942–7. doi:10.1073/pnas.0812686106

780 781 782

Sladky, R., Friston, K.J., Tröstl, J., Cunnington, R., Moser, E., Windischberger, C., 2011. Slicetiming effects and their correction in functional MRI. NeuroImage 58, 588–94. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.06.078

783 784 785

Smith, D.V., Clithero, J.A., Boltuck, S.E., Huettel, S.A., 2014a. Functional connectivity with ventromedial prefrontal cortex reflects subjective value for social rewards. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 9, 2017–2025. doi:10.1093/scan/nsu005

786 787 788

Smith, D.V., Delgado, M.R., 2017. Meta-analysis of psychophysiological interactions: Revisiting cluster-level thresholding and sample sizes. Hum. Brain Mapp. n/a-n/a. doi:10.1002/hbm.23354

789 790 791

Smith, D.V., Gseir, M., Speer, M.E., Delgado, M.R., 2016. Toward a cumulative science of functional integration: A meta-analysis of psychophysiological interactions. Hum. Brain Mapp.

792 793 794

Smith, D.V., Hayden, B.Y., Truong, T.-K., Song, A.W., Platt, M.L., Huettel, S.A., 2010. Distinct value signals in anterior and posterior ventromedial prefrontal cortex. J. Neurosci. 30, 2490–5. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3319-09.2010

795 796 797 798

Smith, D.V., Utevsky, A.V., Bland, A.R., Clement, N., Clithero, J.A., Harsch, A.E.W., McKell Carter, R., Huettel, S.A., 2014b. Characterizing individual differences in functional connectivity using dual-regression and seed-based approaches. NeuroImage 95, 1–12. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.03.042

Page 32 of 38

799 800

Smith, S.M., 2002. Fast robust automated brain extraction. Hum. Brain Mapp. 17, 143–55. doi:10.1002/hbm.10062

801 802 803 804 805 806

Smith, S.M., Beckmann, C.F., Andersson, J., Auerbach, E.J., Bijsterbosch, J., Douaud, G., Duff, E., Feinberg, D.A., Griffanti, L., Harms, M.P., Kelly, M., Laumann, T., Miller, K.L., Moeller, S., Petersen, S., Power, J., Salimi-Khorshidi, G., Snyder, A.Z., Vu, A.T., Woolrich, M.W., Xu, J., Yacoub, E., Uğurbil, K., Van Essen, D.C., Glasser, M.F., 2013. Resting-state fMRI in the Human Connectome Project. NeuroImage, Mapping the Connectome 80, 144–168. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.039

807 808 809 810

Smith, S.M., Fox, P.T., Miller, K.L., Glahn, D.C., Fox, P.M., Mackay, C.E., Filippini, N., Watkins, K.E., Toro, R., Laird, A.R., Beckmann, C.F., 2009. Correspondence of the brain’s functional architecture during activation and rest. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 106, 13040–5. doi:10.1073/pnas.0905267106

811 812 813 814 815

Smith, S.M., Jenkinson, M., Woolrich, M.W., Beckmann, C.F., Behrens, T.E., Johansen-Berg, H., Bannister, P.R., De Luca, M., Drobnjak, I., Flitney, D.E., Niazy, R.K., Saunders, J., Vickers, J., Zhang, Y., De Stefano, N., Brady, J.M., Matthews, P.M., 2004. Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. NeuroImage 23 Suppl 1, S208–19. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.051

816 817 818

Stern, E.R., Fitzgerald, K.D., Welsh, R.C., Abelson, J.L., Taylor, S.F., 2012. Resting-State Functional Connectivity between Fronto-Parietal and Default Mode Networks in Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder. PLoS ONE 7, e36356. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0036356

819 820 821 822

Stern, E.R., Welsh, R.C., Fitzgerald, K.D., Gehring, W.J., Lister, J.J., Himle, J.A., Abelson, J.L., Taylor, S.F., 2011. Hyperactive Error Responses and Altered Connectivity in Ventromedial and Frontoinsular Cortices in Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder. Biol. Psychiatry, Genes and Anxiety 69, 583–591. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.09.048

823 824

Utevsky, A.V., Smith, D.V., Huettel, S.A., 2014. Precuneus is a functional core of the defaultmode network. J. Neurosci. 34, 932–940. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4227-13.2014

825 826

Weissman, D.H., Roberts, K.C., Visscher, K.M., Woldorff, M.G., 2006. The neural bases of momentary lapses in attention. Nat. Neurosci. 9, 971–978. doi:10.1038/nn1727

827 828 829

Winkler, A.M., Ridgway, G.R., Webster, M.A., Smith, S.M., Nichols, T.E., 2014. Permutation inference for the general linear model. NeuroImage 92, 381–397. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.01.060

830 831 832

Woolrich, M.W., Jbabdi, S., Patenaude, B., Chappell, M., Makni, S., Behrens, T.E., Beckmann, C.F., Jenkinson, M., Smith, S.M., 2009. Bayesian analysis of neuroimaging data in FSL. NeuroImage 45, S173–86. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.10.055

833 834 835

Yarkoni, T., Poldrack, R.A., Nichols, T.E., Van Essen, D.C., Wager, T.D., 2011. Large-scale automated synthesis of human functional neuroimaging data. Nat. Methods 8, 665–670. doi:10.1038/nmeth.1635

Page 33 of 38

836 837 838

Young, J.S., Smith, D.V., Coutlee, C.G., Huettel, S.A., 2015. Synchrony between sensory and cognitive networks is associated with subclinical variation in autistic traits. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 146. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2015.00146

839 840

Page 34 of 38

841

Figure Captions

842

Figure 1: Task Design

843

Fifty heterosexual men performed a modified MID task which examined timed responses to

844

view social and nonsocial images as rewards (faces and landscapes, respectively). Each trial

845

of the task began with an abstract image presented as a cue for 1500 ms indicating the

846

potential reward for that trial. Following the cue, a fixation cross appeared for a variable

847

duration (interstimulus interval; 1500 – 4500 ms), followed by a target (a white triangle)

848

that appeared on the screen for 750 ms; participants responded by pressing a button box

849

with their right index finger before the target disappeared. After the target, a feedback image

850

(a colored triangle) appeared for 750 ms; the feedback image was colored green if the

851

participants’ response was sufficiently fast on the trial (a win), and was blue if the response

852

was too slow (a loss). After the feedback image disappeared, the participants waited a delay

853

period that varied according to the cue and whether the subject won or lost that trial. Lastly,

854

the reward image (either a face or a landscape) was shown for 2000 ms. Consent was

855

provided for the use of the image in this figure.

856

Figure 2: Response Times are Slower Following Face Trials Compared to Landscape

857

Trials

858

(A) Distribution of response time (RT) differences according to previous trial’s stimulus

859

type, calculated by subtracting RTs following nonsocial trials from RTs following social trials.

860

Response times on trials following social rewards were greater than those on trials following

861

nonsocial rewards, indicating an effect of previous reward stimulus type on subsequent

862

behavior. (B) Average beta weights (with S.E.M. plotted) across subjects from a behavioral Page 35 of 38

863

regression predicting current RT. We regressed current trial RT on a model including the

864

following regressors: stimulus category (social or nonsocial) on previous trial, RT on

865

previous trial, attractiveness rating of the previous trial’s reward, and stimulus category on

866

current trial. Of these four regressors, current stimulus category most strongly predicted

867

current RT; the negative beta weight indicates that participants are faster to respond during

868

a social trial compared to during a nonsocial trial. The next most predictive regressor was

869

previous trial’s stimulus category; the positive beta weight indicates that participants were

870

slower to respond following social trials compared to following nonsocial trials. Neither

871

previous trial’s RT nor previous trial’s reward attractiveness significantly predicted the

872

current trial’s RT.

873

Figure 3: Networks Identified from Independent Components Analysis

874

Using independent components analysis across our task runs, we identified large-scale

875

functional networks. This analysis produced 25 components. Our analyses focused on the

876

components that best matched default-mode network (highest-correlating component

877

illustrated on top) and the executive control network (highest-correlating component

878

illustrated on bottom) from Smith and colleagues (2009). For visualization purposes, maps

879

are thresholded at Z > 4.

880

Figure 4: Fusiform Gyrus Connectivity Tracks Effect of Stimulus Type on Subsequent

881

Behavior

882

(A) A network PPI indicated that an area in the fusiform gyrus (FG) exhibits heightened

883

effective connectivity with ECN (compared to DMN) during social reward outcomes;

884

thresholded at p < 0.05. We note that this result also held with permutation-based testing

Page 36 of 38

885

(Eklund et al., 2016). (B) Parameters estimates extracted from the FG connectivity track the

886

effect of stimulus type on subsequent behavior (stimulus-type beta weights estimated from

887

our behavioral GLM; see Behavioral Analysis): as FG-DMN connectivity increases relative to

888

FG-ECN connectivity, RTs are further slowed following social trials compared to following

889

nonsocial trials.

890

Page 37 of 38

891

Table Captions

892

Table 1. Behavioral Regression Results

893

To examine the effects of other trial characteristics on current trial RT, we regressed current

894

trial RT on a model including: stimulus category (social or nonsocial) on previous trial, RT on

895

previous trial, attractiveness rating of the previous trial’s reward, and stimulus category on

896

current trial. Our analysis indicated that current stimulus type had the strongest effect on

897

current RT; however, of all the characteristics from the prior trial, only prior stimulus type

898

had a significant effect on current RT. Regressor Current trial (t) stimulus type Prior trial (t-1) stimulus type Prior trial (t-1) attractiveness Prior trial (t-1) response time

Parameter Estimate (s.e.m) -0.055 (0.013) 0.033 (0.013) 0.019 (0.011) < 0.0001 (0.013)

t-stat -4.31 2.46 1.74 0.005

p-value < 0.0001 0.01 0.09 0.99

899

Table 2. Spatial Correspondence with Canonical Networks

900

To identify the DMN and ECN, we ran spatial correlations between canonical neural

901

networks (Smith et al., 2009) and the 25 components from our ICA. The highest-correlating

902

ICA component numbers for each network map and the correlation values are listed. Canonical Network Visual 1 Visual 2 Visual 3 Default-Mode Cerebellar Sensorimotor Auditory Executive Control R Frontoparietal L Frontoparietal

Independent Component No. IC10 IC01 IC01 IC06 IC18 IC13 IC14 IC04 IC03 IC07

Spatial Correlation (r) 0.82 0.66 0.45 0.78 0.32 0.59 0.66 0.64 0.64 0.77

903

Page 38 of 38

Outcome

Cue

ISI

Target

Feedback

+ 1500ms

1500-4500ms

Delay

+ 750ms

750ms

ITI 2000ms

1500-4500ms

5000-11000ms

2000ms

A

8 Faster following face trials

Number of Participants

7

Faster following landscape trials

6 5 4 3 2 1 0 -0.03

-0.02

-0.01

0

0.01

0.02

0.03

0.04

0.05

Face-Land RT Difference (s)

B

*

Previous trial attractiveness Previous trial stimulus type

0.04

Previous trial reaction time

Parameter Estimates (β)

Current trial stimulus type

0.02

0

-0.02

-0.04

-0.06 ** p < 0.001

* p < 0.05

**

Z = 21

Y = -61

X=0

Default-Mode Network

Z = 12

Y = 27

Executive Control Network

X=0

Z = -17 Network Coupling with FG (DMN - ECN PPI β)

Increased FG-ECN coupling

B

Increased FG-DMN coupling

A

Y = -65

X = 42

15

10

5

0

-5

-10 -0.1

0

0.1

0.2

Change ininBehavior (Face-Scene RT Effect β)β) Change Behavior (Current Stimulus

Faster after social trials

Slower after social trials

Suggest Documents